Skip to main content

Are all types of empathy associated with lower aggression in athletes? A cross-sectional study on Iranian athletes

Abstract

Background

This study aimed to evaluate the association between cognitive and affective empathy and aggression in a sample of Iranian athletes.

Methods

We designed a cross-sectional study. The participants were selected by multistage random sampling among six colleges in Tehran, Iran’s capital. We used the interpersonal reactivity index (IRI) to evaluate empathy, and Reactive Proactive Aggression Questionnaire, and the Buss–Perry aggression questionnaire to evaluate aggression.

Results

In total, 492 athletes with a mean age of 27.42 years (SD = 7.72) participated in the study, of which 298 (60.6%) were male, and 194 (39.4%) were female. IRI’s fantasy and personal distress subscales scores were positively associated with proactive and reactive aggression scores (p < 0.05). The score of the perspective-taking subscale of IRI was negatively associated with proactive and reactive aggression scores (p < 0.05). The score of the empathic concern subscale of IRI had a negative association with the proactive aggression score (p < 0.001). The score of the perspective-taking subscale of IRI had negative associations with all Buss–Perry aggression questionnaire subscales’ scores (p < 0.05). The score of the personal distress subscale of IRI had positive associations with all Buss–Perry aggression questionnaire subscales’ scores (p < 0.05), except with the verbal aggression subscale score. The score of the fantasy subscale of IRI was positively associated with the score of the hostility subscale of the Buss–Perry questionnaire (p = 0.001).

Conclusion

Perspective-taking is negatively associated with all kinds of aggression in athletes. Future studies can be conducted to determine the possible role of perspective-taking in preventive aggression, which can be a target for interventions. On the other hand, the score of the personal distress subscale of IRI is positively associated with all types of aggression scores, indicating that not all types of empathy inhibit aggression in athletes.

Peer Review reports

Introduction

Physical and verbal aggressions are common behaviors among athletes, especially in contact sports [1,2,3]. Aggression is defined by Baron et al. as “any form of behavior directed toward the goal of harming or injuring another living being who is motivated to avoid such treatment [4].” Aggressive behaviors are categorized into two subtypes. The first subtype is called reactive or hostile aggression. Reactive aggression is an impulsive or emotionally aggressive response to a minimal provocation that has emerged as a loss of behavioral control. The second subtype is called proactive or instrumental aggression [5]. In contrast, proactive aggression is the planned, instrumental behavior involving computed efforts to get important resources [5]. However, other terminologies beyond reactive and proactive aggression have been utilized to describe aggression in the sport and non-sport contexts. In sports, aggression is commonly divided into sanctioned and unsanctioned types [6]. In brief, sanctioned aggressions are not out of the game’s laws and roles, contrasting the unsanctioned aggression [7]. Unsanctioned aggression may lead to physical and psychological consequences, and there are various motivations for unsanctioned aggression, such as anger, power, and thrill [6, 8].

Aggression in the sports context has been the subject of studies for years. The roles of various factors, such as gender, the importance of the game, the type of sport, and empathy in athletes’ aggression, have been evaluated in previous studies [9,10,11,12]. There are some controversies on the role of aggression in athletes’ performance. It is suggested that hostile aggression may improve the athletes’ performance by raising their arousal to optimal levels [13, 14]. Also, physicality and authorized aggression were perceived as positive mechanisms in certain women’s sports because there is a myth about aggression and anxiety will increase athletic performance [7]. Although it seems true sometimes, studies have shown that aggression does not facilitate performance. For example, aggression increases people’s arousal level and the focused attention to the results of non-executive cues. (e.g., hurting the opponent) that could reduce or interfere with the performance [15, 16].

Studies have shown that aggression has increased among athletes in the past decades. The rise in aggressive behaviors could have resulted from anger rumination, provocation, and the use of anabolic steroids [17,18,19,20]. Several studies on athletes’ aggression and its mediating factors have been conducted. It is found that aggressive behavior could have serious consequences, such as injury, thus enhancing the risk of further damage [21]. In addition to the effects of aggression on performance, aggressive behavior could lead to the players’ exclusion or legal consequences [22]. The International Society of Sport Psychology (ISSP) has some recommendations to reduce aggression in sports, such as considering serious punishments for violating roles by authorities and coaches, participation of athletes in programs designed to reduce aggression tendencies, and emphasizing fair play from junior levels [23]. Therefore, it is crucial to examine mediator factors of aggression to design interventions to regulate athletes’ aggression.

One of the factors related to aggressive behavior is empathy. Studies showed that empathic responses negatively correlated with prosocial and/or antisocial behavior [24, 25]. A person with high empathy could understand others’ feelings via perspective-taking by imagining how the other person feels [26]. Some researchers have intended two components for empathy: cognitive empathy, understanding others’ emotions, and affective empathy, an emotional response, such as verbal or facial, to others’ emotions [27, 28]. It is shown that different anatomical sites of the brain are responsible for cognitive and affective empathy [29]. People with less cognitive empathy have less ability to tolerate opposite viewpoints, leading to aggressive behaviors [30]. Also, individuals with low affective empathy could not understand others’ suffering, the pain inflicted, and the fear of the victim [31]. Conversely, empathy enhancement has inhibited aggressive acts in a bid to reduce their emotional distress. Hence, empathy’s cognitive and affective components seem to inhibit aggression [26]. It should be noted that cognitive and affective empathy may have different roles in preventing aggressive behavior [32]. It is shown that high levels of affective empathy in girls can reduce cyberbullying, regardless of cognitive empathy [33]. However, roles of affective and cognitive empathy in preventing aggression in athletes has not been evaluated yet.

Association between empathy and aggression has been studied in athletes [11, 26] and non-athlete [34, 35] populations, which have found inhibitory effects of empathy on aggression prevention. Previous findings are related to athletes from specific countries, and there is little data on athletes with different backgrounds, especially in developing countries [36]. It gets more important as ethnicity is one of the factors affecting individuals’ normative beliefs, which can moderate the association between empathy and aggression [37, 38]. Also, none of the previous studies separately evaluated cognitive and affective empathy’s role in aggressive behavior. Therefore, this study aimed to evaluate the association between cognitive and affective empathy on aggression in a sample of Iranian athletes. Determining the possible association between aggression and empathy among athletes in developing countries with different backgrounds and ethnicities than athletes in developed countries can enhance our knowledge in this regard. We hypothesized that all kinds of empathy have a negative correlation with aggression among athletes.

Materials and methods

We designed a cross-sectional study. The study protocol was according to the declaration of Helsinki. The ethical committee of the Tehran University of Medical Sciences approved the study protocol (code: IR.TUMS.NI.REC.1399.056).

Participants

We defined an athlete as someone who is recently registered in a sports team, trained with the team, and participated in sports competitions. Sports were also classified as contact and non-contact. All the participants were selected by multistage random sampling among six colleges and twenty-four classes in Tehran, Iran’s capital. In each college, four classes were randomly selected, and 700 athletes were randomly selected from the students of the included classes. We provided a complete explanation of the study goals and objectives, and those interested were enrolled in the study. All the participants filled out their consent forms. We sent the questionnaires to 700 athletes after they gave informed consent to participate in the study and asked them to complete the questionnaires independently. In total, 492 athletes with a mean age of 27.42 years (SD = 7.72) participated in the study, of which 298 (60.6%) were male, and 194 (39.4%) were female. One hundred and ninety-seven (40%) of the participants were married, and 347 (70.5%) had a university degree (Table 1). Also, 344 (69.9%) were contact sports athletes, and 87 (17.7%) team sport athletes (Table 2).

Table 1 Empathy and aggression scores across demographic groups
Table 2 Aggression and empathy across different kinds of sports. Values are reported as mean (SD)

Measures

Empathy

The Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI) [39] is a 28-item questionnaire answered on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 0 (does not describe me well) to 4 (describes me very well). The measure has four subscales, and each has seven items: personal distress, empathic concern, fantasy, and perspective-taking. Empathic concern and personal distress measure affective empathy. While fantasy and perspective-taking measure cognitive empathy. The total and each subscale’s scores were calculated by summing all related item scores, and higher scores indicated more empathy, except for nine items, which should be scored reversely [39,40,41]. We used the Persian version of the interpersonal reactivity index, a reliable (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.79) and valid tool for evaluating empathy in the Iranian population [42].

Aggression

The Reactive Proactive Aggression Questionnaire (RPQ) is a self-report questionnaire with 23 questions that measure aggression. This tool assesses adults’ reactive aggression (11 items) and proactive aggression (12 items). Questions are scored on a 3-point Likert scale (never, sometimes, and often) from 0 to 2, in which higher scores indicate higher levels of aggression. Total and subscales’ scores are the sum of all related items’ scores. We used the Persian version of RPQ in our study. Convergent validity and internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha of 0.85 in males and 0.83 in females) of the Persian version of RPQ have been demonstrated in previous studies [43,44,45].

The Buss–Perry aggression questionnaire is a 29-item questionnaire that measures four subscales: verbal aggression (5 items), physical aggression (9 items), anger (7 items), and hostility (8 items). This questionnaire is rated on a 5-points Likert scale from 1 (does not apply in my case) to 5 (completely true in my case). Total and each subscale’s scores are the sum of all related items’ scores. We used the Persian version of the Buss–Perry aggression questionnaire in our study, which has its internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha of higher than 0.7 for all subscales), test-retest reliability, concurrent validity, and known group validity been demonstrated in previous studies [46,47,48].

Statistical analysis

We calculated the mean and standard deviation (SD) for continuous variables and the number and percentage for categorical variables. We used the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test to determine whether the continuous variables, including age, IRI, RPQ, and Buss–Perry aggression questionnaires and their subscales’ scores, are distributed normally or not. As none of the variables were distributed normally, we used non-parametric tests, including Mann-Whitney and Kruskal Wallis tests, to compare these variables across groups. We used Pearson’s correlation test to evaluate the association between empathy and aggression subscales. Correlation coefficients were interpreted as follows: 0.1–0.39: weak correlation; 0.4–0.69: moderate correlation, 0.7–0.89: strong correlation; 0.9–1: very strong correlation [49]. Also, we used multiple stepwise linear regressions to determine the factors independently associated with aggression questionnaires’ subscales. We used SPSS version 22 for data analysis. p value ≤ 0.05 was considered statistically significant. We performed a posthoc power analysis using G*Power version 3.1.9.7 [50, 51].

Results

The mean IRI score was 63.36 (SD = 11.37) among our participants. Males had significantly lower scores than females in the fantasy (13.56 vs. 15.66; p < 0.001), empathic concern (19.11 vs. 20.95; p < 0.001), personal distress (11.92 vs. 15.52; p < 0.001) subscales, and total IRI score (60.23 vs. 68.28; p < 0.001). Also, 21 to 25 years old participants had significantly lower scores in the empathic concern subscale than older participants (p < 0.05). Participants who were 17 to 20 years old had significantly lower scores in the perspective-taking subscale than those older than 30 years (15.19 vs. 16.85; p < 0.05). Perspective-taking subscale score and IRI total score were significantly higher in athletes who played non-contact sports than those who played contact sports (p < 0.05). There were no other significant differences between demographic groups regarding their IRI and its subscales’ scores (p > 0.05).

The mean RPQ score was 9.73 (SD = 5.83) among our participants. Males had significantly higher scores than females in the proactive aggression subscale (2.94 vs. 1.92; p < 0.01). Those aged 17 to 20 years had significantly higher proactive and reactive aggression subscales and RPQ total scores than those aged 26 years or older (p < 0.05). Also, participants who were 21 to 25 years had significantly higher scores in the reactive aggression subscales and RPQ total score than those older than 30 years (p < 0.05). Married participants had lower scores across all RPQ subscales and their total scores than unmarried participants (p < 0.05). There were no other significant differences between groups regarding the RPQ and its subscales’ scores (p > 0.05).

The mean total score of the Buss–Perry aggression questionnaire was 69.82 (SD = 21.68) among our participants. Males had significantly higher scores than females in the physical aggression subscale (22.5 vs. 20.44; p < 0.01); however, females had higher scores in the anger subscale (18.08 vs. 16.75; p < 0.05). Participants older than 30 years had significantly lower scores than younger participants in the physical aggression subscale score and Buss–Perry aggression questionnaire total score (p < 0.05). Participants older than 30 also had significantly lower scores than those aged 17 to 25 in the hostility subscale (p < 0.05). The Buss–Perry aggression questionnaire’s total score and its subscales’ scores, except the anger subscale, were significantly higher in unmarried participants than in married ones (p < 0.05). There were no other significant differences between groups regarding the Buss–Perry aggression questionnaire total score and subscales’ scores (p > 0.05).

The score of the fantasy subscale of the IRI had weak positive correlations with the RPQ and Buss–Perry aggression questionnaire and all their subscales’ scores (p < 0.05). The empathic concern subscale score correlated weakly and reversely with RPQ and its proactive subscale scores (p < 0.05). It also had weak reverse correlations with the score of the verbal aggression subscale of the Buss–Perry aggression questionnaire (p < 0.05). The score of the perspective-taking subscale of the IRI had weak reverse correlations with the RPQ and Buss–Perry aggression questionnaire and all their subscales’ scores (p < 0.05). The score of the personal distress subscale of the IRI had weak positive correlations with RPQ and Buss–Perry aggression questionnaire and all their subscales’ scores (p < 0.05), except with proactive aggression score. IRI total score had a weak positive correlation with the score of the reactive aggression subscale of RPQ (p < 0.05). It also had weak positive correlations (p < 0.05) with the scores of the anger and hostility subscales of the Buss–Perry aggression questionnaire (Table 3).

Table 3 Correlation between empathy and aggression in athletes

Results of multiple linear regression evaluating the factors independently associated with different kinds of aggression are shown in Tables 4 and 5. The scores of the IRI’s fantasy and personal distress subscales were positively associated with proactive and reactive regression scores (p < 0.05). The score of the perspective-taking subscale of IRI was reversely associated with proactive and reactive aggression scores (p < 0.05). The score of the empathic concern subscale of IRI had a negative association with the proactive aggression score (p < 0.001).

Table 4 Results of the multiple linear regression determining the factors that independently predicted RPQ subscales’ scores
Table 5 The multiple linear regression results determine the factors that independently associated with Buss–Perry aggression questionnaire subscales’ scores

The score of the perspective-taking subscale of IRI had negative associations with all Buss–Perry aggression questionnaire subscales’ scores (p < 0.05). The score of the personal distress subscale of IRI had positive associations with all Buss–Perry aggression questionnaire subscales’ scores (p < 0.05), except with the verbal aggression subscale score. The fantasy subscale of IRI had a positive association with the score of the hostility subscale of the Buss–Perry questionnaire (p = 0.001).

Discussion

Few studies have been conducted to evaluate the association between aggression and empathy among athletes. Still, to the best of our knowledge, this is the first study evaluating such an association among a group of Iranian athletes. Our major finding is that not all kinds of empathy are negatively related to aggression among athletes. Perspective-taking and empathic concern are negatively related to different kinds of aggression, but on the other hand, personal distress and fantasy had positive associations with aggression.

We found that perspective-taking was negatively related to proactive aggression, reactive aggression, physical aggression, verbal aggression, anger, and hostility. This finding aligns with previous studies, which reported that perspective-taking and empathy could prevent aggression in athletes [11, 26]. In 2012, Stanger et al. found that taking the opponent’s perspective can inhibit aggression in athletes, especially among male athletes. They also found that feeling guilt is a mediator of the association between empathy and aggression, as people with higher empathy experienced more guilt when they acted aggressively. They were also less likely to aggress [11]. In another study, Stanger et al. found that perspective-taking could inhibit aggression in male and female athletes at low provocation; however, at high provocation, perspective-taking could only prevent aggression in females [26]. These studies are in line with our findings as we found that perspective-taking is reversely associated with all types of aggression, including reactive aggression, which was evaluated in the Stanger et al. study [26]. Davis defined perspective-taking in IRI as the “tendency or ability of the respondent to adopt the perspective, or point of view, of other people” [40], which can be considered a cognitive component of empathy [52]. In previous studies on students, affective perspective-taking was more responsible for preventing aggression than cognitive empathy [53]. Ours and Stanger et al.’s [11, 26] findings indicate the possible role of cognitive empathy in preventing aggression in athletes, which may have a stronger role in preventing aggression than in other populations. Interventions to enhance perspective-taking in athletes may effectively prevent aggression considering its role in preventing all types of aggression and similar successful interventions in other populations [54].

One of the differences between our findings and Stanger et al. was that the perspective-taking score had a reverse association with the anger score in our study, which is in contrast to Stanger et al.’s findings, as they did not find any association between empathy and anger [26]. There may be several reasons for such a difference. First, Stanger et al. used Taylor Aggression Paradigm to evaluate aggression, provoked and unprovoked, in a competitive context [55]. In their study, the fictitious opponents used electrical shock for provocation. The authors hypothesized that participants might have felt that the opponents were intentionally hurting them, which may be a reason for reduced empathy toward their opponents [26]. Second, we used self-report questionnaires in our study, and people may act differently in real-world and task-based scenarios. Future studies, especially in real-world situations, are needed to determine the association between anger and perspective-taking in athletes, as there are controversies in this regard.

We found that the score of the personal distress subscale of IRI was positively associated with all types of aggression, except verbal aggression, in contrast to empathy’s inhibitory role in aggression. There are several possible explanations for this finding. First, personal distress has been described as the negative side effect of empathy [56], as not all types of empathy are beneficial [57], and internal distresses may prevent people with higher personal distress scores from empathic interactions [56]. Also, personal distress was found to have associations with neuroticism in previous studies [56], and neuroticism is by itself associated with emotions that often precipitate aggression, such as anger, which can explain the association between personal distress and aggression [58].

The main limitation of this study is that we used self-reported questionnaires as people may have different behaviors than what they report in such questionnaires, considering the role of provocation and arousal in the emergence of aggressive behavior [11, 26]. In addition, these questionnaires are not specifically designed to evaluate athletes and evaluated aggression and empathy in general. Future scenario-based or real-world evaluations are needed to determine the association between empathy better. Also, our study had a cross-sectional design unsuitable for evaluating the causal relationship between variables. Also, we only evaluated Iranian athletes in our study, and there is a need for more studies on aggression and the factors affecting it in athletes from developing countries.

Conclusion

Perspective-taking is negatively associated with all kinds of aggression in athletes. Future studies can be conducted to determine the possible role of perspective-taking in preventive aggression, which can be a target for interventions. On the other hand, the score of the personal distress subscale of IRI is positively associated with all types of aggression scores, indicating that not all types of empathy inhibit aggression in athletes.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets generated and/or analysed during the current study are not publicly available due to ensure participants’ confidentiality but are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

IRI:

Interpersonal reactivity index

RPQ:

Reactive Proactive Aggression Questionnaire

SD:

Standard deviation

References

  1. Ribeiro E, de Azevedo M. Empathy and the will to win: two poles in permanent tension in sport. Fair Play Revista de Filosofía Ética y Derecho del Deporte. 2018;11:68–95.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Volk AA, Lagzdins L. Bullying and victimization among adolescent girl athletes. Athl Insight. 2009;11:15–33.

    Google Scholar 

  3. Vveinhardt J, Fominiene VB. Prevalence of bullying and harassment in youth sport: The case of different types of sport and participant role. J Hum Sport Exerc. 2020:1–21.

  4. Baron RA, Richardson DR. Human aggression. Springer; 2004.

    Google Scholar 

  5. Fite PJ, Rubens SL, Preddy TM, Raine A, Pardini DA. Reactive/proactive aggression and the development of internalizing problems in males: the moderating effect of parent and peer relationships. Aggress Behav. 2014;40(1):69–78.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Kerr JH. The enjoyment of sanctioned aggression in rugby: the experience of a pioneering female Canadian team captain. Int J Sport Exerc Psychol. 2019;17(6):578–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Kerr JH, Grange P. Proposed intervention strategies for unsanctioned aggression management: anger, power and thrill aggression. Sport Psychol. 2016;30(2):179–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Spaaij R, Schaillée H. Unsanctioned aggression and violence in amateur sport: a multidisciplinary synthesis. Aggress Violent Beh. 2019;44:36–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Lefebvre L, Passer M. The effects of game location and importance on aggression in team sport. Int J Sport Psychol. 1974

  10. Sofia R, Cruz JFA. Unveiling anger and aggression in sports: the effects of type of sport, competitive category and success level. Revista de Psicología del Deporte. 2017;26(2):21–8.

    Google Scholar 

  11. Stanger N, Kavussanu M, Ring C. Put yourself in their boots: effects of empathy on emotion and aggression. J Sport Exerc Psychol. 2012;34(2):208–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Tucker LW, Parks JB. Effects of gender and sport type on intercollegiate athletes’ perceptions of the legitimacy of aggressive behaviors in sport. Sociol Sport J. 2001;18(4):403–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Cox RH, Cox RH. Sport psychology: concepts and applications. 2002.

  14. Lemieux P, McKelvie SJ, Stout D. Self-reported hostile aggression in contact athletes, no contact athletes and non-athletes. Athl Insight. 2002;4(3):42–56.

    Google Scholar 

  15. Weinberg RS, Gould D. Foundations of sport and exercise psychology, 6E: Human Kinetics; 2014.

  16. Stanger N, Kavussanu M, Ring C. Gender moderates the relationship between empathy and aggressiveness in sport: the mediating role of anger. J Appl Sport Psychol. 2017;29(1):44–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Blair R. Traits of empathy and anger: implications for psychopathy and other disorders associated with aggression. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci. 2018;373(1744):20170155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Isberg L. Anger, aggressive behavior, and athletic performance. Emotions in sport. 2000;1:13–33.

    Google Scholar 

  19. Keeler LA. The differences in sport aggression, life aggression, and life assertion among adult male and female collision, contact, and non-contact sport athletes. J Sport Behav. 2007;30(1).

  20. Maravelias C, Dona A, Stefanidou M, Spiliopoulou C. Adverse effects of anabolic steroids in athletes: a constant threat. Toxicol Lett. 2005;158(3):167–75.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Bredemeier BJ, Shields DL. Athletic aggression: An issue of contextual morality. Sociol Sport J. 1986;3(1):15–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Tenenbaum G, Sacks DN, Miller JW, Golden AS, Doolin N. Aggression and violence in sport: a reply to Kerr’s rejoinder. Sport Psychol. 2000;14(4):315–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Tenenbaum G, Stewart E, Singer RN, Duda J. Aggression and violence in sport: an ISSP position stand. Sport Psychol. 1997;11:1–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Björkqvist K, Österman K, Kaukiainen A. Social intelligence − empathy = aggression? Aggress Violent Behav. 2000;5(2):191–200.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Jolliffe D, Farrington DP. Empathy and offending: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Aggress Violent Beh. 2004;9(5):441–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Stanger N, Kavussanu M, McIntyre D, Ring C. Empathy inhibits aggression in competition: the role of provocation, emotion, and gender. J Sport Exerc Psychol. 2016;38(1):4–14.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Yeo LS, Ang RP, Loh S, Fu KJ, Karre JK. The role of affective and cognitive empathy in physical, verbal, and indirect aggression of a Singaporean sample of boys. J Psychol. 2011;145(4):313–30.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Blair RJR. Responding to the emotions of others: dissociating forms of empathy through the study of typical and psychiatric populations. Conscious Cogn. 2005;14(4):698–718.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Eres R, Decety J, Louis WR, Molenberghs P. Individual differences in local gray matter density are associated with differences in affective and cognitive empathy. Neuroimage. 2015;117:305–10.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Kaukiainen A, Björkqvist K, Lagerspetz K, Österman K, Salmivalli C, Rothberg S, et al. The relationships between social intelligence, empathy, and three types of aggression. Aggress Behav Off J Int Soc Res Aggress. 1999;25(2):81–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Bayram AB, Holmes M. Feeling their pain: affective empathy and public preferences for foreign development aid. Eur J Int Rel. 2020;26(3):820–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Topcu Ç, Erdur-Baker Ö. Affective and cognitive empathy as mediators of gender differences in cyber and traditional bullying. Sch Psychol Int. 2012;33(5):550–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Ang RP, Goh DH. Cyberbullying among adolescents: the role of affective and cognitive empathy, and gender. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2010;41(4):387–97.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Feshbach ND, Feshbach S. The relationship between empathy and aggression in two age groups. Dev Psychol. 1969;1(2):102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Richardson DR, Hammock GS, Smith SM, Gardner W, Signo M. Empathy as a cognitive inhibitor of interpersonal aggression. Aggress Behav. 1994;20(4):275–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Mercado-Crespo MC, Mbah AK. Race and ethnicity, substance use, and physical aggression among US high school students. J Interpers Violence. 2013;28(7):1367–84.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Oropesa RS. Normative beliefs about marriage and cohabitation: a comparison of non-Latino Whites, Mexican Americans, and Puerto Ricans. J Marriage Fam. 1996:49–62.

  38. Ang RP, Li X, Seah SL. The role of normative beliefs about aggression in the relationship between empathy and cyberbullying. J Cross Cult Psychol. 2017;48(8):1138–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Davis MH. Interpersonal reactivity index. Edwin Mellen Press; 1980.

    Google Scholar 

  40. Davis MH. A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy. 1980.

  41. Hengartner MP, Ajdacic-Gross V, Rodgers S, Müller M, Haker H, Rössler W. Fluid intelligence and empathy in association with personality disorder trait-scores: exploring the link. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2014;264(5):441–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Yaghoubi Jami P, Wind SA. Evaluating the psychometric properties of a proposed farsi version of the interpersonal reactivity index using item response theory. Res Soc Work Pract. 2022:10497315221089322.

  43. Raine A, Dodge K, Loeber R, Gatzke-Kopp L, Lynam D, Reynolds C, et al. The reactive–proactive aggression questionnaire: differential correlates of reactive and proactive aggression in adolescent boys. Aggress Behav Off J Int Soc Res Aggress. 2006;32(2):159–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Rezaei A, Dehghani M. Measurement invariance and psychometric properties of the reactive and proactive aggression questionnaire (RPQ) across genders. 2021.

  45. Reazei A, Dehghani M, Heidari M. Prediction of reactive and proactive aggression among adolescents based on the perceived parenting styles.

  46. Mohammadi N. Primary survey of psychometric index of Buss-Perry questionnaire. Farsi J Hum Soc Sci Shiraz Univ Iran. 2006;25(4):135–51.

    Google Scholar 

  47. Buss AH, Perry M. The aggression questionnaire. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1992;63(3):452.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Samani S. Study of reliability and validity of the Buss and Perry’s aggression questionnaire. 2008.

  49. Schober P, Boer C, Schwarte LA. Correlation coefficients: appropriate use and interpretation. Anesth Analg. 2018;126(5):1763–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang A-G, Buchner A. G* Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods. 2007;39(2):175–91.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Buchner A, Lang A-G. Statistical power analyses using G* Power 3.1: tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav Res Methods. 2009;41(4):1149–60.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Smith A. Cognitive empathy and emotional empathy in human behavior and evolution. Psychol Rec. 2006;56(1):3–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Jolliffe D, Farrington DP. Examining the relationship between low empathy and bullying. Aggress Behav Off J Int Soc Res Aggress. 2006;32(6):540–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Day A, Howells K, Mohr P, Schall E, Gerace A. The development of CBT programmes for anger: the role of interventions to promote perspective-taking skills. Behav Cogn Psychother. 2008;36(3):299–312.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Giancola PR, Parrott DJ. Further evidence for the validity of the Taylor aggression paradigm. Aggress Behav Off J Int Soc Res Aggress. 2008;34(2):214–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Kim H, Han S. Does personal distress enhance empathic interaction or block it? Pers Individ Differ. 2018;124:77–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Bloom P. Empathy and its discontents. Trends Cogn Sci. 2017;21(1):24–31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Maciantowicz O, Zajenkowski M. Is narcissistic anger fueled by neuroticism? The relationship between grandiose and vulnerable narcissism, neuroticism, and trait anger. Curr Psychol. 2020;39(5):1674–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

N/A.

Funding

This study was funded by Neuroscience Institute, Tehran University of Medical Sciences.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

The study was designed by AHM & EM. EM & MR were involved in data gathering. ANA performed data analytics and EM designed the tables. AHM supervised the research procedure. AHM, EM, ANA & MR are involved in drafting and proofreading the manuscript. All authors read and approved the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Amir Hossein Memari.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The study protocol was according to the declaration of Helsinki. The ethical committee of Tehran University of Medical Sciences approved the study protocol (code: IR.TUMS.NI.REC.1399.056). We sent the questionnaires to 700 athletes after they gave informed consent to participate in the study, and we asked them to complete the questionnaires independently.

Consent for publication

N/A.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Mahmoudi, E., Nakhostin-Ansari, A., Ranjbar, M. et al. Are all types of empathy associated with lower aggression in athletes? A cross-sectional study on Iranian athletes. BMC Psychol 10, 276 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-022-00985-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-022-00985-4

Keywords