Skip to main content

Savoring life during pandemic: an online intervention to promote well-being in emerging adults

Abstract

Background

Savoring, that is the ability to create and increase positive emotions, represents a promising approach to enhance subjective well-being (SWB) in emerging adults. This controlled study aims to investigate the preliminary effects of a self-help e-savoring intervention on increasing savoring beliefs and strategies and SWB in times of the COVID-19 pandemic.

Methods

Forty-nine emerging adult participants were recruited using the snowball sampling method. The experimental group (n = 23) completed six online exercises (two exercises per week for three weeks) while the control group (n = 26) did not receive the intervention. Both groups filled out online questionnaires before and after the intervention. User experience and perceived usefulness of the intervention were assessed for the experimental group.

Results

A repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) revealed a significant increase for the experimental group in savoring beliefs (especially toward the present and the future) and in positive emotions compared to the control group. The perspicuity, attractiveness, and efficiency of the online platform were very positively evaluated, and most participants rated the intervention as useful.

Conclusions

The results of this preliminary study together with the high level of adherence and the appreciation for the intervention indicate the potential of promoting online savoring and positive emotions in emerging adults. Future research could evaluate its long-term effects and verify its results with other age groups.

Peer Review reports

Background

The lives of people across the world have been severely impacted by the COVID-19 pandemic. Not only major health risks and economic uncertainty but also mental health challenges have been extensively documented [1]. Among the different populations that have been psychologically impacted by the COVID-19 crisis, emerging adults, such as individuals aged 18–30 years [2], who are already called on to face complex and uncertain challenges [3] (e.g., completing their education, leaving the parental home, finding a job, having a stable romantic relationship, and eventually having children), need to be seriously taken into consideration [4, 5].

COVID-19 containment measures such as social distancing, lockdowns, the closure of borders, businesses, and universities have been effective in slowing the spread of the virus and protecting physical health, but they have also limited emerging adults’ opportunities for independence, personal and professional growth, as well as interactions with family members, friends, and co-workers [6, 7]. In particular, all of these unexpected changes together with the impact of COVID itself (e.g., oneself or family members becoming seriously ill, or worries about it) have caused a burden of mental health conditions including anxiety, depression, irritability, reduced sleep quality, psychological distress over the uncertainty of the future, frustration, and loneliness [8,9,10,11,12,13], and have significantly decreased emerging adults’ subjective well-being (SWB) [14]. In fact, according to the ISTAT (Istituto Nazionale di Statistica) report of 2021, in Italy, both adolescents and emerging adults with ages ranging between 19 and 24 reported a decrease in SWB [15,16,17].

SWB is a combination of people’s overall evaluation of how satisfied they are with their lives and their affective responses to everyday events [18]. Several studies have shown that SWB, especially in the form of positive affect, is associated with protective psychological and behavioral factors such as greater social connection, greater perceived social support, more optimism, and a greater likelihood of engaging in healthy behaviors and correlates [18]. Indeed, SWB can foster better psychological health, and it is recognized as a critical factor in the prevention of depression [12, 19, 20]. Some uncontrollable life events and major unexpected changes, such as those associated with a pandemic, can undermine people's SWB, and for this reason developing interventions to support the subjective well-being of young adults in uncertain times is of relevant interest.

Savoring and subjective well-being

Among positive psychology interventions (PPIs), one promising approach that already demonstrated its positive impact on SWB is savoring. Savoring is the ability to generate, prolong, or intensify positive emotions, and it can increase awareness and recognition of the latter [21,22,23,24]. Therefore, the essence of savoring lies in the “conscious awareness of ongoing positive feelings” related to the experience [25]. While savoring, people become aware of ongoing positive feelings, and the intensity and duration of these feelings change.

Theoretically, savoring has been identified as a mechanism strictly connected to the well-known theories of positive psychology, namely the Broaden and Build Theory of Positive Emotions and the Undoing Hypothesis [26,27,28,29]. Indeed, savoring allows the generation, maintenance, and amplification of positive feelings and buffers against adverse health outcomes and negative emotions [29, 30]. Savoring can involve a form of mental time journey through which individuals can generate positive emotional states in the present thanks the focus on the here and now (present moment), the imagination of past (positive reminiscence) or future positive (positive anticipation) events [22, 28].

Research has shown that people may or may not be aware of their savoring skills (savoring beliefs). According to Bryant [31], “savoring beliefs involve a conscious awareness of one’s ability or inability to experience and manage positive experience.” On the one hand, the evidence suggests the presence of individual differences in savoring beliefs that are associated with personal traits such as extraversion, optimism, and emotional stability [24, 32, 33]. On the other hand, Bryant and Veroff [24] identified several savoring strategies that can amplify the intensity and duration of positive emotions. They are, for example, sharing with others, such as experiencing a positive event with a close friend; memory building, which is taking mental photographs of an event to remember it; self-congratulation, such as focusing on personal life achievements; sensory-perceptual sharpening, which is grasping every sensory aspect of a positive event; feeling absorbed in the moment; and using behavioral expressions such as laughing or jumping. Finally, other strategies include having a temporal awareness of the experience (for example, remembering that events end and must be fully experienced when they occur), and counting blessings such as thinking of small positive things of the day.

A meta-analytical review conducted by Smith et al. [34] found a substantial positive impact of savoring interventions on the intensity and duration of positive emotions and on well-being. Savoring outperforms other interventions such as optimism in terms of increasing positive affect, happiness, and life satisfaction while decreasing negative affect [35].

Savoring has been positively associated with well-being and negatively associated with depressive and anhedonia symptoms in both youth and adults [21, 28]. Until now, only a few savoring interventions for emerging adults have been proposed, and they have shown promising results. Bryant and Veroff [24] asked college students to notice as many pleasant aspects as possible of the environment around them (e.g. the flowers, the sun) during a 20-min walk. A week later, these students reported higher levels of happiness than those belonging to the neutral condition and the negative focus condition. Other researchers trained 94 college students to exercise the savoring strategies through recorded or written instructions, and, after 2 weeks, they found a significant reduction in depression and negative affect [36].

Online savoring

Recently, the broad approach of positive technology has recognized the positive role that technologies can have in promoting well-being by taking advantage of different strategies [37, 38]. Within this perspective, online positive psychology interventions (OPPIs) show several potentialities: they are accessible from different devices, they can reach different populations, and they are cost-effective [39,40,41,42]. Furthermore, OPPIs provide several advantages for emerging adults, as they are “digital natives” and use the Internet and apps daily for many activities and are favorable toward using self-help content [41, 43]. Digital psychological self-help interventions can be accessed from any location, thus overcoming logistical and geographical barriers to treatment delivery, and often resulting in more affordable and less time-consuming interventions in comparison to traditional interventions, with a subsequent significant improvement in self-monitoring processes [44,45,46].

Up to now, savoring has been integrated with other strategies in online interventions. This is the case with Heintzelman et al.’s [47, 48] 12-week in-person and online program for promoting SWB called "ENHANCE.". Participants, aged between 25 and 75, were asked to perform certain actions, including complimenting others, actively listening to other people, identifying and focusing on positive events, savoring, practicing mindfulness, and expressing gratitude. At the end of the intervention, participants belonging to both the in-person and online experimental groups reported higher levels of life satisfaction and positive affect and a reduction of negative affect, depressive symptoms, and perceived stress compared to the waiting list control group.

In other recent research, savoring interventions alone have been delivered and tested online. The pilot study by Park [49] aimed to teach the components of savoring through a website by providing several psychoeducational resources (articles, videos, and books) to promote the daily practice of savoring. The intervention lasted 21 days and showed the potential for an online self-managed savoring intervention to increase savoring and subjective well-being. However, only 22% of the initial participants (128) completed the intervention. Yu et al. [50] asked Taiwanese college students to perform a pleasant activity by paying attention to the positive emotions they felt for 20 min a day three times a week. Afterwards participants were asked to describe and post these emotions on a social network (e.g., Facebook) by using a photo and/or a text. The study found a significant increase in positive affect and decrease in depression in the experimental group in the post-test compared to the control condition. However, the decreased level of depression was not maintained in the follow-up.

The present study

When people are overwhelmed by negative events, they may be unable to notice or appreciate positive things; in fact, threats, distress, or distraction can inhibit people’s ability to savor positive experiences [25]. Being able to recognize and appreciate positive moments during challenging events can be a source of strength [29, 51,52,53]. Thus, savoring can represent a favorable approach in dealing with challenges such as the COVID-19 pandemic.

Starting from these premises and taking advantage of the potential of a self-help approach [54], we developed a self-help e-savoring intervention (Savor Project) and conducted a preliminary test of its benefits for emerging adult participants. We hypothesized that (1) people in the experimental group would more strongly believe in their ability to enjoy positive events (savoring beliefs) compared to the control group. Concerning SWB, we hypothesized that (2) participants in the experimental group would have a higher level of life satisfaction and positive affect and a lower level of negative affect than participants in the control group. We also explored whether the intervention would increase the likelihood of behaviors and/or thoughts that can amplify the intensity and duration of positive emotions (savoring strategies). In addition, we categorized the qualitative experiences written as comments by participants in terms of elicited emotions, bodily awareness, and content of the visualized experiences. Finally, we evaluated the user experience and the perceived usefulness of the intervention.

Methods

Participants

The participants were recruited using the snowball method thanks to posters published on social media and through word of mouth. The sample size of the feasibility study was calculated with G Power [55], and it was found that 46 participants would be needed to achieve 90% power with an average effect size (f = 0.25) with a repeated measurement design within/between interaction (number of groups 2; number of measurements: 2; correlation between repeated measurements: 0.5). In the current study, 55 emerging Italian adults were randomly assigned to the experimental and the control groups using block randomization. A label of A or B (A = intervention, B = control) was assigned to each group (block size = 4). Free online software (Research Randomizer 4.0) was used to generate the randomization list. Six people belonging to the experimental condition were excluded from the final sample because they did not complete all the activities, resulting in final sample sizes of 23 in the experimental condition and 26 in the control condition. The only inclusion criteria were age between 18 and 30 years, fluency in the Italian language, and having Internet access. All participants who met the above criteria and provided electronic informed consent were included in the sample.

Demographic characteristics, including age, gender, educational level, employment status, and marital status, were collected at baseline and reported in Table 1. Participants come from different regions of Northern Italy, such as Lombardy, Emilia-Romagna, Piemonte, and Veneto. The continuous variable (age) is reported as mean (M) and standard deviation (SD), whereas categorical variables (gender, education level, marital status, and employment status) are reported as frequencies and percentages. Differences between groups at baseline were analyzed with a Student t-test for age and a chi-square test for categorical data (Jamovi, Version 1.6.23.0) [56] and no significant differences emerged between groups (see Table 1).

Table 1 Participants’ characteristics

The study measures

Savoring beliefs and strategies were assessed both before and after the savoring intervention.

The Savoring Beliefs Inventory (SBI) was used to assess savoring beliefs [21]. Items from the original SBI were translated into Italian and then back translated by two independent translators. The outcome was discussed until all the translators agreed on reaching a consensus on cross-language equivalence. The SBI consists of 24 items that can be answered via a 7-point Likert scale ranging from 1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree. The SBI is composed of three subscales that, when added together, yield the total score (α = 0.89 for this study). The anticipation subscale (α = 0.74 for this study) measures how much a person believes he or she can savor through anticipation (e.g., “I feel a joy of anticipation when I think about upcoming good things”). The present subscale (α = 0.89 for this study) assesses how much the participant believes he or she can savor the present moment (e.g., “I know how to make the most of a good time”). The reminiscence subscale (α = 0.78 for this study) assesses how much the participant is perceived to be able to savor a memory (e.g., “I enjoy looking back on happy times from my past”). High scores on these subscales and the total scale indicate a greater perceived ability to savor positive events.

An adapted short version of the Ways of Savoring Checklist (WOSC) was used to assess savoring strategies [24]. The original WOSC was translated into Italian and then back translated by two independent translators. The outcome was discussed until all the translators agreed on reaching a consensus on cross-language equivalence. The original version is made up of ten sub-scales and a total of 60 items that can be responded to using a 7-point Likert scale from 1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree. We used an adapted short version of 14 items in this study, focusing only on a few amplifying strategies (α = 0.62 for this study), such as sharing with others (e.g., “I looked for people to share it with”), self-congratulation (e.g., “I told myself why I deserved this good thing”), absorption (e.g., “I thought only about the present—got absorbed in the moment”), and counting blessings (e.g., “I thought about what a lucky person I am that so many good things have happened to me”). Both before and after the intervention, we assessed both dimensions of subjective well-being: life satisfaction and negative and positive affect.

The Italian version of the Satisfaction With Life Scale (SWLS) was used to assess the cognitive dimension of subjective well-being and global life satisfaction (five items with responses on a 7-point Likert scale ranging from 1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree, (e.g., “In most ways my life is close to my ideal”) [57, 58]. The sum of all items is used to calculate the score, and high scores indicate high levels of life satisfaction (α = 0.84 for this study).

The Italian version of the Scale of Positive and Negative Experiences (SPANE) was used to measure positive and negative affect [59, 60]. This self-report scale consists of 12 items and is composed of two subscales: the positive affect subscale (6 items: positive, good, pleasant, happy, joyful, contented) (SPANE-P, α = 0.85 for this study) and the second negative affect subscale (6 items: negative, bad, unpleasant, sad, afraid, angry) (SPANE-N, α = 0.76 for this study). For all items, the response scale was a 5-point Likert scale (1 = very rarely or never and 5 = very often or always). The sum of positive and negative emotions can be used to calculate the score.

Furthermore, at the end of the first five activities, we asked participants to write down their thoughts about the exercise they had just performed. This was done after the fifth rather than the last activity because, at this point, participants were told that they would be sent a certificate of participation and access to the website for a full year as a gift for their participation. In this case, self-report measures would not have been appropriate: delivering gifts and then asking participants how they felt could in fact make them suspicious of the researchers’ intentions to manipulate their mood, and this, in turn, could dissipate the positive emotion induced [61, 62].

Finally, at the end of the intervention, the User Experience Questionnaire (UEQ) was used to investigate the participants’ experiences in using the online platform and doing the savoring exercises [63]. It contains six scales and is made up of 26 total items: attractiveness (α = 0.83 for this study) refers to the general impression with respect to the product (e.g., annoying—enjoyable), efficiency (α = 0.58 for this study) focuses instead on the organization and practicality of the website (e.g., impractical—practical), and perspicuity (α = 0.74 for this study) refers to the perceived ease of use (e.g., not understandable—understandable). Furthermore, stimulation (α = 0.85 for this study) refers to the interest and enthusiasm generated by its use (e.g., boring—exciting), and novelty (α = 0.81 for this study) focuses on its innovation and creativity (e.g., conservative—innovative). We have not included dependability, which focuses on how secure and predictable the interaction with the site was, because the internal consistency of the items in the scale appeared unacceptable (α < 0.5). The UEQ response modality is characterized by semantic differentials, and each item can be assigned a score between -3 and + 3, where -3 represents the most negative answer, 0 a neutral answer, and + 3 the most positive answer. For each scale, values between -0.8 and 0.8 represent a neutral evaluation, scores greater than 0.8 indicate a positive evaluation, and scores lower than -0.8 indicate a negative evaluation, while values near + 2 represent a very positive, near-optimal impression of participants [64].

A final item, with responses made on a 5-step Likert scale (1 = very little and 5 = very much), was used to assess the perceived usefulness of the intervention (“Referring to the entire intervention, how much do you think it was useful for you?”). Finally, participants made comments about the whole intervention, as previously mentioned.

E-savoring intervention

The entire intervention was designed and developed exclusively online to reach emerging adults in total safety during the COVID-19 pandemic. An ad hoc website was therefore designed and created via the WordPress platform (at the domain www.progettosavor.it), accessible from any device (smartphone, tablet, or computer) with an Android, iOS, Windows, or macOS operating system. The goal was to create a website with simple graphics that was intuitive and usable. Participants accessed the site with a username and password, and, once participants entered the website, they could access the online questionnaires (through a link to Qualtrics) and the intervention exercises (Table 2). Specifically, participants had to complete 2 exercises a week for this 3-week intervention, for a total of 6 exercises. Participants were emailed a reminder when each exercise was published on the website. Each exercise took about 10 min to complete, and participants were given the opportunity to access the exercise in either recorded audio or written text format, to give participants the chance to choose their favorite modality (Fig. 1).

Table 2 Savoring activities
Fig. 1
figure 1

Online intervention: home page and example of activity

Procedure

The study was conducted from October 2021 to February 2022, during the fourth wave of the COVID-19 pandemic in Italy. This pilot randomized controlled study used a between-subjects design and included two assessment moments. An email was sent to each participant interested in participating that provided information regarding the intervention. Personal credentials for accessing the website were sent to the experimental group participants. Participants were then invited to fill out informed consent forms and online baseline questionnaires—the SBI, WOSC Amplifying, SWLS, and SPANE (T0). The e-savoring intervention took place during the following 3 weeks. At the end of the intervention (T1), participants completed the same questionnaires—SBI, WOSC Amplifying, SWLS, and SPANE—with the integration of the UEQ and usefulness ad hoc questions only for the experimental group. The control group didn’t carry out any activity and completed only the pre- and post-intervention questionnaires in the same time frame as the experimental group. At T1, the control group received by email the credentials for accessing the e-savoring intervention for one year.

Data analyses

All statistical analyses were conducted using Jamovi (Version 1.6.23.0) [56]. Group differences regarding psychological dimensions (savoring beliefs and strategies and subjective well-being) were examined at baseline using the Student t-test for independent samples. The intervention’s effect was examined through a repeated measures ANOVA with time as a within-subjects factor and group as a between-subjects factor. We were interested in the time x group interaction, which would indicate a differential change due to the intervention.

Qualitative data were analyzed thematically [68]. A thematic analysis identifies, analyses and reports patterns of meaning (themes) across the dataset, in order to answer the research question. This approach enabled concise analysis of a large amount of textual data by using a deductive approach in which unexpected themes could arise “bottom-up” from the data. Relevant parts of the data were systematically identified, before being coded into potential themes that were refined, defined, and named. Finally, a narrative analysis of the themes was conducted.

Results

Before analyzing effects related to the e-savoring intervention, we compared the psychological dimensions at baseline and no differences emerged between groups (Table 3).

Table 3 Psychological dimensions at baseline

Effectiveness of the e-savoring intervention: preliminary results

A repeated measures ANOVA with time as a within-subjects factor and group as a between-subjects factor was performed for all variables (Table 4). We were interested in the time x group interaction, which would indicate a differential change due to the intervention. Descriptive data are shown as mean (M) and standard deviation (SD).

Table 4 Comparison between groups: repeated measures ANOVA interactions effects

Concerning the effects of the intervention on savoring beliefs (SBI), a significant group x time interaction was found. For the experimental group, there was a significant greater increase in savoring beliefs (total score) compared to baseline, and in particular in savoring the present (p < 0.05) and the future (p < 0.01), than in the control group, while no significant effect related to reminiscence emerged. Concerning the subjective well-being assessment, we did not find a significant change related to its cognitive dimension, such as life satisfaction (SWLS), but we found a significant change related to its affective dimension (p < 0.05), in particular for the positive emotions (SPANE-P): the experimental group showed a significantly greater increase in the positive emotions compared to the control group, while no significant changes about the negative emotions (SPANE-N) were found. Moreover, the analysis of scores obtained by the participants in the adoption of savoring strategies (WOSC Amplifying), including sharing with others, self-congratulation, absorption, and counting blessings, showed a tendency toward a significant increase (p = 0.061) in the experimental group compared to the control group.

Qualitative analysis of visualized experiences

Qualitative data were thematically analyzed in order to explore the research question: What were participants' emotional experiences activated by the savoring exercises? There was no missing data, because the online questionnaire required participants to provide qualitative comments about each activity performed. Two principal themes were identified: a) activated positive emotions [69, 70] and experienced bodily awareness, and b) the content of the visualized experience. As far as the content is concerned, we considered the social dimension (shared experiences, related to activities performed together with others such as friends, relatives, partner, etc. vs. individual activities that take place alone), the scenario (natural environments referred to outdoor natural places vs artificial environments referred to indoor places such as offices, homes, gyms, etc.), and the performed activity (occupational activities, such as work and/or study, vs. recreational activities, such as reading, walking, and other leisure activities). Details are reported in Table 5.

Table 5 Qualitative evaluation of the visualized experiences: principal themes (number of participants)

The first activity, consisting of asking participants to list three happy moments, led participants to recall both shared (65%) and individual experiences (48%), and mostly related to occupational activities (43%). These memories elicited positive emotions, and in particular pride and satisfaction (39%) and happiness (30%). The second activity, consisting of recalling and amplifying one positive moment, led participants to recall both shared and individual experiences (both 35%) and related to both occupational and recreational activities (22% and 26%, respectively). This activity mostly elicited joy (52%) and boosted bodily awareness (43%). The third activity, asking participants to identify three daily things to be grateful for, led them to visualize mostly shared experiences (83%) related to recreational activities (52%) and also elicited gratitude (56%) and happiness (35%). The fourth activity, which asked participants to be fully involved in the enjoyable activity they were doing, led participants to visualize mostly individual experiences (96%) performed in a natural environment (35%) during recreational time (96%). This activity elicited serenity (52%) and pride and satisfaction (30%). The fifth activity, asking participants to imagine at least two positive events that could happen in the next few days and pay attention to the emotions aroused by the imagined events, led participants to anticipate both shared (91%) and individual experiences (56%), mainly related to natural environments (30%) and recreational activities (91%), which mostly elicited happiness (43%), enthusiasm (35%), and gratitude (30%).

User experience and perceived usefulness

Furthermore, at the end of the e-savoring intervention, participants filled out the User Experience Questionnaire. Participants reported a very positive evaluation of the system in terms of perspicuity, attractiveness, and efficiency; stimulation and novelty were also positively evaluated. Furthermore, participants evaluated the intervention as useful (the average rating is between "quite useful" and "very useful," median = 4). Details are reported in Table 6.

Table 6 E-savoring intervention evaluation: user experience

Regarding perceived usefulness, a qualitative analysis was also conducted. A descriptive analysis of the participants’ comments about the overall evaluation of the intervention revealed some key areas: 15 out of 23 participants left a written comment; 5 out of 15 participants (33%) reported increased awareness of positive experiences and associated feelings. For example, a 24-year-old woman stated, “This path has been a path of inner reflection that helps you reflect on your reactions to the moments of your life. […] For me, it was interesting and enjoyable to take this type of path. It was useful to stop and think and to understand yourself a little more.” Furthermore, 7 out of 15 participants (47%) reported that they had learned strategies to boost positive emotions. For instance, a 26-year-old woman asserted, “I found this to be a very clear method of learning a simple but sometimes difficult concept without input. It gave me the opportunity to reflect, and stop, and think about the good things, which exist and are considerably more common than the bad ones. I will treasure these exercises, especially in times when everything seems difficult. Thank you.” Moreover, 8 out of 15 participants (53%) expressed their intention to put into practice the strategies they learned in the future. For example, a 23-year-old woman reported, “It helped me especially in reliving the positive emotions of past events. I was really savoring all the emotions I felt. In the second activity, I relived the happy moment in my mind, and it really felt like I was there, and I felt a lot of the happiness I felt in that event. I had never tried to do such a thing; I had never been able to savor so well the feelings from a past event. […] I've found that I can relive those emotions whenever I want thanks to this activity! I think I will take all the activities of this project with me for the rest of my life. This project […] has been a great practical help! Thank you!".

Discussion

The COVID-19 pandemic has proven to be a challenging time for emerging adults who reported a worrying decline in mental health and subjective well-being [5, 71]. Starting from these premises, the present study developed and investigated the effects of a self-help e-savoring intervention in improving savoring beliefs and strategies and the subjective well-being of emerging adults.

Findings partially confirmed our hypotheses. Concerning the effectiveness of the intervention on increasing savoring beliefs (H1), participants reported significant improvements and this result is in line with other studies reporting an increase in total SBI levels [49]. Participants felt more able to savor both upcoming positive events and the present moment. Participants showed a slight but not significant improvement in reminiscence beliefs. These results are in line with Bryant’s [21] statement that people typically consider themselves better at savoring through reminiscence. Furthermore, before the intervention (baseline), reminiscence levels were higher than at the other times; thus, participants already felt competent to savor memories and positive past experiences.

Concerning the intervention’s effect on SWB, the second hypothesis (H2) was partially confirmed. On the one hand, there was not a significant increase in life satisfaction, probably due to the short duration of the intervention. The brevity of this intervention (6 activities in 3 weeks) allowed us to achieve a very good adherence rate but did not sustain a change in the global cognitive assessment of one's life. More time is needed to achieve a sustained change—which was reported in other studies that had longer-lasting interventions (e.g., 12 weeks) [35, 57]. In addition, other studies that integrated different positive psychology strategies (e.g., mindfulness, gratitude, optimism, strengths, etc.) or more savoring strategies found increases in life satisfaction as well as other positive psychological outcomes [47, 72, 73]. On the other hand, the intervention was effective in increasing positive emotions. The opportunity to write comments about the performed savoring activities may have stimulated participants' reflection. Indeed, writing about positive experiences can promote the building of a logical narrative, which in turn fosters the maintenance of positive emotions [50]. In line with the study carried out by Yu et al. [50], we did not find a change in negative emotions. It is important to highlight that savoring does not exclusively include focusing on the positive aspects of life while ignoring the negative ones. Indeed, the benefits of savoring are visible in the ability to notice and appreciate positive moments despite and during life difficulties [25, 74]. Thus, in our study, we cannot exclude the possibility that some negative emotions related to the challenging situation of the pandemic affected participants’ emotional states.

Regarding the effectiveness of the intervention in increasing savoring strategies as a research question, participants reported a tendency toward a significant improvement. Interestingly, after a 3-week intervention, a change in participants’ savoring behaviors was triggered. Nevertheless, it is not possible to compare this result with those of previous studies because, to our knowledge, no savoring interventions have investigated the effects on savoring strategies [75, 76]. Future studies that assess this dimension are needed.

Qualitative analyses revealed that participants experienced several positive emotions during the visualized events, such as happiness, joy, pride, gratitude, and serenity. While they were more absorbed by individual activities in savoring the present moment, they were oriented toward savoring relational activities in the future. This finding highlights the importance of encouraging positive moments with family, friends, and partners after social distancing ends. Recreational visualizations prevailed over work activities. Due to the importance and need of promoting well-being and positive emotions in the workplace, savoring interventions could be adapted to this context [77].

Regarding the user experience, the website was perceived as easy to use. Indeed, it was designed and created specifically for this intervention, and all functions were intuitive. In addition, most participants perceived the whole intervention as useful, as the savoring activities elicited positive emotions and reflections.

Taking into account the present study’s findings as a whole, it seems that the online savoring intervention represents an effective protocol to sustain emerging adults’ subjective perception of being able to intensify positive emotions related to the present and the future and to boost positive affect during the COVID-19 pandemic. The present findings are consistent with the positive outcomes reported by other previous online savoring interventions based on psychoeducational resources or proposing different strategies [49, 50].

Limitations and strengths

The current study focuses on the development, implementation, and preliminary evaluation of a new online savoring intervention with emerging adults during the COVID-19 pandemic, leaving the door open for future research to address its limitations and refine its findings.

First, we included a control group that did not engage in any activity other than completing the assessments at the two times, and, according to Kraiss et al. [78], this issue could have an impact on the evaluation of intervention effectiveness. This limitation requires us to view the results with caution and encourage future studies to test the intervention with active control groups. Second, because we used convenience and snowball sampling methods, we have to recognize that this choice could have an influence on the results’ generalizability. Third, we used savoring scales not validated in Italian. On one hand, the ad hoc version of the WOSC was not fully reliable; on the other hand, even if the SBI had not previously been validated in Italian, the reliability of the subscales was good. Furthermore, these results can be considered preliminary and only partially generalizable, as they involve a small sample of emerging adults from Northern Italy.

Despite these limitations, we can recognize some strengths of this study. One is the implementation of a self-help savoring intervention, solidly anchored in some of the amplification strategies proposed by Bryant and Veroff [24]. Furthermore, the online implementation of the intervention can be viewed as one of its strengths, as it allowed participation of emerging adults from different regions during a time when fear of contagion was still high and young people struggled to resume the normal activities they practiced before the pandemic. The ability to amplify positive emotional experiences anchored in one's personal history allows one to feel highly engaged in the activities, and the ease of the exercises fits well with the self-help approach.

Finally, the high adherence rate represents another strength of the current study, probably sustained by engaging activities proposed in a short intervention. In other cases, extended online savoring interventions had a high dropout rate due to the significant effort and time required [49].

Conclusions and implications

The online self-help e-savoring intervention showed great potential for promoting well-being and savoring in the emerging adult population during the COVID-19 pandemic. The high level of adherence and the appreciation for the intervention led us to speculate that it could be adapted and tested with a large sample of participants of various ages. Furthermore, future studies are encouraged in assessing the maintenance of achieved results with a follow-up evaluation. Finally, given the increase in mental disorders such as anxiety and depression in emerging adults as a result of the COVID-19 pandemic, as well as recent research on the role of savoring in reducing depression, it might be worthwhile to apply this intervention to this population as well as others affected by these mental health conditions [79].

Availability of data and materials

The dataset used and analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

SWB:

Subjective Well-Being

ANOVA:

Analysis of Variance

ISTAT:

Istituto Nazionale di Statistica

PPIs:

Positive Psychology Interventions

OPPIs:

Online Positive Psychology Interventions

M:

Mean

SD:

Standard Deviation

SBI:

Savoring Beliefs Inventory

WOSC:

Ways of Savoring Checklist

SWLS:

Satisfaction With Life Scale

SPANE:

Scale of Positive and Negative Experiences

UEQ:

User Experience Questionnaire

References

  1. Vindegaard N, Benros ME. COVID-19 pandemic and mental health consequences: Systematic review of the current evidence. Brain Behav Immun. 2020;89:531–42.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. Arnett JJ, Žukauskienė R, Sugimura K. The new life stage of emerging adulthood at ages 18–29 years: implications for mental health. Lancet Psychiatry. 2014;1(7):569–76. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(14)00080-7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Arnett JJ. Emerging adulthood: a theory of development from the late teens through the twenties. Am Psychol. 2000;55(5):469–80. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066x.55.5.469.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Gruber J, Prinstein MJ, Clark LA, Rottenberg J, Abramowitz JS, Albano AM, et al. Mental health and clinical psychological science in the time of COVID-19: Challenges, opportunities, and a call to action. Am Psychol. 2021;76(3):40926. https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0000707.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Hawes MT, Szenczy AK, Klein DN, Hajcak G, Nelson BD. Increases in depression and anxiety symptoms in adolescents and young adults during the COVID-19 pandemic. Psychol Med. 2022;52(14):3222–30. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291720005358.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Nussbaumer-Streit B, Mayr V, Dobrescu AI, Chapman A, Persad E, Klerings I, et al. Quarantine alone or in combination with other public health measures to control COVID-19: a rapid review. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2020;9(9):CD013574. https://doi.org/10.1002/14651858.CD013574.pub2.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Sibley CG, Greaves LM, Satherley N, Wilson MS, Overall NC, Lee CHJ, et al. Effects of the COVID-19 pandemic and nationwide lockdown on trust, attitudes toward government, and well-being. Am Psychol. 2020;75(5):618–30. https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0000662.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Asmundson GJG, Taylor S. How health anxiety influences responses to viral outbreaks like COVID-19: What all decision-makers, health authorities, and health care professionals need to know. J Anxiety Disord. 2020;71(102211):102211. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2020.102211.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Graziano S, Boldrini F, Righelli D, Milo F, Lucidi V, Quittner A, et al. Psychological interventions during COVID pandemic: Telehealth for individuals with cystic fibrosis and caregivers. Pediatr Pulmonol. 2021;56(7):1976–84. https://doi.org/10.1002/ppul.25413.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Sheroun D, Wankhar DD, Devrani A, Lissamma PV, Chatterjee K. A study to assess the perceived stress and coping strategies among B. Sc. nursing students of selected colleges in Pune during COVID-19 pandemic lockdown. Int J Sci Healthc Res. 2020;5(2):280–8.

    Google Scholar 

  11. Aslan H, Pekince H. Nursing students’ views on the COVID-19 pandemic and their perceived stress levels. Perspect Psychiatr Care. 2021;57(2):695–701.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Genç E, Arslan G. Optimism and dispositional hope to promote college students’ subjective well-being in the context of the COVID-19 pandemic. J Posit Sch Psychol. 2021;5(2):87–96. https://doi.org/10.47602/jpsp.v5i2.255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Zammitti A, Imbrogliera C, Russo A, Zarbo R, Magnano P. The psychological Impact of Coronavirus pandemic restrictions in Italy. The mediating role of the fear of COVID-19 in the relationship between positive and negative affect with positive and negative outcomes. Eur J Investig Health Psychol Educ. 2021;11(3):697–710. https://doi.org/10.3390/ejihpe11030050.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Diener E, Lucas RE, Oishi S. Subjective well-being: The science of happiness and life satisfaction. Handbook Posit Psychol. 2002;2:63–73.

    Google Scholar 

  15. Moksnes UK, Eilertsen MEB, Ringdal R, Bjørnsen HN, Rannestad T. Life satisfaction in association with self-efficacy and stressor experience in adolescents - self-efficacy as a potential moderator. Scand J Caring Sci. 2019;33(1):222–30. https://doi.org/10.1111/scs.12624.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Zheng Y, Zhou Z, Liu Q, Yang X, Fan C. Perceived stress and life satisfaction: A multiple mediation model of self-control and rumination. J Child Fam Stud. 2019;28(11):3091–7. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-019-01486-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. ISTAT. Rapporto Bes 2021: il benessere equo e sostenibile in Italia. Benessere soggettivo. 2021. Retrieved from https://www.istat.it/it/files//2022/04/8.pdf

  18. Diener E, Chan MY. Happy people live longer: Subjective well-being contributes to health and longevity: Health benefits of happiness. Appl Psychol Health Well Being. 2011;3(1):1–43. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1758-0854.2010.01045.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Kansky J, Diener E. Benefits of well-being: Health, social relationships, work, and resilience. J Posit Psychol Wellbeing. 2017;1(2):129–69.

    Google Scholar 

  20. Arslan G, Yıldırım M, Karataş Z, Kabasakal Z, Kılınç M. Meaningful living to promote complete mental health among university students in the context of the COVID-19 pandemic. Int J Ment Health Addict. 2022;20(2):930–42. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-020-00416-8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Bryant F. Savoring Beliefs Inventory (SBI): A scale for measuring beliefs about savouring. J Ment Health. 2003;12(2):175–96. https://doi.org/10.1080/0963823031000103489.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Bryant FB, Chadwick ED, Kluwe K. Understanding the processes that regulate positive emotional experience: Unsolved problems and future directions for theory and research on savoring. Int J Wellbeing. 2011;1(1). https://doi.org/10.5502/ijw.v1i1.18

  23. Croft A, Dunn EW, Quoidbach J. From tribulations to appreciation: Experiencing adversity in the past predicts greater savoring in the present. Psychol Sci. 2014;5:511–6.

    Google Scholar 

  24. Bryant FB, Veroff J. Savoring: A new model of positive experience. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2007.

    Google Scholar 

  25. Smith JL, Bryant FB. Savoring and well-being: Mapping the cognitive-emotional terrain of the happy mind. In: The Happy Mind: Cognitive Contributions to Well-Being. Cham: Springer International Publishing; 2017. p. 139–56.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  26. Fredrickson BL. What good are positive emotions? Rev Gen Psychol. 1998;2(3):300–19. https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.300.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Fredrickson BL. The value of positive emotions: The emerging science of positive psychology is coming to understand why it’s good to feel good. Am Sci. 2003;91(4):330–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Quoidbach J, Berry EV, Hansenne M, Mikolajczak M. Positive emotion regulation and well-being: Comparing the impact of eight savoring and dampening strategies. Pers Individ Dif. 2010;49(5):368–73. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.03.048.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Smith JL, Bihary JG, O’Connor D, Basic A, O’Brien CJ. Impact of savoring ability on the relationship between older adults’ activity engagement and well-being. J Appl Gerontol. 2020;39(3):323–31. https://doi.org/10.1177/0733464819871876.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. van Steenbergen H, de Bruijn ERA, van Duijvenvoorde ACK, van Harmelen A-L. How positive affect buffers stress responses. Curr Opin Behav Sci. 2021;39:153–60. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2021.03.014.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Bryant FB. Current progress and future directions for theory and research on savoring. Front Psychol. 2021;12:771698. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.771698.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Palmer CA. Experimentally-Induced Savoring and its Impact on Positive Emotions, Cognitions, and Behaviors: Investigating Individual Differences in Effectiveness. 2014.

    Google Scholar 

  33. Smith JL, Hollinger-Smith L. Savoring, resilience, and psychological well-being in older adults. Aging Ment Health. 2015;19(3):192–200.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Smith JL, Harrison PR, Kurtz JL, Bryant FB. Nurturing the capacity to savor: Interventions to enhance the enjoyment of positive experiences. In: Parks AC, Shueller SM, editors. The Wiley Blackwell handbook of positive psychological interventions. Oxford: Wiley Blackwell; 2014. p 42–65.

  35. Salces-Cubero IM, Ramírez-Fernández E, Ortega-Martínez AR. Strengths in older adults: differential effect of savoring, gratitude and optimism on well-being. Aging Ment Health. 2019;23(8):1017–24. https://doi.org/10.1080/13607863.2018.1471585.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Hurley DB, Kwon P. Results of a study to increase savoring the moment: Differential impact on positive and negative outcomes. J Happiness Stud. 2012;13(4):579–88. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-011-9280-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Riva G, Baños RM, Botella C, Wiederhold BK, Gaggioli A. Positive technology: using interactive technologies to promote positive functioning. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. 2012;15(2):69–77. https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2011.0139.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Riva G, Villani D, Cipresso P, Gaggioli A. Positive technology: The use of technology for improving and sustaining personal change. In: Villani D, Cipresso P, Gaggioli A, Riva G, editors. Integrating technology in positive psychology practice. Hershey: IGI Global; 2016. p 1–37.

  39. Bolier L, Abello KM. Online positive psychological interventions: State of the art and future directions. 2014.

    Google Scholar 

  40. Sitbon A, Shankland R, Krumm C-M. Interventions efficaces en psychologie positive: Une revue systématique. Can Psychol. 2019;60(1):35–54. https://doi.org/10.1037/cap0000163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Krifa I, Hallez Q, van Zyl LE, Braham A, Sahli J, Ben Nasr S, et al. Effectiveness of an online positive psychology intervention among Tunisian healthcare students on mental health and study engagement during the Covid-19 pandemic. Appl Psychol Health Well Being. 2022;14(4):1228–54. https://doi.org/10.1111/aphw.12332.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Datu JAD, Valdez JPM, McInerney DM, Cayubit RF. The effects of gratitude and kindness on life satisfaction, positive emotions, negative emotions, and COVID-19 anxiety: An online pilot experimental study. Appl Psychol Health Well Being. 2022;14(2):347–61. https://doi.org/10.1111/aphw.12306.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Davies EB, Morriss R, Glazebrook C. Computer-delivered and web-based interventions to improve depression, anxiety, and psychological well-being of university students: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Med Internet Res. 2014;16(5):e130. https://doi.org/10.2196/jmir.3142.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  44. Fleming T, Bavin L, Lucassen M, Stasiak K, Hopkins S, Merry S. Beyond the trial: Systematic review of real-world uptake and engagement with digital self-help interventions for depression, low mood, or anxiety. J Med Internet Res. 2018;20(6):e199. https://doi.org/10.2196/jmir.9275.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  45. Riva G, Bernardelli L, Castelnuovo G, Di Lernia D, Tuena C, Clementi A, et al. A virtual reality-based self-help intervention for dealing with the psychological distress associated with the COVID-19 lockdown: An effectiveness study with a two-week follow-up. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2021;18(15):8188. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18158188.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  46. Villani D, Grassi A, Cognetta C, Toniolo D, Cipresso P, Riva G. Self-help stress management training through mobile phones: An experience with oncology nurses. Psychol Serv. 2013;10:315–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Heintzelman SJ, Kushlev K, Lutes LD, Wirtz D, Kanippayoor JM, Leitner D, et al. ENHANCE: Evidence for the efficacy of a comprehensive intervention program to promote subjective well-being. J Exp Psychol Appl. 2020;26(2):360–83. https://doi.org/10.1037/xap0000254.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Diener E, Biswas-Diener R. . Well-being interventions to improve societies. Global Happiness Council, Global Happiness and Well-being Policy Report. 2019. p. 95–110.

    Google Scholar 

  49. Park SY. Cultivating the skill of savoring: an internet-delivered intervention to promote well-Being. 2019.

    Google Scholar 

  50. Yu S-C, Sheldon KM, Lan W-P, Chen J-H. Using social network sites to boost savoring: Positive effects on positive emotions. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2020;17(17):6407. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17176407.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  51. Bryant FB, Smith JL. Appreciating life in the midst of adversity: Savoring in relation to mindfulness, reappraisal, and meaning. Psychol Inq. 2015;26(4):315–21. https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840x.2015.1075351.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Borelli JL, Rasmussen HF, Burkhart ML, Sbarra DA. Relational savoring in long-distance romantic relationships. J Soc Pers Relat. 2015;32(8):1083–108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Jiao J, Kim S, Pitts MJ. Promoting subjective well-being through communication savoring. Commun Q. 2021;1–20. https://doi.org/10.1080/01463373.2021.1901758

  54. Fu W, Yan S, Zong Q, Anderson-Luxford D, Song X, Lv Z, et al. Mental health of college students during the COVID-19 epidemic in China. J Affect Disord. 2021;280(Pt A):7–10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2020.11.032.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Kang H. Sample size determination and power analysis using the G*Power software. J Educ Eval Health Prof. 2021;18:17. https://doi.org/10.3352/jeehp.2021.18.17.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  56. Jamovi - open statistical software for the desktop and cloud . Jamovi.org. https://www.jamovi.org. [Cited 2023 Feb 14]

  57. Diener E, Suh EM, Lucas RE, Smith HL. Subjective well-being: Three decades of progress. Psychol Bull. 1999;125(2):276–302. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.125.2.276.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Fabio D, Busoni A. Proprietà psicometriche della versione italiana della Satisfaction With Life Scale (SWLS) con studenti universitari. In: iornale Italiano di Ricerca e Applicazioni. 2009.

    Google Scholar 

  59. Diener E, Wirtz D, Tov W, Kim-Prieto C, Choi D-W, Oishi S, et al. New well-being measures: Short scales to assess flourishing and positive and negative feelings. Soc Indic Res. 2010;97(2):143–56. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-009-9493-y.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Corno G, Molinari G, Baños RM. Assessing positive and negative experiences: validation of a new measure of well-being in an Italian population. Riv Psichiatr. 2016;51(3):110–5. https://doi.org/10.1708/2304.24796.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Isen AM. Some ways in which positive affect influences decision making and problem solving. Handbook Emot. 2008;3:548–73.

    Google Scholar 

  62. Yang H, Yang S, Isen AM. Positive affect improves working memory: implications for controlled cognitive processing. Cogn Emot. 2013;27(3):474–82. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2012.713325.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Laugwitz B, Held T, Schrepp M. Construction and evaluation of a user experience questionnaire. In: Lecture Notes in Computer Science. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer Berlin Heidelberg; 2008. p. 63–76.

    Google Scholar 

  64. Rauschenberger M, Schrepp M, Cota MP, Olschner S, Thomaschewski J. Efficient measurement of the user experience of interactive products. How to use the User. Experience Questionnaire (UEQ) Example: Spanish language version. Int J Interact Mult Artif Intell. 2013;2(1):39–46.

    Google Scholar 

  65. Lyubomirsky S, Sousa L, Dickerhoof R. The costs and benefits of writing, talking, and thinking about life’s triumphs and defeats. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2006;90(4):692–708. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.90.4.692.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Eamons B, Mc Cullough ME. Counting blessings versus burdens: An experimental investigation of gratitude and subjective well-being in daily life. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2003;84(2):377–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  67. Quoidbach J, Wood AM, Hansenne M. Back to the future: the effect of daily practice of mental time travel into the future on happiness and anxiety. J Posit Psychol. 2009;4(5):349–55. https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760902992365.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  68. Braun V, Clarke V. Using thematic analysis in psychology. Qual Res Psychol. 2006;3(2):77–101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  69. Shiota MN, Neufeld SL, Yeung WH, Moser SE, Perea EF. Feeling good: autonomic nervous system responding in five positive emotions. Emotion. 2011;11(6):1368–78. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024278.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Fredrickson BL. Positive emotions broaden and build. In: Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, eds Devine, Plant, Vol. 47. Academic Press, Elsevier; 2013. p 1–53. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-407236-7.00001-2.

  71. Graupensperger S, Calhoun BH, Patrick ME, Lee CM. Longitudinal effects of COVID-19-related stressors on young adults’ mental health and wellbeing. Appl Psychol Health Well Being. 2022;14(3):734–56. https://doi.org/10.1111/aphw.12344.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Brouzos A, Vassilopoulos SP, Baourda VC, Tassi C, Stavrou V, Moschou K, et al. “Staying Home-Feeling Positive”: Effectiveness of an on-line positive psychology group intervention during the COVID-19 pandemic. Curr Psychol. 2021;42:1–13.

  73. Brouzos A, Vassilopoulos SP, Romosiou V, Stavrou V, Tassi C, Baourda VC, et al. ‘Stay safe-feel positive’ on the frontline: an online positive psychology intervention for police officers during the COVID-19 pandemic. J Posit Psychol. 2022;17:898–908.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  74. Smith JL, Bryant FB. Enhancing positive perceptions of aging by savoring life lessons. Aging Ment Health. 2019;23(6):762–70. https://doi.org/10.1080/13607863.2018.1450840.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Smith JL, Bryant FB. Are we having fun yet? Savoring, Type A behavior, and vacation enjoyment. Int J Wellbeing. 2013;3(1):1–19. https://doi.org/10.5502/ijw.v3i1.1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  76. Chadwick ED, Jose PE, Bryant FB. Styles of everyday savoring differentially predict well-being in adolescents over one month. J Happiness Stud. 2021;22(2):803–24. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-020-00252-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  77. Fritz C, Taylor MR. Taking in the good: How to facilitate savoring in work organizations. Bus Horiz. 2022;65(2):139–48. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bushor.2021.02.035.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  78. Kraiss J, Redelinghuys K, Weiss LA. The effects of psychological interventions on well-being measured with the Mental Health Continuum: a meta-analysis. J Happiness Stud. 2022;23(7):3655–89. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-022-00545-y.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  79. Irvin KM, Bell DJ, Steinley D, Bartholow BD. The thrill of victory: Savoring positive affect, psychophysiological reward processing, and symptoms of depression. Emotion. 2022;22(6):1281–93. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000914.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

Not applicable.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

D.V., E.P, F.P., L.S, created the first draft of the manuscript. F.P, L.S. prepared Fig. 1. All authors reviewed the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Daniela Villani.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

This study has been conducted in compliance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Ethics Committee of Psychology Department of Università Cattolica del Sacro Cuore of Milan (protocol 21–22). All participants signed an informed consent form before beginning the research about the research protocol, data protection and privacy according to the General Data Protection Regulation (GDPR; EU 2016/679). The study’s objectives, confidentiality, and anonymity were described, and volunteers were given full authority to complete the study. All methods were performed in accordance with the relevant guidelines and regulations in the declaration—Ethics approval and consent to participate section.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Villani, D., Pancini, E., Pesce, F. et al. Savoring life during pandemic: an online intervention to promote well-being in emerging adults. BMC Psychol 11, 196 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-023-01225-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-023-01225-z

Keywords