Skip to main content

Goal management training improves executive control in adults with ADHD: an open trial employing attention network theory to examine effects on attention

Abstract

Background

Adults with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) typically experience poorer attentional control. According to the attention network theory, attentional control relies on three interacting networks of alerting, orienting, and executive control. In ADHD, it is mainly the alerting and executive control networks that are suggested and found to be compromised.

Methods

In the current study, we investigated if a group-based metacognitive remediation program (Goal Management Training [GMT]) in adults with ADHD would enhance attentional control using an experimental measure of the attention network theory. We expected that GMT would specifically enhance the executive control and alerting networks.

Results

Data from post- and follow up-assessments of 21 adults (age: 39.05 [11.93]) with ADHD who had completed GMT were included. Linear mixed-effects modeling revealed significant improvements in the functioning of the executive control network for the majority of the participants, although a small subset of participants showed a negative development following the intervention. Results also showed an improvement in the orienting network at follow up, but no change in the alerting network.

Conclusion

The results may indicate that improvements in the functioning of the executive control network are central to the positive effects of GMT reported in disorders characterized by impaired attentional control.

Trial registration: The study was retrospectively registered in the ISRCTN (Identifier: ISRCTN91988877) on the 18/01/2021.

Peer Review reports

Introduction

Adults with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) struggle with elevated symptoms of inattention and/or hyperactivity/impulsivity in their everyday life [1]. Typically, this is reflected in a poorer ability to voluntarily focus their attention on the task at hand and maintain this attentional focus over time to aid goal-directed behavior [2,3,4,5,6]. As a result, they often experience academic [7,8,9,10,11] and occupational [9, 12,13,14] impairments. In ADHD, pharmacological treatment has been shown to reduce alterations in brain activity and structures in brain networks involved in attentional control [6]. However, pharmacological therapy has limited efficacy and not all individuals with ADHD tolerate such treatment [15]. Efforts have been made to investigate if it is possible to train attentional control and thereby enhance attention functions. Still, there is limited evidence for the effectiveness of non-pharmacological interventions for ADHD [15] including interventions focusing on attentional control [16]. The aim of the current study was therefore to investigate the effectiveness of a group-based metacognitive remediation program, Goal Management Training (GMT; [17, 18], in enhancing attentional control in adults with ADHD.

GMT is a group-based metacognitive remediation program aimed at reducing deficiencies of goal management, based on Duncan’s [19] attentional control theory of goal neglect [17, 18]. The theory describes how goal management fails when an individual is unable to maintain task focus towards future goals due to being unable to cope with the competing demands of other salient stimuli or ongoing activities. The program, therefore, emphasizes teaching participants a five-stage strategy to increase goal attainment. The intervention builds on the assumption that participants, through practice, will be able to improve sustained attention via changes in the brain networks underlying this function and that this will allow for improved executive control [20]. Important components include the intermittent stopping of ongoing behavior (“STOP!-and-think”) to orient towards relevant goals and to evaluate whether ongoing behavior is in line with these. The program also contains components of mindfulness training [21] meant to help participants develop the skills needed to maintain a focus on the present, as well as the active use of self-cueing to regulate alertness to maintain executive control [22]). These strategies and techniques are combined with the aim of improving awareness of attentional lapses to reduce the negative influence of poorer executive control. Thus, GMT can be said to target executive control and attention allocation [20].

GMT has been shown to have positive effects on measures of executive control and attention in several patient groups [23, 24]. However, to the best of our knowledge, only two earlier, small-scale pilot studies have investigated the effects of GMT in adults with ADHD [25, 26]. In de Braek et al. reported positive effects of GMT compared to psychoeducation with regards to clinician-rated, everyday cognitive functioning. However, they only included a general performance-based measure of problem solving, and no performance-based measures that specifically tap into executive control and attention allocation. Jensen et al., studying the same adult ADHD sample as in the current study, reported significant improvements on measures of executive control, such as on the Stroop test and the Tower test following GMT. However, these measures do not allow for differentiation between executive control and attention allocation of alertness and orienting attention [26]. Applying a systems-neuroscience approach for measuring effects of GMT would improve the understanding of which specific attention function(s) GMT improves in adults with ADHD.

The attention network theory [27,28,29] offers such a systems-neuroscience approach, defining three interacting networks of early-operating attention allocation of alerting and orienting, as well as executive control [27, 29,30,31]. The executive control network is involved in complex operations, such as detecting and resolving conflicts between stimuli, for instance in inhibiting salient stimuli to be able to attend to a task-based target stimulus. To facilitate executive control, the alerting network supports preparation for shifting from rest to task-based activity and to maintain effort over time (alertness). The orienting network, on the other hand, is involved in the selection of which stimuli to attend among multiple stimuli, such as the ability to quickly focus attention on task-based stimuli. ANT has not previously been applied in studies of GMT [23]. However, Berger and Posner [30] hypothesized that individuals with ADHD would show poorer functioning of the attention networks of executive control and alerting but not the orienting network. This has been supported in studies of children [32,33,34,35,36] and adults with ADHD [37], although there are also contradictory findings from studies of children [39,40,41]. So far, the use of the attention network theory to examine treatment effects in studies of adults with ADHD is limited. One study by Dotare et al. [42] reported positive effects of a computer-based cognitive remediation training program on executive control and not on the alerting or orienting networks. This is in line with findings showing that children without ADHD experienced positive effects of a tailored computer-based training program [43] specifically on the executive control network, and that children with the poorest attentional control at baseline were most likely to benefit from such interventions. These findings are also comparable to results from studies in which the attention network theory has been applied to test treatment effects in non-ADHD adult samples, such as effects of mindfulness training on attention. In mindfulness training, positive effects have been observed on executive control in addition to the orienting network, whereas positive effects on alerting have only been observed in experienced meditators/mindfulness practitioners [44, 45].

In the present study, we investigated the effects of GMT [17, 18, 46] on the three attention networks as described in the attention network theory [27], using the revised Attention Network Test (ANT-R; 47) in an open trial in which adults with ADHD participated. Since no prior study has tested the effects of GMT with ANT-R, we build on the theory of GMT and previous findings in ADHD samples using the original Attention Network Test [48] to develop the following hypotheses:

We expected (1) that adults with ADHD would show improvements in the executive control and alerting networks following GMT and not on the orienting network. (2) To find the most consistent change in the executive control network since goal achievement through efficient conflict detection is emphasized as a main mechanism in GMT [20]. In addition to the traditional focus on averaged group scores to measure effects of GMT, we also analyzed change scores for every participant to better understand individual differences in effects of GMT.

Methods

Participants

Participants in the current study were part of an exploratory investigation of GMT as an intervention for adults with ADHD recruited from local outpatient clinics in the municipality of Bergen, Norway (see 26 for further details). Inclusion criteria were an age > 18 years and a confirmed clinical diagnosis of ADHD. The participants had been diagnosed by a specialist (i.e., clinical psychologist or psychiatrist) outside of the project according to the current Norwegian guidelines [49] which employ diagnostic codes from the ICD-10 [50] but allow for the use of DSM-IV/5 criteria [1, 51] in the diagnostic assessment. These guidelines describe the necessity of assessing developmental history, current symptoms and effects on functioning across multiple domains, and an assessment of physical or psychiatric illnesses that may explain the symptoms. The guidelines recommend the use of the Diagnostic Interview for Adult ADHD, second edition (DIVA 2.0), the Mini International Neuropsychological Interview (M.I.N.I. Plus) and the Structured Clinical Interview II (SCID-II) for DSM-IV axis I and axis II disorders, respectively [52,53,54], in addition to self-report forms such as the Adult ADHD Self-report Scale (ASRS; [55]) and the Wender-Utah Rating scale (WURS; [56]). These guidelines also emphasize the need to collect collateral reports (e.g., the use of DIVA with parents/older siblings and/or long-term partner/spouse). Exact data on adherence to these guidelines was not available for inclusion in this study. Exclusion criteria in our study sample were a full-scale intelligence quotient below 80, a history of psychotic disorder, or other severe ongoing psychiatric disorders such as severe depression or acute suicidality, which would prohibit participation in the intervention study. In total, 34 participants volunteered for the study of whom 21 participants completed two or more assessment points and were included in the present study. Participants’ age ranged from 21 to 62 years (M = 39.05, SD = 11.93, 57% males). A total of 13 of these 21 participants (61.9%) fulfilled the criteria for at least one other ongoing disorder according to the M.I.N.I. Plus [54]. Various anxiety disorders constituted the majority of these disorders (Table 1). For further details on recruitment and clinical characterstics of the sample see Table 1 and Jensen et al. [26].

Table 1 Descriptive statistics of the sample and included variables

Procedure

Diagnostic assessment of comorbid disorders, self-reports of, among others, ADHD symptoms, and assessments with a neuropsychological test battery and electrocardiogram (ECG) were conducted at the neuropsychological outpatient clinic at the University of Bergen. As participants had an existing ADHD-diagnosis, only self-reports of current symptoms where collected. For further details see Jensen et al. [26]. Participants completed the ANT-R in a soundproof room approximately midway through the assessment and immediately following a break at all three time points.

GMT was administered in nine two-hour group sessions in groups of four to eight participants led by a clinical psychologist (six years of studies at the university level) and a co-therapist who was either a clinical psychologist or a clinical psychology student with clinical experience (i.e., students who had completed at least four and a half of the six years of study and had experience from clinical work as psychologists under supervision). Guidance from a clinical psychologist with extensive experience with GMT was also available. Due to various holidays the duration of the intervention varied between nine and 11 weeks. Participants were asked to complete daily homework assignments between sessions and their experiences with these were discussed in the following session. Participants had to complete a minimum of six out of nine sessions to be included in the post- and follow up-assessments.

Assessments were conducted within three weeks before the first session of the GMT-intervention and within two weeks after the last session. Follow up-assessments were conducted six months after completion of the intervention (± two weeks).

Measurements

Attention network test—revised

The ANT-R, developed by Fan et al. [47], is a revision of the original Attention Network Test [48], introducing new elements that allow for the investigation of interactions between the three constituent attentional networks. In the ANT-R the presentation of the flanker condition is presented at one of two locations on a computer screen and these locations can be either congruent or incongruent. Furthermore, the ANT-R includes three cue conditions: no cue, double cue (alerting cues) and spatial cue (orienting cues). In the current study we included the reaction time scores for alerting, orienting and executive control (see Table 2).

Table 2 Descriptive information about the ANT-r scores

During performance of the ANT-R, the participants are seated in front of a computer. They are instructed to attend to a fixation crosshair at the center of the screen and informed that a set of five arrows will appear inside one of two boxes which are placed to the left and right of this crosshair. Their task is to indicate the direction of the center arrow as quickly and accurately as possible by pressing the left mouse key with their right index finger if the arrow is facing left, and the right mouse key with their right middle finger if the arrow is facing right. The flanker condition is defined as congruent if the center arrow is facing in the same direction as the surrounding flanker arrows and incongruent if the direction of the flanker arrows is opposite of the direction of the target arrow (e.g., center arrow facing right, flanker arrows facing left). Each trial consists of the presentation of the flanker arrows for 500 ms. This is preceded by the presentation of the varying cue conditions for 100 ms, followed by a cue-target interval of either 0, 400 or 800 ms. During the 100 ms in which the cue is presented the participants receive either no cue, a valid (i.e., the box surrounding the location where the flanker will appear flashes) or invalid (i.e., the box surrounding the location where the flanker will not appear flashes) spatial cue, or a temporal cue (both boxes flash). The trials are separated by a fixation period lasting between 2000 and 12,000 ms. Before beginning the task, participants completed a practice round where they were given step-by-step instructions pertaining to the cue and target conditions as well as 32 practice trials demonstrating the task in the same way as during the actual ANT-R procedure. In total (excluding the practice trials), the ANT-R consist of four blocks, each containing 72 trials (see 47 for further details). The task was performed on a desktop PC, using E-Prime™ software (Psychology Software Tools, Pittsburgh, PA).

Adult ADHD self-report scale

The ASRS [55] is an 18-item scale where participants are instructed to evaluate the presence and frequency of ADHD-symptoms over the past six months on a Likert scale ranging from never (0) to very often [4]. The scale showed acceptable internal consistency with a Cronbach’s α of 0.87. In the present study, this scale was used to describe the symptom severity of the included sample of adults with ADHD at pre-, post- and follow-up sessions.

Mini international neuropsychiatric interview plus

The M.I.N.I. Plus [54] was administered to assess participants for eligibility for participation as well as to assess the presence of other psychiatric disorders. The interview was administered by an experienced clinical psychologist or by a clinical psychology student with clinical experience—who was supervised by an experienced clinical psychologist.

Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence

Participants completed two subtests from the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (Matrix reasoning and Vocabulary; [58]) to obtain an estimate of their intelligence quotient.

Statistical analyses

All statistical analyses were conducted using R version 4.0.2 [59] and packages lme4, lmerTest, ggplot2, tidyverse and SIMR [60,61,62,63,64]. Linear mixed-effect modeling was used to examine changes in ANT-R scores within individuals over time. Separate models were built for the three dependent variables alerting, orienting and executive control. The baseline models included a random intercept and slope nested within participants. Random intercept and slope for the effects of session were then examined to investigate if this led to improved fit. The covariates of age, sex, IQ and medication use due to ADHD were included as fixed effects and backwards tested. Model fit was assessed by likelihood-ratio tests and covariates were retained if such tests indicated a significant improvement in model fit (See Additional file 1: Table S1). After inclusion of covariates the moderating effects of each predictor on the change in ANT-R measures were examined by adding the interaction between each of these predictors and assessment number to the model. Effect size estimates were calculated using the procedure described by Westfall et al. [65], resulting in estimates approximating Cohen’s d [66]. Post-hoc power analyses were conducted on the final models using Monte Carlo simulations as implemented in SIMR. Lastly, results were inspected visually to examine individual patterns of change on the ANT-R score(s) that were shown to be enhanced following GMT. The clinical characteristics of participants showing a negative effect of GMT on ANT-R were further analyzed by inspecting their ASRS scores, medication status, IQ, and comorbid disorders (see Fig. 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Individual- and group values for the flanker conflict effect (in milliseconds) at each of the three assessments. The bold line depicts changes in group mean across the three assessments, while the regular lines show the development for individual participants

Results

Data preparation

Reaction time data from trials with correct answers were used in the analyses. These were inspected and responses below 100 ms were removed (N = 1). Visual inspection indicated that the reaction times (RTs) were not normally distributed. RTs were therefore transformed using an inverse gaussian transformation followed by a multiplication of -1000 as described by Baayen and Milin [67] to approximate a normal distribution. These transformed RTs were then used to calculate measures for the flanker conflict, alerting, and validity effects in line with the descriptions of Fan et al. [47]. After calculation of these measures the data were once again inspected for outliers. For the flanker conflict two extreme trials out of a total of 8167 were removed to ascertain a normal distribution of the data. For the alerting and validity effects extreme values resulted in the removal of eight out of 2771 and one out of 2740 trials, respectively. Furthermore, continuous covariates were centered using grand mean centering. Table 1 shows an overview of the baseline characteristics of the sample as well as information about ANT-R effects and changes in ASRS-scores.

Linear mixed-effect models

In line with our hypotheses we investigated the changes in the executive control, alerting, and orienting networks. We expected reductions in the flanker conflict and alerting effects across the assessments. Furthermore, we investigated the orienting effect to assess whether participants showed an improvement in the use of spatial cueing.

Executive control

Following the model fitting approach described under statistical analyses, the final linear-mixed effect model included a random slope and intercept for each individual participant as an effect of assessment-session. Furthermore, the model included fixed effects for session, age and medication status as well as interaction terms for the effects of age by session and medication status by session (AIC 1743.2, see Additional file 1: Table S1 for further information). Monte Carlo simulations based on 1000 repetitions using the final model and an α of 0.05 indicated a power of 78.70% (95% CI: LL = 75.03, UL = 81.20) for the predictor session.

The results showed a significant fixed effect of assessment-session with reductions in the flanker conflict-effect from baseline to post- and follow up-assessments. The most substantial change occurred between baseline- and post-assessments with a small and non-significant increase from post- to follow up-assessment (β = -0.003, SD = 0.021, 95% CI = − 0.044 – 0.037, δt = − 0.011, p = 0.9). The results also showed a significant random effect of assessment-session. Furthermore, results showed negative relationships between the use of medication and the flanker conflict-effect and between age and the flanker conflict effect (See Table 3). The strength of the prediction of the observed values at each assessment-session is visualized in the Additional file 2 : Fig. S1.

Table 3 Summary of a linear mixed-effects model of the Flanker Conflict-effect

Alerting

Results indicated no significant change from baseline to the post- and follow-up assessments (p > 0.25), and the model fit was not significantly improved by adding the covariates (all p- values > 0.85 compared to the model which only included the fixed effects of session and random effect of participants), as such the original model without covariates was used. For further details see Table 4). Monte Carlo simulations based on 1000 repetitions using the final model and an α of 0.05 indicated a power of 17.90% (95% CI: LL = 15.57, UL = 20.42) for the predictor session.

Table 4 Summary of components in a mixed-effects model of the Alerting-effect

Orienting

There was no significant change from baseline to post-assessment, but a significant effect on the orienting effect from baseline to follow-up, after controlling for the random variance within individual participants (p < 0.01. This was also reflected in a significant change between the post and follow up-assessments (β = 0.040, SD = 0.015, 95% CI = 0.012 – 0.069, δt = 0.203, p < 0.01). For further details see Table 5. Monte Carlo simulations based on 1000 repetitions using the final model and an α of 0.05 indicated a power of 45.00% (95% CI: LL = 41.89, UL = 48.14) for the predictor session.

Table 5 Summary of components in a mixed-effects model of the orienting-effect

Visual inspection of individual changes of flanker scores from pre- to follow-up assessments

As can be seen in Fig. 1, the majority of participants showed a decrease in the flanker conflict-effect from baseline to the post-assessment. However, there were relatively large individual differences in change scores (Range = [− 98.6 – 79.7 ms]) and a total of four participants showed increases in the flanker conflict-effect from baseline to post-assessment (Range = [2.6 – 79.7 ms]). Similarly, five participants showed an increase in the flanker conflict-effect from baseline to follow up-assessment (Range = [3.3 – 116.1 ms]). Three of these participants showed an increase from baseline at both timepoints. An inspection showed that all three were men, that two out of the three were older than the average of the sample (51 and 52 years, the third was 33 years), and that the first two had an IQ below the average level of the sample (93 and 103, the third had an IQ of 131). The first two of these participants used medication for ADHD, whereas the last participant did not use such medication. ASRS-scores at baseline were close to the average of the sample (46, 43 and 45, respectively) and increased in parallel with increases in flanker conflict effects for the first two participants at post-assessment (46 to 47 and 43 to 48, respectively) but were greatly reduced for the last participant (45 to 28) despite an increase in the flanker conflict effect. At follow-up all three participants reported changes in ASRS-scores in the same direction as the direction of the flanker conflict-effect relative to post-assessment scores (i.e., the first participant reported an increase [47 to 50], while the last two participants reported reductions [48 to 40, 28 to 26]). The first participant reported ongoing anxiety disorders (panic- and social anxiety disorder) and body dysmorphic disorder, whereas the other two screened positive for antisocial personality disorder.

Discussion

In the current open trial, we investigated the effects of GMT on executive control and attention allocation as defined by the attention network theory (29) in adults with ADHD. The results supported our expectation that improvements would be found in the executive control network following GMT. However, against our á priori expectation we found no significant changes following GMT on the alerting scores but rather a change in orienting attention scores. The positive change in executive control was detectable immediately after the intervention ended, whereas the change in orienting attention appeared on the follow-up assessment six months later.

The current findings showing improvements in executive control following GMT are in line with previous studies showing that this network is malleable to improvement after non-pharmacological interventions in adults with [42] and without ADHD [45]. The findings are also in accordance with earlier investigations of GMT for other disorders showing a reduction in errors on various neuropsychological measures which include an aspect of response ambiguity [24, 46]. This finding may therefore indicate that changes in goal-management following GMT in adults with ADHD are related to an enhanced ability to handle and detect competing stimuli and thought processes. As such, the current results complement the previous study by Jensen et al. [26] showing specific effects of GMT on executive control in adults with ADHD. Using the ANT-R allowed us to provide support for the notion that this effect on executive control was not driven by improved attention allocation, but a specific effect on conflict detection itself. This demonstrates the advantage of applying experimental paradigms to assess effects of treatment by allowing for investigations of potential mechanisms of change. Another advantage is that the attention network theory is based on a defined neuroscience model of the brain. This may provide indications regarding which brain mechanisms are involved in for instance improvement of executive control following GMT [23, 25, 26]. The executive control network has been shown to rely on frontal brain regions in several fMRI studies [68,69,70]. Among the regions involved in the executive control network are the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and insula, regions that are also part of the salience network and involved in conflict monitoring and upregulation of cognitive control in response to uncertainty or error [71, 72]. In accordance with the assumptions underlying GMT, a possible explanation would therefore be that GMT acts through changes in brain networks involved in executive control and orienting/alertness [20]. Importantly, activation in the ACC and insula has been shown to differentiate between individuals with ADHD and non-ADHD controls during performance of cognitive inhibition tasks [73]. Improvements in the connectivity of the ACC has also been shown to be associated with symptom remittance among adolescents with ADHD [74]. As findings from studies of various populations, including ADHD, indicate that both short-term mindfulness interventions [75,76,77,78] and cognitive rehabilitation interventions [79, 80] may improve the functional connectivity of the ACC and/or insula, this may also be a possible mechanism involved in our findings. Future studies may delineate the importance of these components for functional changes following GMT.

The adults with ADHD did not show the expected improvement in alertness after GMT. Rather, and in contrast to our expectations, they showed significant changes in results on the measure of the orienting network at follow up. Interestingly, these findings are in accordance with previous studies of mindfulness training showing effects on executive control and orienting after shorter periods of training [44, 45]. It is important to note that the attention networks are believed both to interact and to operate independently from each other. This is in line with the view emphasizing the importance of also studying supportive processes such as attention allocation in order to improve the understanding of conflict detection [81]. For instance, there may be an overlap of functioning between the orienting attention of ANT-R when defined by validity of cues and executive control since they rely on some of the same brain regions (i.e., ACC and insula; 47). This was supported in a study by Trautwein et al. [70] in that invalidly cued targets induced activation in the ACC and insula. We have also found in a previous study that the interaction between early updating of stimuli (e.g., orienting attention) and conflict detection (e.g., executive control) associated with higher flexibility of the autonomous nervous system (i.e., heart rate variability; 82). Similarly, being alert to when stimuli appear on the screen may also affect an alertness to where the same stimuli appear (see 28). An improvement in the orienting attention and/or the executive control network may therefore reflect higher alertness following GMT despite this not being overtly observed through higher alerting scores following GMT.

In addition to the traditional focus on treatment effects on a group level, it is of clinical relevance to look at individual change scores to investigate if some participants may have a negative effect of GMT. Our sample was too small to statistically test differences between the three men who, on visual inspection of the flanker conflict score (i.e., executive control network score), appeared to have a negative effect by being less efficient on both assessments after GMT compared to baseline. However, by looking at other variables/scores of clinical interest from the assessments of these three male participants, we observed that with the exception of sex and that two out of three had a comorbid antisocial personality disorder (according to the M.I.N.I. Plus), there did not appear to be any specific variable included in our study in which these participants systematically differed from the rest of the sample and that could explain the differential effect of GMT on ANT-R. However, of interest, their negative change on the flanker conflict score for the most part mirrored changes in ADHD symptom reports on ASRS after GMT. It is challenging to delineate if this negative development was due to lack of effect of GMT or if something else occurred in these participants’ lives that prevented them from engaging in the remediation practices of GMT. Few studies show such individual effect scores of GMT or of other types of treatment. This can be an important way to improve the understanding of which participants experience both positive and negative effects, and thereby improve individually targeted treatment in patient groups.

Strengths, limitations, and future directions

We believe that the current results support the use of attention network theory and the ANT-R in future studies of interventions for ADHD and in investigations of GMT, and that such studies may provide important insights into potential mechanisms involved in reducing symptoms and impairments associated with the disorder being studied. Furthermore, the recruitment of a clinically-based sample of adults with ADHD probably increased the transferability of the results to patients with ADHD seeking treatment in the health care system. Also, the description of individual trajectories and the attempts to characterize individual differences in effects of the intervention can be an inspiration for adapting the same approach in future treatment studies.

There are several limitations to the current study. First of all, due to the lack of a control group, practice effects cannot be ruled out. We do, however, believe that the differential effects on the separate networks and the differing timing of effects speak against such an interpretation. Investigations of practice effects in Flanker-tasks also seem to indicate that these are relatively small and quite equal between congruent and incongruent trials [83, 84]. Secondly, the sample size was small and power analyses only indicate that the model investigating executive control achieved statistical power close to what is satisfactory. The sample size also did not allow for statistical power to perform between-group analyses to explore the influence of factors such as medication status and ADHD-subtype. Earlier findings indicate that these factors may influence RTs on the ANT-R (e.g., [42]) and we found an indication in the current study of a negative relationship between medication use and the flanker effect. Related to this, the fact that analyses were limited to participants who completed two or more assessments may have influenced the finding. As the current sample consisted of adults with ADHD that were mainly recruited from local outpatient clinics, many of the participants also had comorbid disorders, mainly anxiety disorders. It is therefore possible that the current results may also reflect effects not directly related to ADHD but to concurrent anxiety disorders or other comorbid conditions. Finally, the current sample attained above average estimates of full scale-IQ. As such, it is possible that the reported results and/or the attained effect of the intervention may not be generalizable to samples with lower intelligence.

We argue that the current results indicate that there is reason to believe that GMT may be an advantageous intervention for adults with ADHD and that this should be evaluated in a larger randomized trial. Furthermore, we believe that inclusion of brain imaging techniques in such a trial could clarify the current results and inform the validity of the possible explanations discussed above.

Conclusion

The current open trial found an improvement in one aspect of attention, namely executive control, following GMT in a sample of adults with ADHD. Our finding thus suggests that GMT may be an efficient intervention for ADHD and further, that improvements in executive control may be a potential mechanism of change for adults with ADHD using this training procedure. A possible brain-correlated mechanism explaining this improvement may be enhanced functioning or connectivity in the ACC and/or insula following the intervention. Future randomized controlled studies of GMT are encouraged to include the ANT-R in combination with brain imaging techniques such as fMRI to increase the neurobiological understanding of effects of GMT in ADHD and other patient groups.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets generated and analyzed during the current study are not publicly available due to limitations in the ethical approval for the study but are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

ACC::

Anterior cingulate cortex

ADHD::

Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder

ANT::

Attention network test

ANT-R::

Attention network test-revised

ASRS::

Adult ADHD self-report scale

DSM-IV::

Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, 4th edition

DSM-5: :

Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, 5th edition

ECG: :

Electrocardiogram

GMT: :

Goal management training

ICD-10: :

International classification of diseases

M.I.N.I. Plus: :

Mini international neuropsychiatric interview plus

RT: :

Reaction time

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 5th ed. Washington, D.C.: American Psychiatric Association; 2013. XLIV, 947 s.

  2. Barkley RA. Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: Constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychol Bull. 1997;121(1):65–94.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Bush G. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and attention networks. Neuropsychopharmacology [Internet]. 2010;35(1):278–300.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Faraone SV, Asherson P, Banaschewski T, Biederman J, Buitelaar JK, Ramos-Quiroga JA, et al. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Nat Rev Dis Prim. 2015;1:15020.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Sonuga-Barke EJS, Bitsakou P, Thompson M. Beyond the dual pathway model: evidence for the dissociation of timing, inhibitory, and delay-related impairments in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry [Internet]. 2010;49(4):345–55.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Spencer TJ, Brown A, Seidman LJ, Valera EM, Makris N, Lomedico A, et al. Effect of psychostimulants on brain structure and function in ADHD: a qualitative literature review of magnetic resonance imaging-based neuroimaging studies. J Clin Psychiatry. 2013;74(9):902–17.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Barry TD, Lyman RD, Klinger LG. Academic underachievement and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: the negative impact of symptom severity on school performance. J Sch Psychol [Internet]. 2002;40(3):259–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Biederman J, Monuteaux MC, Doyle AE, Seidman LJ, Wilens TE, Ferrero F, et al. Impact of executive function deficits and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) on academic outcomes in children. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2004;72(5):757–66.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Biederman J, Petty CR, Fried R, Fontanella J, Doyle AE, Seidman LJ, et al. Impact of psychometrically defined deficits of executive functioning in adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am J Psychiatry [Internet]. 2006;163(10):1730–8. https://doi.org/10.1176/ajp.2006.163.10.1730.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Mannuzza S, Klein RG. Long-term prognosis in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am [Internet]. 2000;9(3):711–26.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Simon V, Czobor P, Sára B, Mészáros Á, Bitter I. Prevalence and correlates of adult attention-deficithyperactivity disorder: meta-analysis. Br J Psychiatry [Internet]. 2009;194(3):204–11.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Halleland HB, Sørensen L, Posserud M-B, Haavik J, Lundervold AJ. Occupational status is compromised in adults with ADHD and psychometrically defined executive function deficits. J Attention Disord. 2015;23(1):76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Halmøy A, Fasmer OB, Gillberg C, Haavik J. Occupational outcome in adult ADHD: impact of symptom profile, comorbid psychiatric problems, and treatment: a cross-sectional study of 414 clinically diagnosed adult ADHD patients. J Atten Disord. 2009;13(2):175–87.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Kessler RC, Adler L, Ames M, Barkley RA, Birnbaum H, Greenberg P, et al. The prevalence and effects of adult attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder on work performance in a nationally representative sample of workers. J Occup Environ Med. 2005;47(6):565–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. De Crescenzo F, Cortese S, Adamo N, Janiri L. Pharmacological and non-pharmacological treatment of adults with ADHD: a meta-review. Evid Based Ment Health. 2017;20(1):4–11.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Nimmo-Smith V, Merwood A, Hank D, Brandling J, Greenwood R, Skinner L, et al. Non-pharmacological interventions for adult ADHD: a systematic review. Psychol Med. 2020;50(4):529–41.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Levine B, Robertson IH, Clare L, Carter G, Hong J, Wilson BA, et al. Rehabilitation of executive functioning: an experimental–clinical validation of goal management training. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2000;6(3):299–312.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Robertson IH. Goal management training: a clinical manual. Cambridge: PsyConsult; 1996.

    Google Scholar 

  19. Duncan J. Disorganisation of behaviour after frontal lobe damage. Cogn Neuropsychol [Internet]. 1986;3(3):271–90. https://doi.org/10.1080/02643298608253360.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Robertson IH, Levine B. Attention and arousal in neurorehabilitation. Princ Front lobe Funct. 2013. https://doi.org/10.1093/med/9780199837755.003.0053.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Kabat-Zinn J. Full catastrophe living. New York: Dell Publishing; 1990.

    Google Scholar 

  22. Robertson IH, Tegnér R, Tham K, Lo A, Nimmo-Smith I. Sustained attention training for unilateral neglect: theoretical and rehabilitation implications. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1995;17(3):416–30.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Stamenova V, Levine B. Effectiveness of goal management training® in improving executive functions: a meta-analysis. Neuropsychol Rehabil [Internet]. 2019;29(10):1569–99. https://doi.org/10.1080/09602011.2018.1438294.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Tornås S, Løvstad M, Solbakk AK, Evans J, Endestad T, Hol PK, et al. Rehabilitation of executive functions in patients with chronic acquired brain injury with goal management training, external cuing, and emotional regulation: a randomized controlled trial. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2016;22(4):436–52.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. In de Braek D, Dijkstra JB, Ponds RW, Jolles J. Goal management training in adults with ADHD: an intervention study. J Atten Disord. 2017;21(13):1130–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Jensen DA, Halmøy A, Stubberud J, Haavik J, Lundervold AJ, Sørensen L. An exploratory investigation of goal management training in adults with ADHD: improvements in inhibition and everyday functioning. Front Psychol [Internet]. 2021;https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.659480

  27. Petersen SE, Posner MI. The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2012;35:73–89.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Posner MI, Fan J. Attention as an organ system. In: Pomerantz JR, editor. Topics in integrative neuroscience: from cells to cognition [Internet]. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2008. p. 31–61.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  29. Posner MI, Petersen SE. The attention system of the human brain. Annu Rev Neurosci [Internet]. 1990;13(1):25–42. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ne.13.030190.000325.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Berger A, Posner MI. Pathologies of brain attentional networks. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2000;24(1):3–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Raz A, Buhle J. Typologies of attentional networks. Nat Rev Neurosci [Internet]. 2006;7(5):367–79.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Abramov DM, Cunha CQ, Galhanone PR, Alvim RJ, de Oliveira AM, Lazarev VV. Neurophysiological and behavioral correlates of alertness impairment and compensatory processes in ADHD evidenced by the Attention Network Test. PLoS ONE. 2019;14(7):e0219472.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Arora S, Lawrence MA, Klein RM. The attention network test database: ADHD and cross-cultural applications. Front Psychol [Internet]. 2020;https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00388

  34. Johnson KA, Robertson IH, Barry E, Mulligan A, Dáibhis A, Daly M, et al. Impaired conflict resolution and alerting in children with ADHD: evidence from the attention network task (ANT). J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2008;49(12):1339–47.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Mullane JC, Corkum PV, Klein RM, McLaughlin EN, Lawrence MA. Alerting, orienting, and executive attention in children with ADHD. J Atten Disord [Internet]. 2011;15(4):310–20. https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054710366384.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Waldon J, Vriend J, Davidson F, Corkum P. Sleep and attention in children with ADHD and typically developing peers. J Atten Disord. 2018;22(10):933–41.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Lundervold AJ, Adolfsdottir S, Halleland H, Halmoy A, Plessen K, Haavik J. Attention network test in adults with ADHD-the impact of affective fluctuations. Behav Brain Funct. 2011;7:27.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  38. Roberts M, Ashinoff BK, Castellanos FX, Carrasco M. When attention is intact in adults with ADHD. Psychon Bull Rev. 2018;25(4):1423–34.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Adólfsdóttir S, Sørensen L, Lundervold AJ. The attention network test: a characteristic pattern of deficits in children with ADHD. Behav Brain Funct. 2008;4:9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  40. Kooistra L, Crawford S, Gibbard B, Kaplan BJ, Fan J. Comparing attentional networks in fetal alcohol spectrum disorder and the inattentive and combined subtypes of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Dev Neuropsychol. 2011;36(5):566–77.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Mogg K, Salum GA, Bradley BP, Gadelha A, Pan P, Alvarenga P, et al. Attention network functioning in children with anxiety disorders, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and non-clinical anxiety. Psychol Med. 2015;45(12):2633–46.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  42. Dotare M, Bader M, Mesrobian SK, Asai Y, Villa AEP, Lintas A. Attention networks in ADHD adults after working memory training with a dual n-back task. Brain Sci [Internet]. 2020;10(10):715.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  43. Rueda MR, Rothbart MK, McCandliss BD, Saccomanno L, Posner MI. Training, maturation, and genetic influences on the development of executive attention. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A [Internet]. 2005;102(41):14931.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  44. Jha AP, Krompinger J, Baime MJ. Mindfulness training modifies subsystems of attention. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci [Internet]. 2007;7(2):109–19. https://doi.org/10.3758/CABN.7.2.109.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Verhaeghen P. Mindfulness as attention training: meta-analyses on the links between attention performance and mindfulness interventions long-term meditation practice and trait mindfulness. Mindfulness (N Y) [Internet]. 2021;12(3):564–581. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-020-01532-1.

  46. Levine B, Schweizer TA, O’Connor C, Tumer G, Gillingham S, Stuss DT, et al. Rehabilitation of executive functioning in patients with frontal lobe brain damage with goal management training. Front Hum Neurosci. 2011;5:1–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Fan J, Gu X, Guise KG, Liu X, Fossella J, Wang H, et al. Testing the behavioral interaction and integration of attentional networks. Brain Cogn. 2009;70:209–20.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. Fan J, McCandliss BD, Sommer T, Raz A, Posner MI. Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. J Cogn Neurosci. 2002;14(3):340–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Schillinger A, Bråtveit B, Hessen JO, Skram K, Øen K, Torgersen T, et al. National clinical guidelines for the assessment and treatment of ADHD in Norway (ADHD/hyperkinetisk forstyrrelse – nasjonal faglig retningslinje for utredning, behandling og oppfølging) [Internet]. Norway: Norwegian Health Department; 2014. Available from: https://www.helsedirektoratet.no/retningslinjer/adhd

  50. World Health Organization. The international classification of diseases, 10th revision. Geneva: World Health Organization; 1992.

    Google Scholar 

  51. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text rev.). Washington, D. C.: American Psychiatric Association; 2000.

  52. First MB, Gibbon M, Spitzer RL, Williams JBW, Benjamin LS. Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV® axis ii personality disorders SCID-II. Washington: American Psychiatric Pub; 1997.

    Google Scholar 

  53. Kooij JJS. Adult ADHD: Diagnostic assessment and treatment, 3rd ed. Adult ADHD: Diagnostic assessment and treatment, 3rd ed. New York: Springer-Verlag Publishing; 2013.

  54. Sheehan DV, Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, Amorim P, Janavs J, Weiller E, Hergueta T, Baker R, Dunbar GC. The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J Clin Psychiatry. 1998;59.

  55. Kessler RC, Adler L, Ames M, Demler O, Faraone SV, Hiripi E, et al. The world health organization adult ADHD self-report scale (ASRS): a short screening scale for use in the general population. Psychol Med. 2005;35(2):245–56.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Ward MF, Wender PH, Reimherr FW. The Wender Utah Rating Scale: an aid in the retrospective diagnosis of childhood attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 1993;150(6):885–90.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Sørensen L, Osnes B, Visted E, Svendsen JL, Adolfsdottir S, Binder P-E, et al. Dispositional mindfulness and attentional control: the specific association between the mindfulness facets of non-judgment and describing with flexibility of early operating orienting in conflict detection. Front Psychol [Internet]. 2018;https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02359.

  58. Wechsler D. Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence (WASI) manual. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; 1999.

    Google Scholar 

  59. R Development Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. [Internet]. Vienna, Austria: R foundation for statistical computing; 2020. Available from: https://www.r-project.org/

  60. Bates D, Mächler M, Bolker B, Walker S. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J Stat Software; 1(1):67. 2015; Available from: https://www.jstatsoft.org/v067/i01

  61. Green P, MacLeod CJ. SIMR: an R package for power analysis of generalized linear mixed models by simulation. Methods Ecol Evol [Internet]. 2016;7(4):493–8. https://doi.org/10.1111/2041-210X.12504.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Kuznetsova A, Brockhoff PB, Christensen RHB. lmerTest package: tests in linear mixed effects models. [Internet]. 2017;82(13):26.

  63. Wickham H, Averick M, Bryan J, Chang W, D’Agostino McGowan L, François R, et al. Welcome to the tidyverse. J Open Source Softw. 2019;4(43):1686–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  64. Wickham H. ggplot2: elegant graphics for data analysis [Internet]. New York: Springer-Verlag, New York; 2016.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  65. Westfall J, Kenny DA, Judd CM. Statistical power and optimal design in experiments in which samples of participants respond to samples of stimuli. Vol. 143, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. Westfall, Jacob: Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of Colorado, Boulder, CO, US, 80309–0345, jake.westfall@colorado.edu: American Psychological Association; 2014. p. 2020–45.

  66. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Academic press; 2013.

  67. Baayen RH, Milin P. Analyzing reaction times. Int J Psychol Res. 2010;3(2):12–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  68. Fan J, McCandliss BD, Fossella J, Flombaum JI, Posner MI. The activation of attentional networks. Neuroimage. 2005;26(2):471–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Markett S, Reuter M, Montag C, Voigt G, Lachmann B, Rudorf S, et al. Assessing the function of the fronto-parietal attention network: insights from resting-state fMRI and the attentional network test. Hum Brain Mapp. 2014;35(4):1700–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Trautwein F-M, Singer T, Kanske P. Stimulus-driven reorienting impairs executive control of attention: evidence for a common bottleneck in anterior insula. Cereb Cortex [Internet]. 2016;26(11):4136–47. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhw225.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  71. Botvinick MM, Cohen JD, Carter CS. Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: an update. Trends Cogn Sci. 2004;8(12):539–46.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Ham T, Leff A, de Boissezon X, Joffe A, Sharp DJ. Cognitive control and the salience network: an investigation of error processing and effective connectivity. J Neurosci [Internet]. 2013;33(16):7091–8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  73. Hart H, Radua J, Nakao T, Mataix-Cols D, Rubia K. Meta-analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging studies of inhibition and attention in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: exploring task-specific, stimulant medication, and age effects. JAMA Psychiatry [Internet]. 2013;70(2):185–98. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.277.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Francx W, Oldehinkel M, Oosterlaan J, Heslenfeld D, Hartman CA, Hoekstra PJ, et al. The executive control network and symptomatic improvement in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Cortex [Internet]. 2015;73:62–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Bachmann K, Lam AP, Sörös P, Kanat M, Hoxhaj E, Matthies S, et al. Effects of mindfulness and psychoeducation on working memory in adult ADHD: A randomised, controlled fMRI study. Behav Res Ther [Internet]. 2018;106:47–56.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. Fam J, Sun Y, Qi P, Lau RC, Feng L, Kua EH, et al. Mindfulness practice alters brain connectivity in community-living elders with mild cognitive impairment. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2020;74(4):257–62.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Kilpatrick LA, Suyenobu BY, Smith SR, Bueller JA, Goodman T, Creswell JD, et al. Impact of mindfulness-based stress reduction training on intrinsic brain connectivity. Neuroimage [Internet]. 2011;56(1):290–8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  78. Santarnecchi E, Egiziano E, D’Arista S, Gardi C, Romanella SM, Mencarelli L, et al. Mindfulness-based stress reduction training modulates striatal and cerebellar connectivity. J Neurosci Res. 2021;99(5):1236–52.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  79. Kim Y-H, Yoo W-K, Ko M-H, Park C, Kim ST, Na DL. Plasticity of the attentional network after brain injury and cognitive rehabilitation. Neurorehabil Neural Repair [Internet]. 2009;23(5):468–77. https://doi.org/10.1177/1545968308328728.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. Parisi L, Rocca MA, Valsasina P, Panicari L, Mattioli F, Filippi M. Cognitive rehabilitation correlates with the functional connectivity of the anterior cingulate cortex in patients with multiple sclerosis. Brain Imag Behav [Internet]. 2014;8(3):387–93. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-012-9160-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  81. Cooper RP. Cognitive control: componential or emergent? Top Cogn Sci [Internet]. 2010;2(4):598–613. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1756-8765.2010.01110.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  82. Sørensen L, Wass S, Osnes B, Schanche E, Adolfsdottir S, Svendsen JL, et al. A psychophysiological investigation of the interplay between orienting and executive control during stimulus conflict: a heart rate variability study. Physiol Behav. 2019;211: 112657.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  83. Paap KR, Sawi O. The role of test-retest reliability in measuring individual and group differences in executive functioning. J Neurosci Methods [Internet]. 2016;274:81–93.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  84. Wöstmann NM, Aichert DS, Costa A, Rubia K, Möller H-J, Ettinger U. Reliability and plasticity of response inhibition and interference control. Brain Cogn [Internet]. 2013;81(1):82–94.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to express our appreciation to the individuals who participated in this study, as well as to Emilie S. Nordby, Bente Ubostad, Paul M. Kauserud, and Marius Stavang for their contributions as co-therapists. We are also thankful for the contributions of Anne Øfsthus, Benedicte Mjeldheim, Liv Heldal, Marius Stavang and Emilie S. Nordby in the data collection for the project.

Funding

Open access funding provided by University of Bergen. This study received funding from the K.G. Jebsen Centre for Neuropsychiatric Disorders and the Norwegian National Research Network for ADHD. Members of the K.G. Jebsen Centre for Neuropsychiatric Disorders participated in the design of the study as well as collection, analysis and interpretation of the data.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

DAJ: Literature search, figures, study design, data collection, data analysis, data interpretation, writing. AJL, JS, AH, JH: Literature search, data interpretation, study design. LS: Literature search, study design, data collection, data interpretation, writing, supervision. All authors have read and approved by the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Daniel A. Jensen.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

All participants gave informed consent, and the study was carried out in line with the Declaration of Helsinki. Ethical approval was granted by the Regional Ethics Committee of Norway (Regional Etisk Komite Vest: Study number 2015/2325).

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

DAJ, AJL, JS, AH, JH and LS declare that the research has been conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest. JH declares that he has received speaker honoraria from Lilly, Shire, HB Pharma, Medice, Takeda and Biocodex.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1:

Table S1: Summary of linear mixed-effects model comparisons for the Flanker Conflict-Effect.

Additional file 2: Fig. S1:

A graphical illustration of observed vs predicted flanker conflict effects for each individual participant

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Jensen, D.A., Lundervold, A.J., Stubberud, J. et al. Goal management training improves executive control in adults with ADHD: an open trial employing attention network theory to examine effects on attention. BMC Psychol 10, 207 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-022-00902-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-022-00902-9

Keywords