Skip to main content

Meaning of life questionnaire (MLQ) in peruvian undergraduate students: study of its psychometric properties from the perspective of classical test theory (CTT)

Abstract

Background

The study of the meaning of life is essential since it plays a protective role in the mental health of university students. However, no studies have shown the adequate psychometric functioning of the MLQ in Latin American university students. For this reason, this research aims to evaluate the internal structure using CTT models, obtain evidence of validity based on the relationship with other variables, perform factorial invariance according to gender, and estimate the reliability of the MLQ.

Methods

A sample of 581 Peruvian undergraduate students of both sexes (29.5% men and 70.5% women) between the ages of 18 and 35 (M = 22.6; SD = 3.3) was collected. Along with the MLQ, other instruments were applied to measure satisfaction with life (SWLS), subjective well-being (WBI), and depression (PHQ-9).

Results

In the present study was evidenced that the model of two related factors of nine items presents better adjustment indices (RMSEA = .075; SRMR = .059; CFI = .97; TLI = .96) compared to other models. Also, it was shown that the factorial structure of the MLQ is strictly invariant for the group of men and women. It was also shown that the presence of meaning was positively related to satisfaction with life (.63) and well-being (.60) and negatively to depression (− .56). In contrast, the search for meaning was not significantly related to life satisfaction (− .05) and well-being (− .07); but yes, to depression (.19).

Conclusion

It is concluded that the MLQ from the perspective of CTT has shown adequate evidence of reliability and validity. Therefore, it could be used in future studies and evaluation and intervention processes. In addition, the study provides the first evidence of the psychometric functioning of the scale in university students from Latin America.

Peer Review reports

Introduction

The meaning of life refers to how people give meaning to their lives, such as the daily pursuit of goals, and implies the ability to strive for happiness [1,2,3]. In addition, this construct is a cognitive process that allows us to understand life, oneself, and others comprehensively and consistently [4,5,6]. Steger [7] adds that the meaning of life integrates two fundamental processes: (a) understanding, which is the ability to find meaning in lived experiences, and (b) coherence, which is the ability to commit and achieve long-term goals.

On the other hand, this construct has been studied from two main factors: (a) presence of meaning of life, is experienced when the person understands himself, the world and identifies his purpose in it [4, 6], and (b) search for meaning, understood from two perspectives: first, as the absence or deficit of meaning [8, 9] and second, as a motivational force and fundamental psychological need of the human being to understand his existence [1, 10]. In this regard, Steger et al. [11] suggests that the search will be healthy according to the internal motivations of each person; that is, it will be positive for those who consider it important to achieve their development, while negative for those who appreciate it as an indicator of low personal growth. In this sense, both factors (presence and search) are inversely related, stronger in individuals with less open-mindedness [11].

Several studies show that the meaning of life plays a fundamental role in people since it reduces the tendency to feel easily upset, moody, and anxious [12]. It is also related to a lower presence of indicators of depression [13], anxiety [14], and post-traumatic distress. Regarding the university population, various studies show that it is a factor that favors academic performance since it allows them to experience less demotivation [15]. Likewise, it improves coping with stressful situations [16] and allows prosocial actions [17]. Other studies show that the meaning of life is linked to happiness [4], optimism [18], satisfaction with life [19], gratitude [20], adaptation to stressors [21], prosocial behavior [17, 22], tolerance for uncertainty [12], and hope [13, 23].

Therefore, the study of this construct is critical because it plays a fundamental role in the mental health of university students, and therefore, it is necessary to have instruments that adequately measure this construct. Concerning this, there are various instruments among the most used are: The Life Regard Index [24], Purpose in Life Test [3], Personal Meaning Index [25], and Meaning in Life Index [26]. The Meaning of Life Questionnaire (MLQ) by Steger et al. [6] is the most widely used, made up of 10 items that measure the meaning and significance of the person's life and existence. The MLQ comprises two dimensions: the presence of meaning in life (MLQ-P) and the search for meaning in life (MLQ-S).

Table 1 shows that multiple adaptations of the MLQ have been made in countries such as Japan [27], Argentina [28], Turkey [29], South Africa [19], China [30], Brazil [31], India [32], Australia [33], Nigeria [34], Italy [35], Iran [36] y Ghana [37].

Table 1 Psychometric studies of the meaning of life questionnaire (MLQ) in various samples

Concerning the psychometric properties of the MLQ, the original study [6] carried out on university students showed a two-dimensional structure through confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) whose adjustment indices were satisfactory (CFI = 0.93, TLI = 0.91, RMSEA = 0.09) and presented good reliability values in its dimensions: MLQ-P (α = 0.82) y MLQ-S (α = 0.87). These results have been corroborated in different investigations [19, 28, 29, 32,33,34,35] where a CFA supports the two-dimensional structure with adequate fit indices (CFI > 0.90, TLI > 0.90 and RMSEA < 0.08), as well as appropriate reliability values for both dimensions α > 0.77.

Regarding Latin America, the first evaluation of the psychometric properties of the MLQ was carried out in Argentina, where it was found that by eliminating item 9, the adjustment in the two-dimensional structure was adequate for the samples of adults (CFI = 0.95, TLI = 0.92, RMSEA = 0.09) and adolescents (CFI = 0.94, TLI = 91, RMSEA = 0.08). Regarding its reliability, it was shown that both dimensions were greater than 0.80 [28]. A second study conducted in Brazil proposed an orthogonal model of the MLQ, where the dimensions of MLQ-P (CFI = 0.99, TLI = 0.99, RMSEA = 0.075) and MLQ-S dimension (CFI = 0.97, TLI = 0.95, RMSEA = 0.149) presented adequate fit indices. Regarding reliability, the values ​​were satisfactory in the MLQ-P (α = 0.90) y MLQ-S (α = 0.90) [31]. These findings show no agreement on the internal structure of the scale in the Spanish-speaking population. Furthermore, it is essential to point out that only these two studies have analyzed the internal structure of the MLQ in Latin America. In a university population, no evidence of the psychometric functioning of the scale has been found.

Also, it is essential to point out that several studies show that the correlations between the MLQ-P and MLQ-S factors differ according to culture. In Nigeria [34], Iran [36], China [30], and Japan [27], there is a positive association between both dimensions, which in turn, they vary in their magnitude (0.21 to 0.72). While in countries such as Italy [35], Turkey [29], India [32], Argentina [28], the United States [6], South Africa [19] and Australia [33] show a negative relationship in different magnitudes (− 0.12 to − 0.49).

Due to the above, the lack of psychometric studies that confirm the internal structure of the MLQ in Latin America in the undergraduate students' population is evident. For this reason, this research aims to evaluate the internal structure using CTT models, obtain evidence of validity based on the relationship with other variables, perform factorial invariance according to gender, and estimate the reliability of the MLQ in Peruvian university students. In addition, the following hypotheses were proposed: (a) The MLQ scale presents adequate adjustment indices, (b) The scale presents factorial invariance according to the sex of the participants, and (c) The scale is positively related to life satisfaction and well-being; and is negatively related to depression.

Method

Participants

The sample consisted of 581 Peruvian university students, of which 231 were men (29.5%) and 350 were women (70.5%) with an age range of 18 to 40 years (M = 22.6, SD = 3.3). The vast majority (51.9%) lived with their parents, 18.8% lived alone with their mother, 3.1% only with their father, 11.4% lived with other relatives, 3.6% lived with their friends, and 11.2% lived alone. Regarding the type of university, 91.2% belonged to private institutions and 8.8% to public institutions. Only 42.2% reported working, while 57.8% did not work.

Instruments

Meaning in life questionnaire (MLQ)

The Questionnaire was developed in the United States by Steger et al. [6] and adapted to Spanish in Argentina by Góngora et al. [28]. This scale comprises ten items that measure two dimensions: the presence of meaning (1,4,5,6,9) and the search for meaning (2,3,7,8,10). In addition, the items present seven Likert-type response categories: (1) Absolutely false, (2) Mostly false, (3) Somewhat false, (4) Neither true nor false, (5) Somewhat true, (6) Mostly true, and (7) Absolutely true. Concerning the psychometric properties, in the study by Góngora et al. [28], the scale has validity based on the internal structure (CFI = 0.95, TLI = 0.92, RMSEA = 0.09). It also presents adequate levels of internal consistency for the dimensions of the presence of meaning (α = 0.78) and the search for meaning (α = 0.80).

Satisfaction with life scale (SWLS)

The Spanish version of Atienza et al. [38] was used to assess the global judgment that people make about their level of satisfaction with life. The scale is made up of five items with five response categories ranging from (1) totally disagree to (5) totally agree. Regarding the psychometric properties, the one-dimensional model presented adequate fit indices (GFI = 0.98, NFI = 0.99, NNFI = 0.99) and an adequate level of internal consistency (α = 0.84).

Weil-being index (WBI)

The version adapted to Peru by Caycho-Rodríguez et al. [39] was used in the study. The scale aims to assess the subjective well-being of the person, for which it has five items with 4 Likert-type response categories, ranging from (0) never, (1) sometimes, (2) many times, and (3) forever. In relation to its psychometric properties, the scale showed validity based on the internal structure in the unidimensional model (CFI = 0.99, RMSEA = 0.053, SRMR = 0.018) and in terms of reliability, adequate values were obtained (α = 0.85, ω = 0.88).

Patient health questionnaire (PHQ‑9)

The version adapted by Villarreal-Zegarra et al. [40] was used, which evaluates indicators of depressive symptomatology during the last two weeks. This questionnaire is made up of nine items with 4 Likert-type response categories: (0) nothing, (1) several days, (2) more than half the days, (3) almost every day. Regarding its psychometric properties, the one-dimensional model showed adequate adjustment indices (CFI = 0.94, TLI = 0.91, RMSEA = 0.089, SRMR = 0.039). Regarding reliability, adequate values were obtained in internal consistency (α = 0.87, ω = 0.87).

Procedure

In the present study, the standards of the Helsinki declaration [41], and authorization was obtained from the Ethics Committee of the Universidad Peruana Unión (2021-CE-FCS-UPeU-00230). In the initial phase, content validity was performed by five judges, who evaluated the relevance, coherence, and clarity of the items in the Peruvian context. Later, an apparent validity was performed with 18 students, who evaluated the degree of clarity and were asked to leave recommendations to understand the items better obtaining the suggestions of the two groups, some modifications were made to the items. Finally, for the data collection phase, a virtual form was developed using the Google Forms platform due to the restrictions imposed by the Peruvian Government due to COVID-19. In the first part of the form, the informed consent of the participants was obtained, where the study's objective and the anonymity of the responses were explained. Only the participants who marked the option of agreement agreed and continued to fill out the other parts of the form. The application of the form was carried out in the virtual classrooms of the students.

Data analysis

In Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA), because the items presented more than five response categories [42], the Robust Maximum Likelihood estimator was used [43]. To evaluate the fit of the models, the RMSEA, SRMR, CFI, and TLI indices were used. For the RMSEA and SRMR indices, values less than 0.08 were considered acceptable [44]. For the CFI and TLI indices, values greater than 0.95 were considered adequate [45]. To evaluate the reliability of the scale, Cronbach's alpha coefficient [46] and omega coefficient [47] was used, where a value greater than 0.70 is adequate [48].

The Multi-group Confirmatory Factor Analysis (MGCFA) was used to evaluate the factorial invariance of the scale according to sex, where a sequence of hierarchical variance models was proposed. First, configural invariance (reference model) was evaluated, followed by metric invariance (equality of factor loadings), scalar invariance (equality of factor loading and intercept), and finally, strict invariance (equality of factor loadings, intercept, and residuals). To compare the sequence of models, a formal statistical test was first used, for which the chi-square difference (Δχ2) was used, where non-significant values ​​(p > 0.05) suggest invariance between groups. Second, a modeling strategy was used, for which the differences in the CFI (ΔCFI) were used, where values ​​less than < 0.010 show the invariance of the model between the groups [49]. The RMSEA (ΔRMSEA) was also used, where differences less than < 0.015 demonstrate the invariance of the model between the groups [49].

All statistical analyzes were performed using the "lavaan" package for the CFA [50], the "semTools" package for factorial invariance [51]. In all cases, the R Studio Team [52] environment was used for r [53].

Results

Content based validity

Table 2 shows the judges' evaluation using the criteria of clarity, relevance, coherence, and context, where the values were adequate (V > 0.70). In addition, modifications were made to the items following the judges' suggestions (item 1, item 4, item 5, item 6, item 7, item 9, and item 10). On the other hand, the apparent validity was carried out, where the focus group of university students indicated that the items were understandable and clear.

Table 2 Content validity

Descriptive analysis

Table 3 shows that item 5 ("I know very well what gives meaning to my life") and item 1 ("I know what the purpose of my life is") present the highest average scores in the sample. That is, most of the participants agree with these statements. It can also be seen that item 10 ("I am looking for meaning in my life") has the lowest average score; that is, most of the participants are undecided regarding this statement ("Neither true nor false"). Regarding the asymmetry and kurtosis indices, it can be seen that all the items present adequate indices (As <  ± 2; Ku <  ± 7), according to the Finney et al. [54] criteria.

Table 3 Descriptive analysis of the items

Validity based on internal structure

Table 4 shows that the original model of the scale (model 1a) does not show adequate fit indices (RMSEA = 0.081 [IC90% [0.067—0.095]; SRMR = 0.074; CFI = 0.96; TLI = 0.95). Similarly, the two unrelated factor model (model 1b) does not fit the data (RMSEA = 0.083 [IC90% [0.070–0.097]; SRMR = 0.106; CFI = 0.96; TLI = 0.94). In the two previous models, it can be seen that item 9 shows a low factorial weight (λ < 0.49) with a high level of associated error (e5 > 0.75). Faced with this, item 9 was removed, and a model of two related factors of nine items was proposed (model 2a), which presented adequate adjustment indices (RMSEA = 0.075 [IC90% 0.059 – 0.092]; SRMR = 0.059; CFI = 0.97; TLI = 0.96). Factor weights in this model were high (λ > 0.70) with a low level of associated error in all items (e < 0.49). Due to the low relationship between the two factors (− 0.15), a fourth model of two unrelated factors was evaluated (model 2b). Table 2 shows that although this model has acceptable fit indices (RMSEA = 0.078 [IC90% 0.062 – 0.094]; SRMR = 0.090; CFI = 0.97; TLI = 0.96), model 1a continues to show better fit indices. Therefore, the model of two related factors of nine items (2a) will be used for the other analyses.

Table 4 Adjustment indices and factorial weights of the different models of the scale

Reliability estimates

In the total study sample, the dimensions of the presence of meaning (α) and the search for meaning (α = 0.86; ω = 0.87) and search for meaning (α = 0.88; ω = 0.88) present adequate reliability indices. Similarly, it occurs in the male sample: the presence of meaning (α = 0.92; ω = 0.92) and search for meaning (α = 0.86; ω = 0.86), and in the female sample: the presence of meaning (α = 0.90; ω = 0.90) and search for meaning (α = 0.89; ω = 0.89).

Factorial invariance by sex

Table 5 shows that the factorial structure of the scale has shown evidence of being strictly invariant for the groups of men and women in the sequence of proposed invariance models: metric invariance (ΔCFI = 0.001; ΔRMSEA = − 0.007), scalar (ΔCFI = 0.000; ΔRMSEA =  − 0.002) y strict (ΔCFI =  − 0.003; ΔRMSEA =  − 0.001).

Table 5 Invariance models according to sex

Validity base on the relationship to other constructs

Considering the literature review, a Structural Equation Modelling (SEM) was proposed to evaluate the latent relationship between the MLQ scale and the level of satisfaction with life [19, 28, 35, 37, 55], well-being [33, 56, 58], and depression [19, 59]. It was evidenced that the model presents adequate fit indices (χ2 = 747.75; df = 340; p = 0.000; RMSEA = 0.049 [IC90% 0.044 – 0.054]; CFI = 0.95; TLI = 0.94). Figure 1 shows that the presence of meaning in life is significantly related to the level of satisfaction with life (0.63; p < 0.01), well-being (0.60; p < 0.01), and depression (− 0.56; p < 0.01). In contrast, the search for meaning in the life dimension is not significantly related to the level of satisfaction with life (− 0.050; p > 0.05) and psychological well-being (− 0.07; p > 0.05). However, it does maintain a significant relationship with the level of depression (0.19; p < 0.01). Considering these results, it can be concluded that the scale presents validity based on that related to other constructs.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Relationship model with other variables

Discussion

The meaning of life is considered an essential factor that favors academic performance in university students [15]. About this, MLQ is one of the most used instruments for measuring this construct. However, in Latin America, no studies have yet been reported that have analyzed the psychometric properties of the MLQ in the university context. Therefore, the present study aimed to evaluate the psychometric properties of the MLQ in Peruvian university students.

Regarding the validity based on the internal structure, the results of the CFA show that model 2a presents adequate adjustment indices compared to the other models (1a, 1b y 2b). Models 1a and 1b values present adjustment problems in the indices of RMSEA (models 1a and 1b) and SRMR (1b). Also, item 9 ("My life does not have a clear purpose") is the only one that has a markedly negative connotation in the wording since it shows the lack of meaning in life. In this regard, it is important to specify that the presence of negative items decreases the reliability values [60,61,62], the variance of the scores decreases [62] and causes other additional factors in the factorial structure [62, 63]. This could explain why other psychometric studies of the MLQ conducted in the Spanish-speaking population found the presence of item 9 to be problematic [28, 64]. For all these reasons, it was considered appropriate to eliminate item 9. Subsequently, when evaluating model 2b, an excessive value was found in the SRMR, as well as a theoretical inconsistency, and the theoretical proposal of Steger et al. [6] indicates that both factors, the presence of meaning and the search for meaning, are related.

Given the above, model 2a was chosen since it presented adequate adjustment indices to the data. These findings of the two-dimensional structure are consistent with the theoretical proposal of Steger et al. [6], who points out that the meaning of life is explained by a cognitive factor that causes people to identify and understand its meaning from their experiences lived while the second motivational factor allows individuals to establish an active commitment to continue increasing their understanding about the meaning of life. Likewise, these findings coincide with previous studies carried out in Argentina [28], Ghana [37], Iran [36], United States [64] and Australia [33].

Regarding the relationship between presence and search for meaning, both dimensions show a low negative correlation, which is similar to that reported in studies carried out in countries such as Italy [35], Turkey [29], India [32], Argentina [28], United States [6], South Africa [19], and Australia [65]. However, it is different from the positive relationship reported in studies carried out in the countries of Nigeria [34], Iran [66], China [30], and Japan [27]. These differences in the relationship between both dimensions could be explained by cultural differences between countries since culture shapes the way people see themselves and their relationship with the world [67]. Also, culture influences how people develop and experience the presence and the search for meaning in life [68]. So cultural differences (individualism versus collectivism) and the resulting social and cognitive orientations (e.g., self-construction, open-mindedness) can influence the search for meaning in life. In this way, people from more collectivist cultures understand the search for meaning as a motivational force and a fundamental psychological need of the human being to understand his existence [1, 10], achieving a positive relationship between both factors. On the other hand, for people from more individualistic cultures, the search would mean an absence of meaning in life and an indicator of low personal development [8, 9, 56], which would explain a relationship negative between both factors.

Regarding reliability, the dimensions of the presence of meaning and search for meaning obtained good values in Cronbach's alpha coefficient. These findings are similar to those reported in other studies [28, 29, 34, 36, 57]. Similarly, it was found that both dimensions presented good values in the omega coefficient. It is important to note that this is the first study to show the internal consistency of the questionnaire through a more robust indicator, such as the omega coefficient [69]. Regarding the factorial invariance of the questionnaire, it was shown that the items could measure the meaning of life in men and women similarly and with the same precision. This result also suggests that the meaning of life measured through the MLQ is understood similarly by men and women. All this allows comparisons between both groups without measurement bias [70, 71]. This result is also consistent with the invariance findings in Brazil and China [31, 57].

Regarding the validity based on the relationship with other variables, it was found that the dimension of the presence of meaning in life obtained a positive relationship with life satisfaction and subjective well-being. These results are consistent with previous studies [28, 31, 35]. In addition, these results could be explained by the fact that the presence of meaning in life can lead an individual to develop moral self-acceptance while helping them find satisfaction in their lives, being a fundamental point to optimizing well-being [72]. Also, in the present study, a negative relationship between the presence of meaning and depression was evidenced. This result is consistent with previous literature since the presence of meaning in life is one of the most important factors in reducing the presence of depressive symptoms and promoting mental health indicators [13, 19].

On the other hand, it is shown that the search for meaning in life does not present a significant relationship with life satisfaction and subjective well-being. These results are different from those reported in studies conducted in South Africa [19], the United States [6, 58], Italy [35], Argentina [28] and Brazil [31]; where there is a negative relationship. This could be due to the moderating role of age; the literature mentions that for young people, the search for meaning can be normative and adaptive because they are exploring their identity. Similarly, studies show that with advancing age, there is a stronger association between the search for meaning and subjective well-being, since when the individual ages the search levels decrease [56, 73]. In this sense, having a sample of university students where most participants are young can explain the absence of association, unlike other studies with a greater age range. Likewise, the lack of relationship could be because this dimension can be experienced in different ways in people; that is, it can be understood as a factor of resilience, which motivates the individual to develop, or as an indicator of maladjustment or low personal development [58, 74]. On the other hand, the results show that the search for meaning is positively related to depression, consistent with previous studies [6, 19]. This relationship may be because people may consider the search a frustrating and challenging process that requires a deeper understanding of oneself and the world [75]. Finally, the observed relationships evidence the validity based on the relationship with other variables.

The study is not without limitations. In the first place, purposeful non-probabilistic sampling was carried out for the data collection, whose disadvantage is that it does not allow the generalization of the results. Therefore, it is suggested that future studies use probabilistic sampling. Second, the participants were 18 to 35 years, which limited the evaluation of the construct in other age groups. It is suggested that future studies expand the sample size and the age range for a greater understanding of the construct. Third, a longitudinal invariance was not performed to identify the temporal stability of the construct. Fourth, the test–retest reliability was not estimated, not allowing for evaluating the scores' temporal stability. Fifth, the construct was analyzed only by Peruvian university students. Therefore, cross-cultural studies are suggested to study whether the construct is similar in other Latin American countries.

Despite the limitations, this study showed that the MLQ has adequate psychometric properties from the perspective of CTT. In addition, the study provided evidence in favor of the two-dimensional model. Likewise, it was found that the scale is invariant in men and women, which will allow comparing the meaning of life between both groups without measurement bias. In addition, it was reported that the MLQ presents evidence of validity based on the relationship with other variables such as life satisfaction, well-being, and depression.

For all the above, the study provides the first psychometric evidence of the MLQ in the Peruvian university context, being the first study in Latin America, allowing the meaning of life to be adequately measured in university students.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

References

  1. Frankl VE. Man’s search for meaning. Herder; 1979.

  2. King LA, Heintzelman SJ, Ward SJ. Beyond the search for meaning: a contemporary science of the experience of meaning in life. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2016;25(4):211–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Scheier MF, Wrosch C, Baum A, Cohen S, Martire LM, Matthews KA, et al. The life engagement test: assessing purpose in life. J Behav Med. 2006;29(3):291–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Park C, Folkman S. Meaning in the context of stress and coping. Rev Gen Psychol. 1997;1(2):115–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Ryff CD, Singer B. The contours of positive human health. Psychol Inq. 1998;9(1):1–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Steger M, Frazier P, Kaler M, Oishi S. The meaning in life questionnaire: assessing the presence of and search for meaning in life. J Couns Psychol. 2006;53(1):80–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Steger M. Meaning in Life. Oxford handb posit psychol (2 Ed) [Internet]. 2009 [cited 2021 Sep 30]; Available from: https://www.oxfordhandbooks.com/view/https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195187243.001.0001/oxfordhb-9780195187243-e-064

  8. Baumeister RF. Meanings of life. New York: The Guilford Press; 1991.

    Google Scholar 

  9. Klinger E. The search for meaning in evolutionary perspective and its clinical implications. In: The human quest for meaning: a handbook of psychological research and clinical applications. Mahwah, NJ, US: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers; 1998. p. 27–50.

  10. Maddi SR. Creating meaning through making decisions. In: The human quest for meaning: theories, research, and applications, 2nd ed. New York, NY, US: Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group; 2012. p. 57–80. (Personality and clinical psychology series.).

  11. Steger M, Kashdan TB, Sullivan BA, Lorentz D. Understanding the search for meaning in life: personality, cognitive style, and the dynamic between seeking and experiencing meaning. J Pers. 2008;76(2):199–228.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Garrison YL, Lee KH. Emotionality and tolerance of uncertainty and their associations with meaning in life among Korean college students. Pers Individ Dif. 2017;1(112):26–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Maryam MA, Mahmoud MA. An investigation of the relationship between depression, meaning in life and adult hope. Procedia - Soc Behav Sci. 2014;21(114):598–601.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Li P, Wong Y, Chao R. Happiness and meaning in life: Unique, differential, and indirect associations with mental health. Couns Psychol Q. 2019;32(3):396–414.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Bailey T, Phillips L. The influence of motivation and adaptation on students’ subjective well-being, meaning in life and academic performance. High Educ Res Dev. 2015;35(2):201–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Mason HD. Meaning as a coping resource: experiences of nursing students. J Psychol Africa [Internet]. 2017;27(1):74–9. https://doi.org/10.1080/14330237.2016.1268294.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Van Tongeren DR, Green JD, Davis DE, Hook JN, Hulsey TL. Prosociality enhances meaning in life. J Posit Psychol [Internet]. 2016;11(3):225–36. https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2015.1048814.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Yu EA, Chang EC. Meaning in life as a predictor of optimism: how parents mattering matters to Asian and European Americans. Pers Individ Dif. 2019;138(1):366–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Temane L, Khumalo IP, Wissing MP. Validation of the meaning in life questionnaire in a South African context. J Psychol Africa. 2014;24(1):51–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Kleiman EM, Adams LM, Kashdan TB, Riskind JH. Gratitude and grit indirectly reduce risk of suicidal ideations by enhancing meaning in life: evidence for a mediated moderation model. J Res Pers. 2013;47(5):539–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Park J, Baumeister RF. Meaning in life and adjustment to daily stressors. J Posit Psychol. 2017;12(4):333–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Klein N. Prosocial behavior increases perceptions of meaning in life. J Posit Psychol. 2017;12(4):354–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Trzebiński J, Cabański M, Czarnecka JZ. Reaction to the COVID-19 pandemic: the influence of meaning in life, life satisfaction, and assumptions on world orderliness and positivity. J Loss Trauma. 2020;25(6–7):544–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Battista J, Almond R. The development of meaning in life. Psychiatry. 1973;36(4):409–27.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Reker G. Meaning in life of young, middle-aged, and older adults: factorial validity, age, and gender invariance of the personal meaning index (PMI). Pers Individ Dif. 2005;38(1):71–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Francis L, Hills P. The development of the meaning in life index (MILI) and its relationship with personality and religious behaviours and beliefs among UK undergraduate students. Ment Heal Relig Cult. 2008;11(2):211–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Steger M, Kawabata Y, Shimai S, Otake K. The meaningful life in Japan and the United States: levels and correlates of meaning in life. J Res Pers. 2008;42(3):660–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Góngora V, Solano AC. Validación del cuestionario de significado de la vida MLQ en población adulta y adolescente argentina. Rev Interam Psicol J Psychol. 2011;45(3):395–404.

    Google Scholar 

  29. Boyraz G, Lightsey OR, Can A. The Turkish version of the meaning in life questionnaire: assessing the measurement invariance across Turkish and American adult samples. J Pers Assess. 2013;95(4):423–31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Chan WCH. Factor structure of the Chinese version of the meaning in life questionnaire among Hong Kong Chinese caregivers. Heal Soc Work. 2014;39(3):135–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Figueiredo-Damásio B, Koller SH. Meaning in life questionnaire: adaptation process and psychometric properties of the Brazilian version. Rev Latinoam Psicol [Internet]. 2015;47(3):185–95.

    Google Scholar 

  32. Singh K, Junnarkar M, Jaswal S, Kaur J. Validation of meaning in life questionnaire in hindi (MLQ-H). Ment Heal Relig Cult. 2016;19(5):448–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Rose LM, Zask A, Burton LJ. Psychometric properties of the meaning in life questionnaire (MLQ) in a sample of Australian adolescents. Int J Adolesc Youth. 2017;22(1):68–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Chika JB, Ekpedoho EA, Ifeagwazi CM, Iorfa SK, Nwonyi SK. Psychometric properties of the meaning in life questionnaire – Hausa version among internally displaced persons in Nigeria. Transcult Psychiatry. 2019;56(1):103–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Negri L, Bassi M, Delle FA. Italian validation of the meaning in life questionnaire: factor structure, reliability, convergent, and discriminant validity. Psychol Rep. 2020;123(2):578–600.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Naghiyaee M, Bahmani B, Asgari A. The psychometric properties of the meaning in life questionnaire (MLQ) in patients with life-threatening illnesses. Sci World J. 2020;2020:8361602.

  37. Wilson-Fadiji A, Meiring L, Wissing MP. Understanding well-being in the Ghanaian context: linkages between lay conceptions of well-being and measures of hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. Appl Res Qual Life. 2021;16(2):649–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Atienza FL, Pons D, Balaguer I, García-Merita M. Propiedades psicométricas de la Escala de Satisfacción con la Vida en adolescentes. Psicothema [Internet]. 2000;12(12):314–9.

    Google Scholar 

  39. Caycho-Rodríguez T, Ventura-León J, Azabache-Alvarado K, Reyes-Bossio M, Cabrera-Orosco I. Validez e invariancia factorial del Índice de Bienestar general (WHO-5 WBI) en universitarios peruanos. Rev Ciencias La Salud. 2020;18(3):1–15.

    Google Scholar 

  40. Villarreal-Zegarra D, Copez-Lonzoy A, Bernabé-Ortiz A, Melendez-Torres GJ, Bazo-Alvarez JC. Valid group comparisons can be made with the patient health questionnaire (PHQ-9): a measurement invariance study across groups by demographic characteristics. PLoS One [Internet]. 2019;14(9):0221717. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0221717.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Asociación Mundial de Medicina. Declaración de Helsinki de la Asociación Médica Mundial - Principios éticos para las investigaciones médicas en seres humanos [Internet]. Asociación Mundial de Medicina. 2015. Available from: https://bit.ly/3MlYIvv

  42. Rhemtulla M, Brosseau-Liard PÉ, Savalei V. When can categorical variables be treated as continuous? A comparison of robust continuous and categorical SEM estimation methods under suboptimal conditions. Psychol Methods. 2012;17(3):354–73.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Yuan K-H, Bentler PM. Three Likelihod-based methods for mean and covariance structure analysis with non-normal missing data. Socio Methodol. 2000;30(1):165–200.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Kline RB. Principles and practice of structural equation modeling, 4th ed. Principles and practice of structural equation modeling, 4th ed. New York, NY, US: Guilford Press; 2016. xvii, 534–xvii, 534. (Methodology in the social sciences.).

  45. Schumacker RE, Lomax RG. A Beginner’s guide to structural equation modeling: Fourth Edition. Routledge; 2015.

  46. Cronbach LJ. Coefficient alpha and the internal structure of tests. Psychometrika [Internet]. 1951;16(3):297–334. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02310555.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. McDonald RP. Test theory: a unified treatment. Taylor & Francis; 1999.

  48. Viladrich C, Angulo-Brunet A, Doval E. Un viaje alrededor de alfa y omega para estimar la fiabilidad de consistencia interna. An Psicol. 2017;33(3):755–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Chen FF. Sensitivity of goodness of fit indexes to lack of measurement invariance. Struct Equ Model [Internet]. 2007;14(3):464–504. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705510701301834.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Rosseel Y. lavaan: an r package for structural equation modeling. J Stat Softw [Internet]. 2012;48(2):1–36.

    Google Scholar 

  51. Jorgensen TD, Pornprasertmanit S, Schoemann AM, Rosseel Y. semTools: Useful tools for structural equation modeling [Internet]. 2018. Available from: https://cran.r-project.org/package=semTools

  52. R Studio Team. A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2021.

  53. R Core Team. A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2019.

  54. Finney SJ, DiStefano C. Nonnormal and categorical data in structural equation modeling. In: In G r Hancock & R O Mueller (Hrsg) Structural equation modeling: a second course. Greenwich: Connecticut: Information Age Publishing; 2006. p. 269–314.

  55. Yu EA, Chang EC. Relational meaning in life as a predictor of interpersonal well-being: a prospective analysis. Pers Individ Dif. 2021;1:168.

    Google Scholar 

  56. Li J, Dou K, Liang Y. The relationship between presence of meaning search for meaning and subjective well-being: a three-level meta-analysis based on the meaning in life questionnaire. J Happiness Stud [Internet]. 2021;22(1):467–89. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-020-00230-y.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Datu JAD, Yuen M. Factorial validity of meaning in life questionnaire in Hong Kong secondary school students: a construct validation approach. Couns Psychol Q. 2021;35(2):467.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Steger M, Oishi S, Kesebir S. Is a life without meaning satisfying? The moderating role of the search for meaning in satisfaction with life judgments. J Posit Psychol. 2011;6(3):173–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Gross GM, Laws H, Park CL, Hoff R, Hoffmire CA. Meaning in life following deployment sexual trauma: prediction of posttraumatic stress symptoms, depressive symptoms, and suicidal ideation. Psychiatry Res. 2019;1(278):78–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Menold N. How do reverse-keyed items in inventories affect measurement quality and information processing? Field Methods. 2019;32(2):140–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  61. Salazar MS. El dilema de combinar ítems positivos y negativos en escalas. Psicothema. 2015;27(2):192–9.

    Google Scholar 

  62. Suárez-Alvarez J, Pedrosa I, Lozano LM, García-Cueto E, Cuesta M, Muñiz J. Using reversed items in likert scales: a questionable practice. Psicothema. 2018;30(2):149–58.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Woods CM. Careless responding to reverse-worded items: Implications for confirmatory factor analysis. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. 2006;28(3):189–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  64. Vela JC, Karaman MA, Garcia C, Aguilar A. Evaluation of Spanish versions of meaning in life and hope measures with Latinx students. Meas Eval Couns Dev. 2020;53(2):104–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  65. Hallford DJ, Mellor D, Cummins RA, McCabe MP. Meaning in Life in earlier and later older-adulthood: confirmatory factor analysis and correlates of the meaning in life questionnaire. J Appl Gerontol. 2018;37(10):1270–94.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Tahereh D, Rasoul Y, Hossein TM, Reyhaneh D, Jamal A. The relationship between meaning in life and depression, anxiety and stress status among college students of Iran university of medical sciences in 2013. J Educ Health Promot [Internet]. 2013;1(3):83–92.

    Google Scholar 

  67. Singelis TM, Triandis HC, Bhawuk DPS, Gelfand MJ. Horizontal and vertical dimensions of individualism and collectivism: a theoretical and measurement refinement. [Internet]. 2016;29(3):240–75. https://doi.org/10.1177/106939719502900302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  68. Koltko-Rivera ME. The psychology of worldviews. Rev General Psychol [Internet]. 2004;8(1):3–58. https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.8.1.3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  69. Zinbarg RE, Revelle W, Yovel I, Li W. Cronbach’s, α Revelle’s β and McDonald’s ω H: Their relations with each other and two alternative conceptualizations of reliability. Psychometrika. 2005;70(1):123–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  70. Dimitrov DM. Testing for factorial invariance in the context of construct Validation. Meas Eval Couns Dev. 2010;43(2):121–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  71. Svetina D, Rutkowski L, Rutkowski D. Multiple-group invariance with categorical outcomes using updated guidelines: an illustration using Mplus and the lavaan/semtools packages. Struct Equ Model. 2020;27(1):111–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  72. Jauncey T, Strodl E. Love of god, others, and self and their association with satisfaction with life and mental health for christians. J Spiritual Ment Heal. 2018;20(3):239–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  73. Steger M, Oishi S, Kashdan T. Meaning in life across the life span: levels and correlates of meaning in life from emerging adulthood to older adulthood. J Posit Psychol. 2009;4(1):43–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  74. Park N, Park M, Peterson C. When is the search for meaning related to life satisfaction? Appl Psychol Heal Well-Being [Internet]. 2010;2(1):1–13. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1758-0854.2009.01024.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  75. Park H, Jeong D. Moderation effects of perfectionism and meaning in life on depression. Pers Individ Dif. 2016;1(98):25–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable

Funding

The authors received no financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

AT-C, LWV, JQ-B, and SLH provided initial conception, organization, and main writing of the text. LWV and TC-R analyzed the data and prepared all figures and tables. AT-C, LWV, JQ-B, SLH, RD-V, and TC-R were involved in data collection and acted as consultants and contributors to research design, data analysis, and text writing, read and approved the draft. All authors read and approved by the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Lindsey W. Vilca.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The study obtained the approval of the ethics committee of the Center for Research and Innovation in Health of the Universidad Peruana Unión (2021-CE-FCS-UPeU-00230), and the standards of the Helsinki declaration were met. The informed consent of the participants was obtained, where the study's objective and the anonymity of the responses were explained.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Travezaño-Cabrera, A., Vilca, L.W., Quiroz-Becerra, J. et al. Meaning of life questionnaire (MLQ) in peruvian undergraduate students: study of its psychometric properties from the perspective of classical test theory (CTT). BMC Psychol 10, 206 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-022-00913-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-022-00913-6

Keywords