Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Exploring presentation differences in multi-cultural youth seeking assistance for mental health problems

Abstract

Background

Mental ill-health can impact an individual’s capacity to interact with others, make decisions, and cope with social challenges. This is of particular importance for many Culturally and linguistically diverse (CALD) individuals who may be at various stages of the acculturation process. The increasing diversity of the Australian population necessitates informed and culturally relevant services that meet the needs of a changing demographic. However the extant research on the mental health needs of CALD Australians is limited. This study aimed to further our understanding of the mental health needs of young CALD Australians by exploring the mental health concerns and social factors exhibited by CALD individuals accessing community based youth mental health services in two major cities.

Methods

We performed a series of logistic regression models to ascertain if a concert of factors (i.e., clinical, socio-economic, criminal justice system involvement, child maltreatment, social support) were associated with CALD status

Results

Comparisons across factors revealed no significant differences between groups. A small number of correlates differentiated between CALD and non-CALD participants (mental illness diagnosis during childhood, family history of mental illness/suicide, sensation seeking, sensitivity to punishment, maternal overprotection) however these factors were no longer meaningful after adjustment for multiple comparisons.

Conclusions

In help-seeking mainstream youth populations, cultural differences across clinical and environmental factors appear to be minimal.

Peer Review reports

The increasing diversity of the Australian population is well documented. This is most apparent in two of Australia’s largest cities, Melbourne and Sydney where more than 3.2 million overseas-born Australians reside [1]. Australians from non-English speaking backgrounds are often referred to as culturally and linguistically diverse (CALD) populations [2]. CALD communities are heterogeneous; they comprise people with diverse cultural norms, practices and traditions, languages, religions, family structures and life experiences [1,2,3]. Moreover, many individuals from CALD backgrounds have experienced distinctive socio-historical and pre- and post-migratory experiences and stressors which are often implicated in their sub-group’s collective health and wellbeing [4,5,6,7].

Mental health is one important marker, given its impact on one’s capacity to effectively interact with others, make decisions, relate to others and cope with social challenges. This is of particular importance for many CALD individuals who may be at various stages of the integration and/or acculturation processes, both of which can bestow unique challenges and personal stressors [3, 8, 9]. The extant research on the mental health of CALD Australians is limited [10]. A national survey from 2007 noted lower reported levels of life-time mental disorder for CALD Australians compared to the general population [11]. This appears to be especially pronounced for CALD individuals born in non-English speaking countries [11]. Other Australian studies have found similar, or marginally higher rates of mental ill-health in immigrant populations compared to majority culture populations [12, 13]. However analyses focusing on specific CALD sub-populations, particularly humanitarian arrivals, have uncovered high rates of selected mental health concerns [14,15,16,17]. Worldwide, refugees experience post-traumatic stress disorder at approximately ten times the rate of the general population [18]. A high occurrence of Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder has been identified among Australian refugee populations from South Sudan, Vietnam, Burma and Sri Lanka [17, 19,20,21]. Generally however, the available literature on the mental health of CALD populations has been small in scope and possesses definitional inconsistencies [10]. Accurately identifying CALD subjects in datasets (where country of birth, language spoken, and ethnicity are not consistently detailed) is often challenging. Furthermore, the term ‘CALD’ combines numerous heterogeneous cultural sub-groups, which when combined in aggregate form, precludes unique information about such sub-groups.

Progressing our knowledge on the mental health of CALD Australians is a constructive endeavour for several reasons. First, the increasing diversity of the Australian population necessitates informed and relevant services that can readily meet the needs of a changing demographic. Prior research has noted both low levels of mental health literacy and an underutilization of mental health services [22,23,24,25,26] for CALD residents in Australian settings. Cultural viewpoints on understanding or recognising mental ill-health (i.e., stigma, shame) may also prevent help-seeking and medical intervention [27,28,29]. Second, mental health practitioners may require an improved understanding of CALD interaction and presentation styles in order to enhance the effectiveness of the clinical encounter and therapeutic alliance. For example, CALD individuals may present with unique symptom reporting styles or exhibit culturally specific behaviours [30, 31]. Moreover, some CALD individuals may be unwilling to disclose personal health information or minimize psychopathology in clinical scenarios as a result of mistrust or a discomfort with impersonal, generic services [22, 32,33,34]. Third, an inability to address the mental health needs of CALD migrants can impact their ability to effectively integrate, engage with, and contribute to the broader society.

This study aims to further our understanding of the mental health of CALD Australians by exploring the mental health concerns and social factors exhibited by young CALD individuals accessing community based youth mental health services in the greater metropolitan regions of Melbourne and Sydney, Australia. In particular we aim to identify any differences in mental health problems (i.e., diagnostic information) and social factors (i.e., employment, family support, adverse life events, justice system involvement) between CALD and non-CALD participants. Findings will elucidate the key concerns faced by young CALD Australians who are seeking assistance for mental health problems.

Method

Sample

Established by Australia’s federal government in 2006, headspace provides physical and mental health services, drug and alcohol services, and vocational assistance to people aged 12–25 years with emerging and established mental health problems. Because headspace delivers a range of services informed by principles underpinning early intervention, service users may present with a range of psychosocial problems with varying degrees of severity, although mood and affective symptoms predominate [35]. Clinical services are delivered by general practitioners, psychologists, psychiatrists, and other allied health professionals, and are largely subsidised through publicly funded health-care schemes. Most young people either self-refer or are referred by family, friends, health professionals, or school counsellors.

All young people who attended one of four headspace services in Melbourne or Sydney, Australia, between January 2011 and August 2012, spoke English, and were capable of providing informed consent, were approached to seek their participation in a longitudinal cohort study examining the course of psychiatric disorders in this population [36]. Three of the centres are in outer-city suburbs characterised by socioeconomic disadvantage and limited private sector investment in mental health. The fourth centre is in a relatively affluent inner-city suburb. It should be noted, however, that since there are no defined geographical catchment areas for headspace centres, young people can attend a service irrespective of their place of residence. Prospective participants included those who were receiving clinical services at the time of the study as well as those who were waitlisted. Those who were significantly intellectually impaired (i.e., IQ < 65) and could not either provide informed consent or complete the assessment tasks were excluded from the study, while those who were acutely suicidal (as determined by their assessing or treating headspace clinician) were not approached to participate until their risk was reduced.

Procedure

The human research ethics committees at the University of Melbourne and the University of Sydney approved the study. Following assessment by a headspace Access Team clinician or completion of their first treatment session, prospective participants were contacted by telephone or in person by research assistants (RA) with a minimum four-year graduate psychology degree to discuss the aims and nature of the study and to determine their interest in participating. Participants aged 15 years and older provided written informed consent, whereas those aged 12–14 years assented and written informed consent was provided by a parent or guardian. The RAs conducted semi-structured interviews with each participant using a range of clinical measures. Registered psychologists trained the RAs in the use of the measures, such that the RAs achieved very good inter-rater reliability on each measure (kappa ≥ 0.8) before recruitment commenced. Following the interview, the RAs provided participants with an iPad or laptop on which they completed several self-report measures. Participants received a $20 gift voucher to compensate the time associated with completing each assessment.

Measures

Demographic and socioeconomic information

Participants’ age, sex, country of birth (and their parents’ country of birth), languages spoken, relationship status, accommodation status, living arrangements, education and employment status, financial difficulties, and social welfare entitlements were ascertained using questions adapted from the national census [37] and other published sources [38, 39].

Cultural and linguistic diversity

Researchers and practitioners alike often describe people as being culturally and linguistically diverse (CALD) if their primary language, cultural norms, and values differ from those of the mainstream community in which they reside [2]. The term is typically applied to those from non-English speaking backgrounds. Here, we have used the term to represent “people of colour”, a term which generally excludes White/Caucasian populations with largely European ancestry. As such, the CALD group in this study largely comprises people who were born (or whose parent[s] was born) in Africa, Asia, Pacific Islands, Latin America, the Caribbean, or the Middle East. This was to ensure that our CALD group was likely to be from a visibly non-White minority group in addition to possessing non-English speaking ancestry. Those who were born (or whose parents were born) in a primarily European-language-speaking country (including Australia), but reported primarily speaking a non-European language were also considered to be CALD. We considered people who identified (or whose parents identified) as Maori to be CALD. Although Maori people are Indigenous to New Zealand, the term Indigenous in the Australian context solely refers to those of Aboriginal and Torres Strait Islander ancestry. Aboriginal and Torres Strait Islanders are typically not included as CALD in the Australian context and as such were not included in the CALD group in this study. Because of the strong European heritage of both countries, those with Argentine or Uruguayan backgrounds were not considered to be CALD unless their primary language suggested otherwise (i.e., spoke a non-European language). Although Zulu is the language spoken most often in South Africa, English is the principal language of most South African migrants to Australia. Therefore, people with South African backgrounds whose self-reported primary language was either English or Afrikaans were not considered to be CALD. However, South African migrants who reported primarily speaking a non-European (and non-Afrikaans) language were considered to be CALD.

Clinical measures

General psychopathology

The Kessler 10 (K-10) [40] was used as a broad measure of psychological distress. The scale comprises 10 questions that enquire about the respondent’s negative emotional states experienced during the past four weeks. The degree to which each item is experienced is measured on a five-point scale. Item scores are summed and range between 10 and 50. Scores between 25 and 29 suggest the presence of a moderately severe mental disorder, while scores of 30 and above are indicative of more severe psychopathology.

Anxiety

The generalized anxiety disorder scale (GAD-7) [41] is a seven-item self-report measure of the most salient diagnostic features of generalised anxiety disorder. The frequency with which each of the symptoms is experienced in the previous two weeks is rated on a four-point scale. Research has suggested that the GAD-7 is a valid screening tool for GAD in primary care settings and for assessing its severity in clinical practice and research [41]. Scale scores range from 0 to 21 with higher scores indicating more severe psychopathology. Scores of five, 10, and 15 indicate the presence of a “mild”, “moderate”, and “severe” anxiety disorder, respectively. The overall anxiety severity and impairment scale (OASIS) [42] consists of five items that assess the frequency and severity of anxiety, use of avoidance behaviours, and the extent to which anxiety interferes with the respondent’s social and occupational functioning. Items are scored on a five-point scale and represent the respondent’s self-reported experience over the past week.

Depression

The clinician-rated quick inventory of depressive symptomatology (QIDS-C16), [43] assesses the presence, during the previous seven days, of the major symptoms of depression as defined by the fourth edition of the diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-IV). Its 16 items, reflecting depressed mood, sleep disturbance, appetite/weight disturbance, diminished interest, lowered energy/fatigue, poor concentration, self-criticism, and suicidal ideation are rated on a four-point scale and summed to provide a score ranging from 0 to 27. Scores above 16 are considered to indicate the presence of a severe depressive disorder.

Ruminative style

Rumination was assessed using a 10-item questionnaire [44] derived from a longer, validated scale [45]. Respondents indicated the extent to which they experienced each item on a four-point scale.

Mania

The young mania rating scale (YMRS) [46] measures the nature and severity of core manic symptoms experienced within the past 48 h. Ratings for the 11 items are based on the interviewee’s subjective report of his or her clinical condition. Additional information is based on the interviewer’s clinical observations. Seven items are scored on a four-point scale. The remaining four items are scored on an eight-point scale. These latter items are weighted more heavily to compensate for poor cooperation by those who are severely ill. Scores are summed and range from 0 to 60.

Psychosis

The risk of psychosis was assessed using the comprehensive assessment of the at-risk mental state (CAARMS) [47], a semi-structured interview designed for use by mental health professionals to assess the presence and severity of psychotic symptoms over the past 12 months. The schedule measures symptoms across several domains: positive symptoms, concentration and attention, emotional disturbance, negative symptoms, behavioural change, motor abnormalities, and general psychopathology. The Positive Symptom scale, used in this study, comprises four subscales: unusual thought content, non-bizarre ideas, perceptual abnormalities, and disorganised speech. These subscales were rated according to the intensity, frequency, and duration of the symptoms, their relationship to substance use, and associated level of distress. Pre-set thresholds on both the intensity and frequency of these symptoms were used to classify participants as “psychotic”, “at risk” for psychosis (based on their subthreshold psychotic symptoms), or “not at risk” for psychosis.

Personality

The behavioural inhibition/behavioural activation system (BIS/BAS) [48] was used as a broad measure of participants’ subjective personality style. It comprises 24 items that index the person’s behavioural inhibition, responsiveness to reward and punishment, drive, and fun-seeking. The items are rated on a four-point scale and summed.

Childhood-onset disorder

Participants’ recalled whether they had ever received a diagnosis of a mental (i.e., neurocognitive, emotional, or behavioural) disorder during childhood.

Substance use

The alcohol, smoking and substance involvement screening test (ASSIST) [49] is an eight-item self-report measure of tobacco, alcohol, and illicit drug use, and associated concerns. The responses to six of the items are summed and used to indicate the level of risk (i.e., “lower”, “moderate”, or “’high”) associated with each substance. Responses to the final item are not used in these calculations, but when endorsed, indicate the recency of injecting substance use (which may itself indicate elevated risk of substance-related harm).

Clinical stage

Each participant was assigned a clinical stage based on criteria established by McGorry et al. (2006) [50]. The clinical staging model comprises six discrete stages: stage 0 (asymptomatic people at risk of a disorder who have not yet presented for care); stage 1a (help-seekers with mild symptoms and functional impacts); stage 1b (people with attenuated syndromes, often with mixed or ambiguous symptomatology and moderate or severe functional impacts); stage 2 (people with discrete disorders [i.e., those presenting with clear psychotic, manic, or severe depressive episodes); stage 3 (people with a recurrent or persistent disorder); and stage 4 (people with a severe, persistent, and unremitting illness). Staging decisions are informed by clinical assessment of the person’s current symptomatology (severity, frequency, and type); characteristic mental features; age of onset and course of illness before presentation to health services; previous “worst ever” symptoms and treatment episodes (inc., hospital admissions); current level of risk of harm due to the person’s illness; previous suicide attempts or other risky behaviours; and current (compared to premorbid) levels of social and occupational functioning.

Clinical severity

Rated on a seven-point scale, the single-item clinical global impressions scale (CGI) [51] indicates the severity of the person’s illness with higher scores indicating a more severe presentation. The item is completed by the interviewer and represents his or her clinical impression of the interviewee’s presentation and level of functioning, derived from all available information obtained during the assessment.

Occupational functioning and disability

The social and occupational functioning scale (SOFAS) [52] is an observer-rated scale that provides a global assessment of one’s social and occupational functioning. Scores range from 0 to 100. Higher scores indicate a superior level of functioning. For the purpose of this study, scores were calculated based on the participants’ lowest level of functioning in the past year. The assessment of the person’s level of functioning is independent of the severity of his or her symptomatology and includes impairments that are caused by either physical or mental disorders. To be considered, impairments must be caused by the illness per se rather than a reflection of a lack of opportunity or environmental limitations. The 12-item version of the World Health Organization Disability Assessment Schedule (WHODAS 2.0) [53] was used to examine participants’ difficulties in performing daily life activities during the past 30 days. Items are rated on a five-point scale and reflect six domains of functioning: cognition, mobility, self-care, social interactions, life activities (e.g., domestic responsibilities, leisure, work, and school), and participation in community activities. Total scores represent the simple sum of the 12 items and range from 0 to 48. Scores can also be scaled with a range from 0 to 100. Higher scores indicate a greater degree of disability.

Quality of life

Participants’ perceptions of their overall quality of life in the preceding four weeks were assessed using a single Likert item derived from the World Health Organization Quality of Life (WHOQOL) [54].

Family history of mental disorder

Participants were asked whether, to the best of their knowledge, any immediate family members (i.e., parents or siblings) had (1) ever experienced a serious psychological or emotional problem or (2) died by suicide [55]. Those who had no knowledge of their biological family did not complete this measure.

Forensic history

Participants were asked whether they had ever been (1) charged with a criminal offence; (2) convicted of a criminal offence; or (3) a victim of crime. Affirmative responses were followed up to clarify the nature of the crime and the outcome of any charges or convictions. Violence—whether perpetrated or experienced—was defined as any intentional behaviour involving threatened or actual physical harm (e.g., sexual or non-sexual assault, threats to kill or inflict injury). All other criminal behaviour (including theft, drug use or possession, property damage) was considered non-violent.

Child maltreatment

The childhood trauma questionnaire (CTQ) [56] was administered to assess participants’ experience of abuse and neglect during childhood and adolescence. Its 28 items measure the experience of three forms of abuse (physical, sexual, and emotional) and two forms of neglect (physical and emotional); a three-item scale is used to detect false-negative reports. Respondents rate the extent to which they experienced each item on a five-point scale. Higher scores indicate more frequent maltreatment; defined thresholds are used to classify cases according to their severity (i.e., “none”, “low”, “moderate”, or “severe”).

Parental style

A short version of the parental bonding instrument (PBI) [57] was used to assess participants’ recollection of their parents’ behaviour toward them. The scale comprises 18 items, which measure both maternal and paternal care, overprotection, and authoritarianism. Respondents rate the frequency with which they experienced each item during childhood on a four-point scale and the scores are summed.

Social support

A 20-item questionnaire [58] was used to assess the quality of participants’ interactions with their family, friends, and partner (where applicable). The items are scored on a four-point scale and summed to yield six indexes that measure either positive or negative qualities within these domains. The composite scores are scaled such that they theoretically range from 0 to 1 with higher scores reflecting greater endorsement of the underlying items.

Stigmatisation

Three items were adapted from the discrimination scale in the quality of life in newly diagnosed epilepsy instrument (NEWQOL) [59]. Participants were asked whether or not, because of their mental health problems, others: (1) are uncomfortable with them; (2) treat them as inferior; or (3) prefer to avoid them.

Statistical analysis

All analyses were conducted using R version 4.0.2 [60]. For all standard analyses, two-tailed tests were used with a significance level of α < 0.05. We corrected for multiple comparisons using a false discovery rate (FDR) of 5% [61]; q-values were calculated using the qvalue package [62].

Missing data

Of the 1615 eligible help-seekers approached by the RAs to participate, 806 consented (of whom four subsequently withdrew), representing a participation rate of 49.9%. The majority of those records retained for analysis (662 [82.5%]) were incomplete. Nearly three-quarters (483 [73.0%]) of these records were missing ≤ 5.3% of their values (range, 0.9–84.7%). Only three (2.7%) variables were complete. The number of missing values across variables ranged from one (0.1%) to 319 (39.8%).

Missing indicators were created for each variable with missing data. Large correlations among variables with missing values suggested that the propensity for missingness in a given variable was related to the presence of missing values in other variables. Fitted binary logistic regression models with the indicator variables as outcomes demonstrated that missingness was associated with observed values, suggesting they were at least partly missing at random [63].

We estimated missing values using multiple imputation by chained equations (MICE) [64, 65], a technique that involves imputing each missing value with multiple values estimated from the posterior predictive distribution of the missing data conditional on the observed data. Variables were imputed using flexible additive regression models as implemented in the aregImpute function from the Hmisc package [66]. aregImpute accounts for all aspects of uncertainty in the imputations by using the bootstrap to approximate the process of drawing predicted values from a full Bayesian predictive distribution. We generated eighty complete datasets. Trace plots demonstrated that the results were stable across the iterations for each imputation, suggesting convergence had been achieved. Density plots showed the distributions of the imputed and complete data were similar.

Descriptive analyses

Numeric variables were expressed as an arithmetic mean and standard deviation. Categorical variables were expressed as counts and percentages. The distributions of characteristics of CALD and non-CALD help-seekers were compared using independent-sample t-tests and Pearson chi-square tests of independence. Mean differences (MD) and odds ratios (OR), and their associated 95% confidence intervals, were reported as the primary measures of effect.

Correlates of CALD status

Sixty-six variables of interest were classified to one of nine domains: demographic, financial security, mental disorder, substance use, functional impairment, forensic history, child maltreatment, social support, and stigma. Given the large number of variables, we performed a series of principal components analyses (PCA) using the psych package [67] to reduce the dimensionality of the dataset and eliminate redundancy among the covariates. We used the Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin (KMO) measure of sampling adequacy [68] to determine the proportion of variance in each set of variables that might be explained by underlying factors and Bartlett’s test of sphericity [69] to assess the suitability of the data for PCA. Principal components with eigenvalues greater than 1 were retained. Oblique (promax) rotation was performed on the variables to improve interpretability (see supplementary information). Component scores were computed for each participant and retained for subsequent analyses.

We constructed several logistic regression models using the lrm function in the rms package [70] to identify those factors that were associated with CALD status. Participants were categorised into CALD and non-CALD status as delineated earlier. We performed univariate analyses to examine the association of CALD status and each candidate variable. Then, we examined the association of CALD status and each of the eight variable domains by developing a series of restricted multivariate models comprising only those variables classified to each domain. We assessed model fit using a likelihood ratio test comparing each model to the null model and assessed model performance using the Nagelkerke R2 [71]. Calibration was determined by computing the mean squared deviation of each predicted probability from the true observed value of the outcome (i.e., the Brier score) [72]. The Brier score can be considered a weighted loss function in which increasing distance between predicted and observed values is penalised by a quadratic measure. It is a proper score function that ranges from 0 to 1. Although its numerical value has no direct meaning, lower scores indicate better performance. Internal validation was performed using bootstrap resampling (5000 replicates). Finally, we quantified the importance of each domain by comparing the fit of the restricted models to those of a fully adjusted multivariate model, in which all variables were entered simultaneously, using a likelihood ratio test.

Sensitivity analyses

Following multiple imputation (MI), each complete dataset is typically analysed separately using standard methods. Then, the M parameter estimates and their associated variances are pooled using Rubin’s rules to provide a single parameter estimate that incorporates both between- and within-imputation variability, thereby enabling correct inference [73]. However, problems arise when using MI in the context of PCA. Because of the variability in the imputed values, there is no guarantee that the eigenvector corresponding to a given eigenvalue is comparable across datasets. Consequently, pooling the eigenvectors (principal axes, factor loadings) using the order or the obtained eigenvalues of the covariance matrix estimated from each imputed dataset is likely to lead to misleading or meaningless results. Similarly, determining a common set of principal components across imputed datasets can be problematic, with the variability in the imputed values leading to different decisions being made for different datasets [74]. Considering these difficulties, we performed our analyses on a single dataset selected at random. To assess whether our results were sensitive to the variability in the imputed values, we conducted the same analyses in 10 additional datasets selected at random.

Results

Demographic characteristics

Participant characteristics are reported in Table 1. Most participants (718 [91.6%]) were born in Australia, thirty-five (4.9%) of whom identified as Australian Aboriginal or Torres Strait Islander. Of the 66 participants born overseas, 36 (54.5%) were born in Europe, New Zealand, Canada, or the USA. The remaining 30 participants were born in Asia (17 [25.8%]), Africa (10 [15.2%]), the Middle East (2 [3.0%]), and Latin America (1 [1.5%]). Similarly, most of the participants’ parents (558 [71.5%] of mothers and 520 [68.7%] of fathers) were born in Australia. Three in every five overseas-born parents (134 [66.0%] of mothers, 123 [56.7%] of fathers) were born in Europe, New Zealand, Canada, or the USA. Among those participants born in Australia, 283 (39.4%) had at least one parent who was born overseas; 93 (13.0%) had at least one parent who was born in Asia (51 [54.8%]), the Pacific Islands (16 [17.2%]), Latin America (11 [11.8%]), the Middle East (9 [9.7%]), and Africa (7 [7.5%]). Virtually all (771 [98.5%]) participants reported speaking English at home; 108 (13.8%) reported speaking more than one language. Nearly one in ten (63 [8.1%]) reported speaking a non-European language. Based on these characteristics, 128 participants (17.3% of the sample) were classified as CALD. Missing values precluded the classification of 62 participants.

Table 1 Baseline characteristics of CALD and non-CALD participants

Correlates of CALD status

Logistic regression was used to examine the extent to which CALD status was associated with a range of demographic, socioeconomic, clinical, and forensic indicators. The Box–Tidwell [75] procedure was used to test for linearity of continuous independent variables and their logits. Significance tests performed on the interaction between each continuous variable and its log were not significant (p > 0.05), suggesting that the data were consistent with a model in which the assumption was valid.

Table 2 reports the unadjusted and adjusted OR for the potential correlates of CALD status. Only two of the nine domain-based models were statistically significant (Table 3). Demographic characteristics and socioeconomic indicators were similar for both groups. Moreover, there were no differences in risk of substance use, levels of functional impairment, rates of criminal offending or victimisation, social support, or stigmatising experiences. The mental disorder model was statistically significant, χ2(13) = 31.25, p = 0.003, but explained only 2.3% of the variance in CALD status (bias-corrected Nagelkerke R2 = 0.023). CALD participants reported being less responsive to reward and punishment and reported experiencing lower drive and sensation-seeking tendencies than non-CALD participants. Moreover, they were less likely to report being diagnosed with a mental disorder during childhood and to report having an immediate family member (i.e., parent or sibling) who had experienced a mental disorder or died by suicide. None of these variables remained significant after correcting for multiple comparisons. The child maltreatment model was also significant, χ2(7) = 24.72, p < 0.001 (bias-corrected Nagelkerke R2 = 0.027). Although CALD participants reported higher levels of maternal overprotection than non-CALD participants, the observed difference was not significant following correction (Table 4).

Table 2 Correlates of CALD status
Table 3 Model fit and performance metrics for full and restricted domain-based models
Table 4 Likelihood ratio tests comparing full model to domain-based models

The fully adjusted model was statistically significant, χ2(37) = 69.30, p = 0.001, but explained only 1.8% of the variance in CALD status (bias-corrected Nagelkerke R2 = 0.018). Two of the 31 variables entered in the model were significantly associated with CALD status. In addition to being less likely to report being diagnosed with a mental disorder during childhood, CALD participants reported higher levels of maternal overprotection. However, neither variable remained significant following correction (see supplementary information for raw and corrected p-values). Results did not differ substantially across datasets, suggesting they were largely unaffected by the presence of missing data (see supplementary information for full details).

Discussion

There has been limited research on the mental health concerns of CALD Australians compared to majority culture populations. This study aimed to further our understanding by exploring differences in mental health problems and social factors between CALD and non-CALD young people presenting at a community based mental health services in metropolitan regions across the Australian states of Victoria and New South Wales. Similar levels of mental health symptoms, substance use, early adverse life experiences, and socioeconomic dynamics were identified cross-culturally. Although four correlates meaningfully differentiated between CALD and non-CALD populations, these findings were no longer significant after correcting for multiple testing. Comparisons across numerous clinical factors (e.g., general psychopathology, stigma, disability, clinical stage, quality of life, substance use, psychotic symptoms, depression/anxiety, criminal history) revealed no significant differences between groups.

Baseline characteristics were largely similar across groups. Recent clinical symptoms (for psychosis, anxiety, and depression) in particular, were comparable. Though limited, prior surveys have pointed to lower [11] and higher [12] levels of mental health concerns among non-English speaking migrants compared to Australian-born and overseas-born English speakers. Differences in findings likely reflect the nature of the surveys administered (i.e., clinical diagnostic criteria vs. generic symptoms). In our study, CALD status included Australian-born individuals with non-English speaking background ethnicities. This grouping perhaps encompassed many ‘second-generation’ individuals whose lifestyles may be consistent with those of the non-CALD group (for example, more than two-thirds of our CALD sample were born in Australia and several others born in English-speaking countries overseas). This may have engendered similarities in reporting, unlike prior research whereby ‘second generation’ CALD individuals are included in native-born populations [11, 12].

Cultural considerations

A small number of cross-cultural differences were noted prior to multiple comparison adjustments. These bear some consideration and are potential avenues for further scientific inquiry. The CALD group was less likely to report a mental illness diagnosis during childhood, less likely to report a family history of mental illness or suicide, and presented with lower levels of sensation seeking and sensitivity to punishment/reward stimuli. Moreover, CALD participants were more likely to report maternal authoritarianism compared to non-CALD participants.

Lower reported levels of child-onset disorder were identified for CALD participants. The extent to which this finding reflects actual child prevalence rates across cultures in our sample, or rather the influence of sociocultural factors, is unknown. Community stigmas around mental illness are present within some CALD populations [22, 28, 29]. This may preclude families from seeking assistance from formal mental health service providers for mental health concerns. Evidence suggests that some CALD populations have a greater preference for informal support [23, 76]. Moreover, mental health literacy may be low or families may be unfamiliar with (or perhaps mistrust) mainstream mental health services [14, 22, 23, 77]. This may impede the possibility of an early childhood diagnosis for an affected family member with (in)attention to the behaviour, or remedies being handled ‘in house’. Some CALD youth may have spent the earlier parts of their childhood overseas where mental health services may be less established. Additionally, prior Australian research with CALD youth detected a minimisation of psychopathology [33], possibly the result of cultural shame or an unwillingness to share personal or humiliating experiences. This may induce differences between CALD and non-CALD participants when disclosing information on ‘child-onset disorder’ and ‘family history of mental illness or suicide’.

CALD youth presented with lower levels of drive, sensation seeking and sensitivity to punishment/reward stimuli. These items refer to self-reported involvement in fun-seeking behaviours and the aspiration of reward and the impulsive and/or persistent nature of this pursuit. No prior work has been conducted on how these traits may differ cross-culturally in Australia. Lower reported levels of these behaviours may reflect cultural differences in adolescent risk-taking. Western societies with ‘loose’ cultures (tolerant; relaxed social norms) are more likely to encourage exploration, assertiveness, permissiveness, and independence during adolescence [78, 79], whereas non-Western collectivist societies (particularly those with tight cultures) favour self-control, cooperation, cautiousness, and socially restrained behaviours [79,80,81]. CALD youth in our study may have possessed some of the latter traits. While the bulk of CALD youth were born in Australia, many may have been raised in bicultural environments where elements of their parents’ culture of origin were preserved. Maternal overprotection was associated with CALD status, as was maternal authoritarianism when analysing domain-specific models. Disciplinarian parenting styles in CALD communities have been noted in prior literature [82, 83]. While this style of parenting may restrict youth autonomy and subsequent risk-taking behaviour, it has also been found to incite familial intergenerational conflict and youth disconnection in acculturating migrant families [84, 85].

Implications

Study findings indicate that in a mental health help-seeking youth population possessing similar social demographics, cultural differences (CALD and non-CALD) appear to be minimal. This is unsurprising given that CALD youth in this sample are likely to be ‘second generation’ and inhabit more mainstream lifestyles. The fact that they accessed a mainstream mental service supports this contention. For clinicians working in such venues, expectations of cultural difference typically anticipated of first generation migrants, could perhaps be tempered for second-generation individuals. Prior Australian work suggests that for second generation CALD youth, rapport development, building trust, and understanding their unique challenges (i.e., negotiating two cultures, perceptions of racism) may warrant more attention rather than a focus on cultural idiosyncrasies and behaviours expected of recently arrived migrants [86]. Nevertheless, some CALD young people may inherit particular non-Western cultural norms if raised in bicultural environments or if they spent some part of their earlier life in a non-English speaking country. This could mean that mental health services may not have been previously accessed due to cultural stigmas or low mental health literacy, rendering psychopathologies and family histories of illness formally unrecognised or unrecorded. Efforts should be made by clinical staff to ascertain the extent to which historical undiagnosed mental health concerns may have transpired. Several frameworks exist to assist clinicians in tactfully extracting this information from CALD patients in non-direct, culturally appropriate ways [87]. It has been noted in prior research that some CALD young people may be less emotionally forthcoming in clinical encounters due to cultural expectations of stoicism and the suppression of feelings [87].

Limitations

This study has several limitations. While the proportion of CALD participants in this sample is representative of their proportion in the general population, it may not reflect the demographics and help-seeking behaviours of CALD youth generally. Many CALD youth will still keep mental health concerns private or will seek assistance initially from a family member or trusted community member. The study also precluded participants who were unable to speak English, which would have excluded recently arrived CALD migrants from the sample. It is possible that the psychometric properties of the instruments employed in the study may not generalise to non-Western populations, however, this may be inconsequential given the probably high acculturation levels of the CALD sample. Moreover, the number of questionnaires administered may have engendered a degree of survey fatigue, which may have contributed to the amount of missing data. Finally, the self-report nature of the questionnaires may have engendered under-reporting from CALD participants who may have minimised personal and familial behaviour deemed to be shameful or humiliating.

Conclusion

This study explored differences in mental health problems and social factors between CALD and non-CALD young people presenting at a community based mental health service in Australia. Similar mental health and socioeconomic profiles were identified. A small number of correlates discriminated between CALD and non-CALD participants (mental illness diagnosis during childhood, family history of mental illness/suicide, sensation seeking, sensitivity to punishment, maternal overprotection) however these factors were no longer meaningful after adjustment for multiple comparisons. In help-seeking mainstream youth populations, cultural differences across clinical factors appear to be minimal.

Availability of data and materials

The data that support the findings of this study are available from author RP but restrictions apply to the availability of these data, which were used under license for the current study, and so are not publicly available.

Abbreviations

ASSIST:

The alcohol, smoking and substance involvement screening test

BIS/BAS:

The behavioural inhibition/behavioural activation system

CAARMS:

Comprehensive assessment of the at-risk mental state

CALD:

Culturally and linguistically diverse

CGI:

Clinical global impressions scale

CTQ:

The childhood trauma questionnaire

DSM-IV:

Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders

FDR:

False discovery rate

GAD-7:

The generalized anxiety disorder scale

K-10:

The Kessler 10

KMO:

Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin

MD:

Mean differences

MI:

Multiple imputation

MICE:

Multiple imputation by chained equations

OASIS:

The overall anxiety severity and impairment scale

OR:

Odds ratios

NEWQOL:

Newly diagnosed epilepsy instrument

PBI:

Parental bonding instrument

QIDS-C:

Quick inventory of depressive symptomatology

PCA:

Principal components analysis

RA:

Research assistant

SOFAS:

The social and occupational functioning scale

WHODAS:

World Health Organization disability assessment schedule

WHOQOL:

World Health Organization quality of life

YMRS:

The young mania rating scale

References

  1. Australian Bureau of Statistics [ABS]. 4517.0—census of population and housing: reflecting Australia—stories from the Census. 2016. https://www.abs.gov.au/ausstats/abs@.nsf/Lookup/by%20Subject/2071.0~2016~Main%20Features~Cultural%20Diversity%20Data%20Summary~30. Accessed 5 Aug 2020.

  2. Australian Institute of Family Studies. Enhancing family and relationship service accessibility and delivery to culturally and linguistically diverse families in Australia. https://aifs.gov.au/cfca/publications/enhancing-family-and-relationship-service-accessibility-and/characteristics-and. Accessed 7 Aug 2020.

  3. Francis S, Cornfoot S. Multicultural Youth in Australia: Settlement and Transition. Canberra: Australian Research Alliance for Children & Youth; 2007.

    Google Scholar 

  4. Australian Institute of Health and Welfare. Australia’s Health 2018 Australia’s Health Series (No. 16). 2018. Canberra, AIHW.

  5. Centre for Multicultural Youth. Migrant & refuge young people negotiating adolescence in Australia. Victoria: Centre for Multicultural Youth; 2014.

  6. Shepherd SM. Criminal engagement and Australian culturally and linguistically diverse populations: challenges and implications for forensic risk assessment. Psychiatry Psychol Law. 2016;23:256–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. VicHealth, Data61, CSIRO and MYAN. Bright futures: spotlight on the wellbeing of young people from refugee and migrant backgrounds. Melbourne: Victorian Health Promotion Foundation; 2017

  8. Berry JW. Immigration, acculturation, and adaptation. Appl Psychol Int Rev. 1997;46:5–34.

    Google Scholar 

  9. Centre for Multicultural Youth. Fair and accurate? Migrant and refugee young people, crime and the media. Victoria: Centre for Multicultural Youth; 2014.

  10. Minas H, Kakuma R, Too LS, Vayani H, Orapeleng S, Prasad-Ildes R, et al. Mental health research and evaluation in multicultural Australia: developing a culture of inclusion. Int J Ment Health Syst. 2013;7:1–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Australian Institute of Health and Welfare. Comorbidity of mental disorders and physical conditions, 2007. 2011. https://www.aihw.gov.au/getmedia/6a8b451c-8def-47b2-a41a-9bcf59c77e76/10953-20140807.pdf.aspx. Accessed 9 Aug 2020.

  12. Jatrana S, Richardson K, Samba SRA. Investigating the dynamics of migration and health in Australia: a longitudinal study. Eur J Popul. 2017;34:519–65.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. Straiton M, Grant JF, Winefield HR, Taylor A. Mental health in immigrant men and women in Australia: the North West Adelaide health study. BMC Public Health. 2014;14:1111.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Khawaja NG, McCarthy R, Braddock V, Dunne M. Characteristics of culturally and linguistically diverse mental health clients. Adv Ment Health. 2014;11:172–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Khawaja NG, White KM, Schweitzer R, Greenslade JH. Difficulties and coping strategies of Sudanese refugees: a qualitative approach. Transcult Psychiatry. 2008;45:489–512.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Schweitzer R, Melville F, Steel Z, Lacherez P. Trauma, post-migration living difficulties, and social support as predictors of psychological adjustment in resettled Sudanese refugees. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2006;40:179–87.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Tempany M. What research tells us about the mental health and psychosocial wellbeing of Sudanese refugees: a literature review. Transcult Psychiatry. 2009;46:300–15.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Fazel M, Wheeler J, Danesh J. Prevalence of serious mental disorder in 7000 refugees resettled in western countries: a systematic review. Lancet. 2005;365:1309–14.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Schweitzer RD, Brough M, Vromans L, Asic-Kobe M. Mental health of newly arrived Burmese refugees in Australia: contributions of pre-migration and post-migration experience. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2011;45:299–307.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Silove D, Steel Z, McGorry P, Mohan P. Trauma exposure, postmigration stressors, and symptoms of anxiety, depression and post-traumatic stress in Tamil asylum-seekers: comparison with refugees and immigrants. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1998;97:175–81.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Steel Z, Silove D, Phan T, Bauman A. Long-term effect of psychological trauma on the mental health of Vietnamese refugees resettled in Australia: a population-based study. Lancet. 2002;360:1056–62.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Colucci E, Szwarc J, Minas H, Paxton G, Guerra C. The utilization of mental health services by children and young people from a refugee background: a systematic literature review. Int J Cult Ment Health. 2014;7:86–108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. De Anstiss H, Ziaian T, Procter N, Warland J, Baghurst P. Help-seeking for mental health problems in young refugees: a review of the literature with implications for policy, practice, and research. Transcult Psychiatry. 2009;46:584–607.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Federation of Ethnic Communities’ Councils of Australia. Mental health and Australia’s culturally and linguistically diverse communities—a submission to the Senate Standing Committee on Community Affairs. 2011. http://www.fecca.org.au/images/stories/documents/Submissions/2011/submissions_2011050.pdf. Accessed 9 Aug 2020.

  25. Murray KE, Davidson GR, Schweitzer RD. Psychological wellbeing of refugees resettling in Australia. Melbourne: The Australian Psychological Society; 2008.

    Google Scholar 

  26. Stolk Y, Minas H, Klimidis S. Access to mental health services in Victoria: a focus on ethnic communities. Melbourne: Victorian Transcultural Psychiatry Unit; 2008.

    Google Scholar 

  27. Colucci E, Minas H, Szwarc J, Paxton G, Guerra C. Barriers to and facilitators of utilisation of mental health services by young people of refugee background. 2014. https://refugeehealthnetwork.org.au/wp-content/uploads/Barriers+and+facilitators+pdf+final.pdf. Accessed 9 Aug 2020.

  28. Mellor D, Carne L, Shen Y-C, McCabe M, Wang L. Stigma toward mental illness: a cross-cultural comparison of Taiwanese, Chinese immigrants to Australia and Anglo-Australians. J Cross-Cult Psychol. 2012;44:352–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Omar Y, Kuay J, Tuncer C, Wriedt K, Minas H. Emotional wellbeing and access to culturally appropriate services: a comparative study of Muslim men of refugee background from the Horn of Africa, living in the inner-northern suburbs of Melbourne. Melbourne: Victorian Transcultural Mental Health; 2015.

    Google Scholar 

  30. Kirmayer LJ, Sartorius N. Cultural models and somatic syndromes. Psychosom Med. 2007;69:832–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Shepherd SM, Lewis-Fernandez R. Forensic risk assessment and cultural diversity—contemporary challenges and future directions. Psychol Public Policy Law. 2016;22:427–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Colucci E, Minas H, Szwarc J, Guerra C, Paxton G. In or out? Barriers and facilitators to refugee-background young people accessing mental health services. Transcult Psychiatry. 2015;52:766–90.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Kenny DT, Lennings CJ, Nelson PK. The mental health of young offenders serving orders in the community. J Offender Rehabil. 2008;45:123–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Wohler Y, Dantas JAR. Barriers accessing mental health services among culturally and linguistically diverse (CALD) immigrant women in Australia: policy implications. J Immigr Minor Health. 2017;19:697–701.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Rickwood DJ, Telford NR, Parker AG, Tanti CJ, McGorry PD. Headspace—Australia’s innovation in youth mental health: who are the clients and why are they presenting? Med J Aust. 2014;200:1–4.

    Google Scholar 

  36. Purcell R, Jorm AF, Hickie IB, Yung AR, Pantelis C, Amminger GP, et al. Transitions study of predictors of illness progression in young people with mental ill health: study methodology. Early Interv Psychiatry. 2013;9:38–47.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Trewin D. How Australia Takes a Census. Canberra: Australian Bureau of Statistics; 2006. https://www.abs.gov.au/AUSSTATS/abs@.nsf/DetailsPage/2903.02006. Accessed 10 Aug 2020.

  38. Baumann K. Direct measures of poverty as indicators of economic need: evidence from the Survey of Income and Program participation. In: Population division technical working paper no. 30, Washington: US Census; 1998.

  39. Killackey E, Jackson HJ, McGorry PD. Vocational intervention in first-episode psychosis: Individual placement and support v. treatment as usual. Br J Psychiatry. 2008;193:114–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Kessler RC, Barker PR, Colpe LJ, Epstein JF, Gfroerer JC, Hiripi E, et al. Screening for serious mental illness in the general population. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2003;60:184–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Spitzer RL, Kroenke K, Williams JB, Lowe B. A brief measure for assessing generalized anxiety disorder: the GAD-7. Arch Int Med. 2006;166:1092–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Norman SB, Cissell SH. Development and validation of an overall anxiety severity and impairment scale (OASIS). Depress Anxiety. 2006;23:245–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Rush AJ, Trivedi MH, Ibrahim HM, Carmody TJ, Arnow B, Klein DN, et al. The 16-item quick inventory of depressive symptomatology (QIDS), clinician rating ( QIDS-C), and self-report (QIDS-SR): a psychometric evaluation in patients with chronic major depression. Biol Psychiat. 2003;54:573–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Leach LS, Christensen H, Mackinnon AJ, Windsor TD, Butterworth P. Gender differences in depression and anxiety across the adult lifespan: the role of psychosocial mediators. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2008;43:983–98.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Nolenhoeksema S, Morrow J. Effects of rumination and distraction on naturally-occurring depressed mood. Cognit Emot. 1993;7:561–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Young R, Biggs J, Ziegler J, Meyer D. A rating scale for mania: reliability, validity and sensitivity. Br J Psychiatry. 1978;133:429–35.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Yung AR, Yuen HP, McGorry PD, Phillips LJ, Kelly D, Dell’Olio M, et al. Mapping the onset of psychosis: the comprehensive assessment of At-risk mental states. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2005;39:964–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Carver CS, White TL. Behavioral-inhibition, behavioral activation, and effective and affective responses to impending reward and punishment—the BIS BAS scales. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1994;67:319–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Humeniuk R, Ali R. Validation of the alcohol, smoking and substance involvement screening test (ASSIST). Addiction. 2008;103:1039–47.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. McGorry PD, Hickie IB, Yung AR, Pantelis C, Jackson HJ. Clinical staging of psychiatric disorders: a heuristic framework for choosing earlier, safer and more effective interventions. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2006;40:616–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Guy W. The clinical global impressions scale. In: Guy W, Bonato RR, editors. ECDEU assessment manual for psychopharmacology, Rev Ed. Rockville: National Institute of Mental Health; 1976. pp.157–69.

  52. Goldman H, Skodol A, Lave T. Revising axis V for DSM-IV: a review of measures of social functioning. Am J Psychiatry. 1992;149:1148–56.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Ustun TB, Chatterji S, Kostanjsek N, Rehm J, Kennedy C, Epping-Jordan J, et al. Developing the World Health Organization disability assessment schedule 2.0. Bull World Health Organ. 2010;88:815–23.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  54. Murphy B, Herrman H, Hawthorne G, Pinzone T, Evert H. Australian WHOQoL instruments: user’s manual and interpretation guide. Melbourne: Australian WHOQoL Field Study Centre; 2000.

    Google Scholar 

  55. King M, Walker C, Levy G, Bottomley C, Royston P, Weich S, et al. Development and validation of an international risk prediction algorithm for episodes of major depression in general practice attendees. The PRedictD Study. JAMA Psychiat. 2008;65:1368–76.

    Google Scholar 

  56. Bernstein DP, Fink L, Handelsman L, Foote J, Lovejoy M, Wenzel K, et al. Initial reliability and validity of a new retrospective measure of child abuse and neglect. Am J Psychiatry. 1994;151:1132–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Heider D, Matschinger H, Bernert S, Alonso J, Brugha TS, Bruffaerts R, et al. Adverse parenting as a risk factor in the occurrence of anxiety disorders: a study in six European countries. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2008;43:266–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Schuster TL, Kessler RC, Aseltine RH Jr. Supportive interactions, negative interactions, and depressed mood. Am J Community Psychol. 1990;18:423–38.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Abetz L, Jacoby A, Baker GA, McNulty P. Patient-based assessments of quality of life in newly diagnosed epilepsy patients: validation of the NEWQOL. Epilepsia. 2000;41:1119–28.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. R Core Team R: A language and environment for statistical computing [Computer software]. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. 2000.

  61. Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J R Stat Soc: Ser B (Methodol). 1995;57(1):289–300.

    Google Scholar 

  62. Storey JD, Bass AJ, Dabney A, Robinson D. qvalue: Q-value estimation for false discovery rate control. R package version 2.20.0 [Computer software]. 2020.

  63. Little RJA, Rubin DB. Statistical analysis with missing data. 2nd ed. Hoboken: Wiley; 2002.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  64. Raghunathan TE, Lepkowski JM, van Hoewyk J, Solenberger P. A multivariate technique for multiply imputing missing values using a sequence of regression models. Surv Pract. 2001;27:85–95.

    Google Scholar 

  65. van Buuren S. Flexible imputation of missing data. 2nd ed. Boca Raton: CRC Press; 2018.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  66. Harrell FE. Hmisc: Harrell miscellaneous. R package version 4.4.1 [Computer software]. 2020.

  67. Revelle W. psych: procedures for personality and psychological research. Evanston: Northwestern University; 2020.

    Google Scholar 

  68. Kaiser HF. An index of factorial simplicity. Psychometrika. 1974;39:31–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  69. Bartlett MS. The effect of standardization on a chi square approximation in factor analysis. Biometrika. 1951;38:337–44.

    Google Scholar 

  70. Harrell FE. rms: regression modeling strategies. R package version 6.0-1 [Computer software]. 2020.

  71. Nagelkerke N. A note on a general definition of the coefficient of determination. Biometrika. 1991;78:691–2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  72. Schmid CH, Griffith JL. Multivariate classification rules: calibration and discrimination. In: Armitage P, Colton T, editors. Encyclopedia of biostatistics. 2nd ed. Chichester: Wiley; 2005.

    Google Scholar 

  73. White I, Carlin J. Bias and efficiency of multiple imputation compared with complete-case analysis for missing covariate values. Stat Med. 2010;29:2920–31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Nassiri V, Lovik A, Molenberghs G, Verbeke G. On using multiple imputation for exploratory factor analysis of incomplete data. Behav Res Methods. 2018;50:501–17.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Box GEP, Tidwell PW. Transformation of the independent variables. Technometrics. 1962;4:531–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  76. Bairami Q, Shepherd SM, Burke L, Spivak B. Psychometric evaluation of a mental health help-seeking scale in a Muslim-Australian population. J Cross-cult Psychol (Manuscript submitted for publication).

  77. Saunders V, Roche S, McArthur M, Arney F, Ziaian T. Refugee communities intercultural dialogue: building relationships, building communities. Institute of Child Protection Studies, Australian Catholic University. 2015.

  78. Friedman HL. Culture and adolescent development. J Adolesc Health. 1999;25:1–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  79. Gelfand MJ, Harrington JR, Jackson JC. The strength of social norms across human groups. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2017;12:800–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. Arnett JJ. Adolescent psychology around the world. New York: Psychology Press; 2012.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  81. Chen X, Cen G, Li D, He Y. Social functioning and adjustment in Chinese children: the imprint of historical time. Child Dev. 2005;76:182–95.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  82. Renzaho AMN. Evaluation of the African migrant parenting program: a summary of the evaluation findings. 2009. http://hdl.handle.net/10536/DRO/DU:30032203. Accessed 10 Aug 2020

  83. Renzaho AMN, Dhingra N, Georgeou N. Youth as contested sites of culture: the intergenerational acculturation gap amongst new migrant communities—parental and young adult perspectives. PLoS ONE. 2017;12:e0170700.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  84. Deng SA. South Sudanese youth acculturation and intergenerational challenges. In: Proceedings of the 39th African Studies Association of Australasia and the Pacific, 5–7 December 2016. The University of Western Australia, pp. 199–210.

  85. Shepherd SM, Newton D, Farquharson K. Pathways to offending for young Sudanese Australians. Aust N Z J Criminol. 2017;51:481–501.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  86. Shepherd SM, Masuka G. Working with at-risk culturally and linguistically diverse young people in Australia—risk factors, programming and service delivery. Crim Justice Policy Rev. 2020. https://doi.org/10.1177/0887403420929416.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  87. Shepherd SM, Rose A, Madu M. Working with multi-cultural offenders in a clinical context. In: Brown JM, Horvath M, editors. The cambridge handbook of forensic psychology. 2nd ed. Cambridge University Press; 2020.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

This work was supported by a NHMRC Program Grant (ID: 566529). The funding body maintained no role or influence in the design of the study, data collection, analysis, interpretation of data and in the writing of the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

SS conceptualised the study and wrote the manuscript. CH assisted with study design, performed the statistical analyses and reviewed the manuscript. BS assisted with study design, assisted with statistical analyses and reviewed the manuscript. AP assisted with statistical analyses and reviewed the manuscript. RP provided study oversight and reviewed the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Stephane Shepherd.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The human research ethics committees at the University of Melbourne and the University of Sydney approved the study. Following assessment by a headspace Access Team clinician or completion of their first treatment session, prospective participants were contacted by telephone or in person by research assistants (RA) with a minimum four-year graduate psychology degree to discuss the aims and nature of the study and to determine their interest in participating. Participants aged 15 years and older provided written informed consent, whereas those aged 12–14 years assented and written informed consent was provided by a parent or guardian.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Shepherd, S., Harries, C., Spivak, B. et al. Exploring presentation differences in multi-cultural youth seeking assistance for mental health problems. BMC Psychol 9, 63 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-021-00571-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-021-00571-0

Keywords