Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Interpersonal Behaviors Questionnaire (IBQ) applied to parenting of emerging adults: dimensional structure and criterion validity

Abstract

Background

The prolonged transitions to adulthood strengthen interest in parenting characteristics that can shape emerging adults’ development and progression to full-fledged adulthood. It also strengthens interest in instruments suitable for measuring such parenting characteristics. The current study contributes to this area of research by applying the Interpersonal Behavior Questionnaire (IBQ), to assess parenting behaviors that are pertinent for emerging adults’ development and wellbeing, and seeks evidence of its dimensional structure and criterion validity.

Method

The sample of the study consisted of 600 emerging adults (Mage = 24.94, SDage = 3.03, range 19–29 years; 52.3% women). The dimensionality of IBQ was assessed by testing a sequence of theoretically plausible models representing alternative factor score structures. Criterion validity was investigated by exploring the associations between the IBQ dimensions and basic psychological need satisfaction and frustration, negative affectivity, and thriving.

Results

The findings provide evidence of a hypothesized six-factor structure, but advocate for the use of exploratory structural equation as it may more accurately reflect associations between the six dimensions. Findings also provide evidence of criterion validity.

Conclusions

The findings indicate that IBQ applied to parenting targets dimensions highly relevant for EAs’ flourishing or impoverished functioning. Findings also offer evidence of structure and criterion validity for the IBQ applied to emerging adults’ parent behaviors. As such, researchers may find IBQ attractive, as it is relatively concise yet also a holistic measure that captures the behaviors that both support and thwart an emerging adult’s need for autonomy, competence, and relatedness. Findings also shed light on the process of becoming an adult, the shift in parent–child relationships during this process, and emerging adults’ wellbeing.

Peer Review reports

Background

A transition from parental dependence to self-reliance that represents achieving adulthood [1]intensifies and finalizes during emerging adulthood (EA; the period between 19 and 25/29 years of age; [2]. During these years, relationships between emerging adults (EAs) and their parents undergo a transformation [1] that is characterized by a gradual decrease in the frequency of parent–child interactions and an increase in mutuality, reciprocity, horizontality, closeness, openness, and overall harmony in parent–child relationships [3, 4]. While this transformation is considered normative for this period of life, there is significant variability in the timing of these changes. That is, changes in parent–child relationships can occur relatively early for some EAs and much later for others [1]. This variability can partially be attributed to prolonged semi-dependence on parental financial and social support, which now characterizes many EAs, as it takes longer for recent generations of young people to reach financial self-reliance [5, 6]. As a result, parents remain active agents in the development of many EAs and can significantly affect their wellbeing [3].

The heterogeneity of adulthood transitions and prolonged EAs’ dependence on family support strengthen interest in parenting practices, which shape EAs’ development and progression to full-fledged adulthood, and theory-based instruments suitable for measuring such parenting characteristics (e.g., [7]). The current study contributes to this area of research by applying the Self-Determination Theory (SDT)-based scale of need-supportive and -thwarting interpersonal context, namely Interpersonal Behavior Questionnaire (IBQ; [8, 9]), to assess parenting behaviors that are pertinent for EAs’ development and wellbeing. While doing so, the study also addresses its dimensional structure and seeks evidence of its criterion validity.

The role of autonomy-, competence-, and relatedness-supportive and -thwarting parenting in emerging adults’ wellbeing and maturation process

According to the SDT, a person is more likely to flourish and follow a more adaptive development trajectory when one’s interpersonal context supports three basic psychological needs (BPN)—the ones for autonomy, competence, and relatedness [10, 11]. SDT posits that the lack of support for BPN may fail to foster the growth potential of individuals, but the interpersonal context that actively blocks or thwarts the satisfaction of BPN may impede growth, promote ill-being and impoverished functioning [12, 13, 14]. The theory also states that the satisfaction of BPN is necessary for an individual’s growth and positive functioning at any developmental stage [14, 15]. This also applies to EA, in which attaining autonomy without making sacrifices in relatedness in relationships with parents is considered one of the central developmental tasks [1, 16, 17]. At the same time, competence (self-efficacy, mastery) is considered an essential resource in navigating the transition to adulthood [18, 19, 20]. More so, SDT stresses that the satisfaction of three basic psychological needs is a universal requirement for wellness and adaptive growth,it applies to everyone regardless of the sociocultural context [11].

As parents still play a significant role in EAs’ development [6], they can also support or thwart the satisfaction of three needs and shape the inherent propensities toward the positive development and wellbeing of their EA children. For example, autonomy-supportive parenting (often referred to as parental promotion of volitional functioning [21]; was linked to EAs’ higher psychological wellbeing [22], fewer internalizing problems [23, 24, 25], fewer externalizing problems, lower depression, and loneliness [26], and a better adjustment in an academic and social context [22]. In contrast, autonomy-thwarting parenting (referred to as psychological control; [21] was linked to perceived incompetence in reaching adulthood [3], dysfunctional individuation [27], higher depression symptoms, lower self-esteem [28], higher internalizing problems [23], difficulties in establishing general [29] and vocational identity commitments [3], and relational aggression [30]. Compared to other parenting combinations, the combination of low autonomy-support and high autonomy-thwarting, as shown in the person-oriented studies, was also most strongly predictive of problems in multiple domains of psychological functioning [31, 32].

Relatedness-supportive parenting was also linked with the psychological functioning of EAs. For example, both autonomy- and relatedness-supportive parenting were linked to lower externalizing problems, depression, and loneliness, while the relatedness-supportive parenting also contributed to lower levels of stress and anxiety [26]. Relatedness-supportive parenting was also linked to academic self-efficacy and individuation of EAs involved in tertiary education [33]. While fewer studies addressed the effects of competence-supportive parenting, one study has shown that among the three needs, the satisfaction of the need for competence in the relationships with parents was most strongly related to a more optimal personal identity development of EAs [34].

In sum, plenty of evidence suggests that parenting characteristics reflecting need-supportive or -thwarting behaviors may play a major role in the adaptive or maladaptive development and wellbeing of EAs. Nevertheless, the majority of studies with EAs on need-supportive parenting focus only on autonomy, and relatively few address the two remaining needs. Such a situation may be explained by a lack of validated and holistic tools that target parental behaviors related to the satisfaction and frustration of all three needs. One tool that could measure the characteristics of family context from the SDT perspective is the IBQ. It was initially developed to assess BPN satisfaction and frustration in the overall interpersonal context of an individual [8, 9], but it was also adapted to the contexts of general education [35], physical education [36], and sports [8, 9]. Being relatively flexible, IBQ can be adapted to measure parenting, which would provide a possibility to more extensively study and bring more light on the role of parenting during the years of EA.

The current study

In the current study, we apply the IBQ to measure general parenting as perceived by EAs and seek evidence of its structure and criterion validity. The IBQ has six scales that measure six SDT-based interpersonal styles. Three of these measure BPN supportive interpersonal behaviors, and three—BPN thwarting. Table 1 provides definitions of each style.

Table 1 Conceptual definitions of the six need-supportive and need-thwarting interpersonal context dimensions and sample items

While addressing the internal structure of the measure, we also consider the debate of dimensionality that surrounds the instruments targeting BPN satisfaction/frustration and BPN supportive/thwarting contexts. This debate is focused on what set of dimensions (correlated factor, bi-factor, or second-order models) best represent or could be used to represent such measures and what measurement models (CFA-type or ESEM-type models) are best suited to capture the underlying factors. This debate stems from the numerous ways these six dimensions were modeled in the studies on BPN (for an overview, see [37, 38]). We address this issue by investigating the dimensionality of IBQ applied to parenting using an analytical framework utilized in the studies with IBQ targeting general interpersonal context [38] and the BPN satisfaction and frustration [37] that includes testing a sequence of theoretically plausible models representing alternative factor score structures.

In this framework, there is an established sequence of testing the structure of IBQ, and uniform names of the models are used to refer to specific model solutions. We follow this sequence and names in our study to facilitate a comparison of our findings to those of the previous research. Models one to seven are “correlated-factor” models, which hypothesize the presence of a different set of correlated dimensions. Specifically, model 1 (M1), a CFA-type (confirmatory factor analysis type) model, hypothesizes the presence of one global overreaching need-nurturing interpersonal context (in our case—parenting) dimension. Models 2, 4, and 6 are CFA-type models hypothesizing the presence of two (M2; need-supportive and need-thwarting interpersonal styles), three (M4; autonomy-, competence-, and relatedness-supportive style), and six (M6; autonomy-, competence-, and relatedness need-supportive and autonomy-, competence-, and relatedness need-thwarting) interpersonal styles. Model 6 represents the score structure reported in the initial IBQ validation study [8, 9] and was expected to have a better fit than Models 1, 2, and 4. Models 3, 5, and 7 are respective ESEM (exploratory structural equation model) alternatives of models 2, 4, and 6. These models have the same set of factors but are modeled using the ESEM approach, which does not constrain any cross-loadings to zero.

The next set consists of bi-factor models that, in addition to the dimensions modeled in the “correlated-factor” models (models 1 to 7), include one or two general factors. A unique contribution of bifactor models is that they enable extracting the balanced need-nurturing context as a general factor and the satisfaction or frustration of a specific need as specific factors (e.g., [38]. Models 8, 10, and 14 are bi-factor CFA alternatives of M2, M4, and M6, which, in addition to the set of specific factors, also include a single general “need-nurturing context” factor that loads on all indicators. Models 9, 11, and 15 are respective ESEM alternatives of M8, M10, and M14. Models 12 and 16 are respective CFA alternatives of M4 and M6, but both include two correlated general factors instead of one. Models 13 and 17 are ESEM alternatives of M12 and M16.

Lastly, in line with the previous study that used this framework to investigate the dimensionality of IBQ in a general interpersonal context [38], we also tested higher-order alternatives for the M14 (labeled as M14.2), M15 (labeled as M15.2), M16 (labeled as M16.2), and M17 (labeled as M17.2). The higher-order models tested a hypothesis that the shared variance between the six first-order factors is accounted by one (M14.2 and M15.2) or two (M16.2 and M17.2) superordinate higher-order dimensions. Such models are interesting as they test if the six dimensions can be summarized and represented by more general dimensions.

To obtain evidence of criterion validity, we also investigate the associations between the dimensions of need-supportive and -thwarting parenting and a set of external variables, namely BPN satisfaction and frustration, and negative affectivity and thriving. Lastly, since gender and age may also shape child and parent relationships during emerging adulthood [3], we sought evidence of generalizability of the IBQ’s factor structure and tested for measurement invariance in groups of age and gender.

Methods

Participants, procedures, and missing data

In the current study, we use the first wave of data from a three-wave longitudinal research project (see [39], for a more thorough description of the study). The sample includes 600 emerging adults (Mage = 24.94, SDage = 3.03; 52.3% women). The participants were recruited from a Lithuanian online survey panel. A controlled quota sampling strategy ensured that the distribution of occupational status (educational and employment status configuration) in the sample reflected the general situation of emerging adults in Lithuania (Statistics Lithuania 2021). The participants were diverse in terms of parental socioeconomic status and other demographic characteristics. Eleven participants (1.8%) did not complete measures targeting perceptions of parenting. Consequently, the effective sample used in this study was 589 participants. Additional file 1 provide more details on participant characteristics.

The participants were recruited by sending random invitations to the panel members of a predetermined age (19–29 years old) until the established quotas were filled. Before proceeding to the survey, participants were acquainted with the purpose of the study and signed consent forms. Participants were rewarded with 1–2 Euros worth tokens for taking a survey.

Measures

To assess the need-supportive and -thwarting parenting, we slightly modified the IBQ [8, 9]. The original instructions asking the participants to think about the people in their life in general were replaced by the instructions to think about one’s own parents. IBQ assesses six interpersonal behavior styles, with four items each: autonomy-supportive and -thwarting, competence-supportive and -thwarting,relatedness-supportive and -thwarting. The items were rated on a five-point scale (1 = Do not agree at all; 5 = Completely agree). Table 1 provides item examples and composite reliability for each scale.

Basic psychological need satisfaction and frustration were assessed using a scale (BNSFS) developed by Chen et al. [12]. It has six subscales, each assessed with four items: autonomy satisfaction (e.g., “I feel that my decisions reflect what I really want”; α = 0.78) and frustration (e.g., “My daily activities feel like a chain of obligations”; α = 0.74); competence satisfaction (e.g., “I feel capable at what I do”; α = 0.85) and frustration (e.g., “I feel insecure about my abilities”; α = 0.85); relatedness satisfaction (e.g., “I experience a warm feeling with the people I spend time with”; α = 0.87) and frustration (e.g., “I have the impression that people I spend time with dislike me”; α = 0.83). The participants rated the items on a five-point scale (1 = Completely untrue; 5 = Completely true).

Negative affectivity was assessed using the Depression, Anxiety, and Stress Scale [40]. It assesses depression, anxiety, and stress symptoms with 12 items (e.g., “I felt sad, down, and blue”) scored on a four-point scale (1 = “Did not apply to me at all”,4 = “Applied to me very much, or most of the time”). Averaged items formed a reliable (α = 0.91) negative affectivity score.

Thriving was measured using the Brief Inventory of Thriving [41]. The instrument includes 10 items (e.g., “My life is going well”,α = 0.93) scored on a five-point Likert-type scale (1 = “Strongly disagree”; 5 = “Strongly agree”).

All measures were translated into Lithuanian using a teamwork approach, which involved conducting multiple translations by two study contributors and one professional translator. The team discussed all translations, and the best-translated items were selected for the main study. All instruments are free to use for academic research purposes. Permission to adapt the IBQ to parenting behaviors was received from the author of the instrument.

Data analysis

All models were estimated using the exact specification used in the earlier studies [37, 38] to compare our findings to those of the previous studies directly. All analyses were conducted using the Mplus 8.7 software. We used the robust maximum likelihood estimator (MLR) to assess the models. Target rotation [42] was used in ESEM models. An oblique rotation strategy was used for the correlated-factor ESEM models (Models 3, 5, and 7) and orthogonal for the bi-factor models (Models 9, 11, 13, 15, and 17). Higher-order ESEM models (M15.2 and M17.2) were estimated in a two-step approach, using the ESEM-within-CFA strategy [43].

To choose the best representation of the dimensional structure, we followed the recommendations of assessing bi-factor CFA and ESEM models [43, 44, 45], which, in addition to examining the model-data fit, strongly advocate for close examination of item-factor and between factor associations. Specifically, when examining the models, we first assessed their fit with the data. To assess model fit we used RMSEA (Root Mean Square Error of Approximation) and CFI (Comparative Fit Index). RMSEA lower than 0.05, accompanied by CFI higher than 0.95, indicated good model-data fit [46]. Models that were not characterized by good fit were not further examined. Once good-fitting models were identified, we closely examined the pattern of factor loadings and between-factor correlations to identify models with a structure that matches the theoretical expectations and has a meaningful and interpretable structure.

To obtain evidence of criterion validity, we investigated the correlations between the dimension of need-supportive and need-thwarting parenting and basic psychological need satisfaction and frustration and negative affectivity and thriving. Measurement invariance (MI) analysis was performed by testing a series of models that impose equality constraints across groups for factor loadings (weak), item intercepts (strong), and residual error variances (strict) (Brown 2015). At each step, we evaluated how the model with fewer constraints compares to the model with more constraints. A certain level of invariance was supported if the constraints did not result in a substantial decrease (equal to or higher than 0.01) of CFI supplemented by a substantial (equal to or higher than 0.015) increase in RMSEA [47].

Results

Correlated factor models

Table 2 summarizes the fit of the seven correlated-factor models. Models 1 through 4 were not characterized by a good fit with data and were not further considered. Model 5 fit the data well, but the three factors (autonomy, competence, and relatedness) were poorly defined in terms of factor loadings. Expectedly, six-factor models 6 and 7 had a good fit with the data. Also, in both of these models, all six factors were clearly defined in terms of factor loadings. However, in the CFA model (M6), the three correlations between the need-supportive parenting dimensions were over 0.80, indicating a substantial overlap between the factors and suggesting that the three need-supportive parenting factors may not be distinguishable (Table 3 presents latent correlations). The same situation was evident for the three need-thwarting parenting dimensions. The six-factor ESEM alternative (M7), on the other hand, did not exhibit this problem, i.e., the size of the correlations between the six factors did not indicate a lack of discriminant validity. The comparison of the two models (M6 vs. M7) also suggested that ESEM had a better fit with data than the CFA alternative, advocating for the ESEM solution.

Table 2 Model fit statistics for the different factor analysis models of the need-supportive and -thwarting parenting
Table 3 Internal and criterion validity correlations (N = 589)

Bi-factor models

Table 2 summarizes the fit of the ten bi-factor models. All bi-factor CFA models (M8, M10, M12, M14, and M16) were poorly defined in terms of factor-item associations, and all, except for M12, did not have a good fit with the data. As such, bi-factor CFA models were not further considered. Two of the most complex bi-factor ESEM models, namely M15 and M17, did not converge. Using the restricted maximum likelihood estimation, i.e., forcing the residual variances to be positive, did help with convergence for M17, but not M15. The remaining bi-factor ESEM models (M9, M11, M13) converged and did have a good model-data fit. However, neither of these models was well-defined in terms of factor-item associations, and the same problem was evident for the M17. Overall, findings suggested that neither CFA nor ESEM bi-factor models were applicable to represent the structure of IBQ applied to parenting.

Higher-order models

Table 2 summarizes the fit of the four higher-order models. The two higher-order CFA models, namely M14.2 and M16.2, did not fit the data well, and the fit of these models was much worse than that of the M6, based on which the two higher-order models were constructed. More so, the initial run for M16.2 converged in an inadmissible solution, and a restricted maximum likelihood estimation was used to deal with this issue. However, the two ESEM alternatives, namely M15.2 and M17.2, converged well and had a good fit with the data. In addition, the fit of both models was very similar to that of the M7, based on which these two were constructed. However, of the two, the M17.2, predicting the presence of two general factors instead of one, had a slightly better fit with the data. The correlation between the two general factors was also moderate (equal to − 0.42), supporting the distinctiveness of the two general factors. Overall, the factor structure analysis provided the strongest support for the six-factor ESEM model and the second-order ESEM model, predicting two higher general dimensions of need-supporting and need-thwarting parenting.

Criterion validity

To obtain criterion validity evidence, we explored the correlations between the dimensions of parenting and our criterion validity variables: BPN satisfaction and frustration, negative affectivity, and thriving. Considering the findings of factor structure analysis, we investigated correlations between criterion variables and the six specific need-supportive and -thwarting dimensions of parenting, as well as with the two general dimensions (overall need-supportive and need-thwarting) of parenting.

The six parenting dimensions positively correlated with the corresponding dimensions of need satisfaction and frustration, providing evidence of criterion validity. In addition, general and need-specific supportive parenting dimensions were positively and moderately correlated with thriving. Similarly, general and need-specific thwarting parenting dimensions were positively and moderately correlated with negative affectivity, further evidence of criterion validity. Table 3 presents these correlations.

Measurement invariance across groups based on gender and age

Lastly, we proceeded to test for MI of the six-factor ESEM model. Constraining factor loadings, intercepts, and residuals did not substantially decrease the model fit. As such, results indicated that the scores obtained by the IBQ scale adapted for parenting are measurement invariant across groups based on gender. MI results are reported in Table 4.

Table 4 Results of measurement invariance analysis across groups based on gender (N = 589)

Discussion

The primary goal of the current study was to investigate if the IBQ, a measure developed to assess the characteristics of the overall interpersonal context of an individual, can also be used to measure parenting characteristics as perceived by EA children. To do so, we investigated the structure of the scale, its criterion validity, and its measurement invariance across groups based on gender and age.

Factor analysis results for the correlated-factor models predicting one, two, or three factors in IBQ scores closely replicated the findings of two earlier investigations on IBQ structure in a general interpersonal context [37, 38], i.e., neither of these models were well-defined in terms of factor-item associations, and most had a poor fit with the data. This finding indicated that oversimplified internal structure representations, predicting one, two, or three factors, misrepresent the internal associations of the IBQ. The six-factor CFA representation of the IBQ, on the other hand, had a good fit with data, similar to that reported in the initial IBQ validation study [8, 9] and two subsequent applications to specific interpersonal contexts [8, 9, 36]. Overall, this finding indicated that IBQ applied to parental behaviors retains its hypothesized factor structure.

Notably, while the six-factor CFA model fit well with the data, the correlations between the three scales of need-supportive parenting and those between the scales of need-thwarting were very high (over 0.80), putting the discriminant validity of the scales in question. However, the six-factor ESEM model had an even better fit with data than the CFA one and substantially weaker correlations between latent dimensions. Such a finding signals that high correlations observed in the CFA model can be attributed to small cross-loadings that are present yet constrained to zero in the CFA model [45]. Since constrained to zero cross-loadings may inflate and mispresent the associations between the factors, findings suggest that the ESEM representation is more suitable for research, i.e., it may more accurately reflect associations between the six dimensions.

The results for bi-factor models for the IBQ applied to parenting behaviors were also closely in line with two earlier investigations [37, 38] and warned against using such models. CFA model predicting six specific dimensions and one general, also poorly fit with data, echoing results of earlier studies. However, deviating from two earlier investigations, an ESEM representation of the same model did not converge, which was an unexpected finding, considering that this model was advocated in two earlier studies [37, 38]. The finding could be attributed to the fact that earlier studies assessed a very generalized view of interpersonal relationships without considering a particular social context, in which the distinction between supportive and thwarting interpersonal behaviors may have been less substantial.

In contrast, the results of our study supported higher-order ESEM models. Of the tested two, a better fit was observed for the one predicting two general factors (general need-supportive parenting and general need-thwarting parenting). This finding suggests that for the analytic parsimony, a broader need-supportive parenting dimension can be used to represent autonomy-, competence-, and relatedness-supportive parenting styles, and the same principle is applicable with need-thwarting parenting. The strength of the correlation between the two general dimensions also suggested that the two general dimensions were rather distinctive and provided evidence for the discriminant validity.

Proving evidence that the six SDT-based dimensions of parenting align well with the theoretical framework proposed by the SDT, correlations between the scores of six need-specific and two general parenting dimensions were substantially associated with the dimensions of need satisfaction and frustration. This suggests that emerging adults’ sense of autonomy, competence, and relatedness is still closely related to how they see their parents behaviors. Noteworthy, all six parenting dimensions had slightly stronger associations with satisfaction and frustration of the need for relatedness. These findings suggest that parental need-supportive and -thwarting behaviors may have a stronger effect on relatedness need-satisfaction and frustration than on satisfaction/frustration of autonomy and competence needs.

Lastly, providing evidence of criterion validity, all six parenting dimensions were significantly associated with well- and ill-being. In particular, thriving was significantly positively and more strongly than negative affectivity associated with each specific need-supportive and general need-supportive parenting dimension. In the meantime, negative affectivity was significantly positively and more strongly than thriving associated with each specific need-thwarting and general need-thwarting parenting dimension. This finding closely supported the central tenets of the SDT, postulating that when one’s interpersonal contexts support the three BPN, a person is more likely to flourish [10, 11], and when interpersonal context actively blocks or thwarts BPN, a person is likely to display more impoverished functioning [12, 13, 14].

Overall, the findings indicate that IBQ applied to study parenting characteristics targets the aspects of parental interpersonal style that: (a) align well with the theoretical framework proposed by the SDT; (b) are highly relevant for EAs’ wellbeing. The former provides further support for IBQ authors’ earlier suggestions [8, 9] that IBQ is a versatile tool and may be adapted to assess the interpersonal contexts embedded in different life domains. Perhaps the essential feature of the IBQ that researchers may find attractive is that it is relatively concise yet also a holistic measure that captures the behaviors that both support and thwart an individual’s need for autonomy, competence, and relatedness. Since need-supportive and -thwarting behaviors represent distinctive dimensions that have differential implications for those facing such interpersonal styles (e.g., [48], the IBQ, when used to assess parenting, may offer a possibility to distinguish parents that engage in neither of such behaviors (e.g., indifferent), some such behaviors (e.g., only need-thwarting behaviors), or even both of these (e.g., overinvolved). While distinguishing between behaviors affecting specific needs may require the ESEM approach, the use of such an instrument may open a new research avenue for those interested in the role of parenting on emerging adults’ positive and problematic development patterns. Specifically, it may shed more light on the parental behavior patterns that may be more or less beneficial in becoming an independent, self-sufficient, and responsible person.

Limitations

One of the study’s limitations is that EAs reported their subjective perspective on their parents’ behaviors, which may give a biased view of parenting. Nevertheless, subjective perceptions of parenting may be more important for developmental outcomes than actual parental behaviors. Also, the findings may be less generalizable to other developmental stages and other interpersonal contexts. Future studies using parents and perhaps multi-informant assessment strategies could provide valuable new evidence on the dimensionality and role of need-supportive and -thwarting parenting for EAs. Another considerable limitation of the study is that EAs reported their perspectives about parents in general, instead of reporting separately on mother’s and father’s interpersonal styles. Future studies with separate assessments are necessary to understand if measurement structures are similar for mothers’ and fathers’ interpersonal styles.

Conclusions

Parents still play a significant role in EA children’s lives, and parenting that supports or thwarts basic psychological needs can shape their offspring’s development and wellbeing. Studies investigating the effects of need-supportive and -thwarting parenting focus on strategies that target a specific need, and very few take a broader SDT-based view and address those that support or thwart all three needs. To address a lack of comprehensive measures that assess parenting styles related to all three BPN, the study offers evidence of structure and criterion validity for the IBQ applied to EAs’ parent behaviors. It also sheds light on the process of becoming an adult, the shift in parent–child relationships during this process, and emerging adults’ wellbeing.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets generated and/or analyzed during the current study are available in the OSF repository, https://bit.ly/3AJ0CRM (link is anonymized).

Abbreviations

EA:

Emerging adulthood

EAs:

Emerging adults

SDT:

Self-determination theory

IBQ:

Interpersonal Behavior Questionnaire

BPN:

Basic psychological needs

CFA:

Confirmatory factor analysis

ESEM:

Exploratory structural equation modeling

References

  1. Tanner JL. Recentering during emerging adulthood: a critical turning point in life span human development. In: Emerging adults in America: Coming of age in the 21st century. American Psychological Association. 2006. p. 21–55. https://doi.org/10.1037/11381-002.

  2. Arnett JJ. Emerging adulthood: the winding road from the late teens through the twenties. 2nd ed. Oxford University Press; 2014.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  3. Lindell AK, Campione-Barr N. Continuity and change in the family system across the transition from adolescence to emerging adulthood. Marriage Fam Rev. 2017;53(4):388–416. https://doi.org/10.1080/01494929.2016.1184212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Oliveira C, Fonseca G, Sotero L, Crespo C, Relvas AP. Family dynamics during emerging adulthood: reviewing, integrating, and challenging the field. J Fam Theo Rev. 2020;12(3):350–367. https://doi.org/10.1111/jftr.12386.

  5. Fingerman KL, Cheng Y-P, Kim K, Fung HH, Han G, Lang FR, Lee W, Wagner J. Parental involvement with college students in Germany, Hong Kong, Korea, and the United States. J Fam Issues. 2016;37(10):1384–411. https://doi.org/10.1177/0192513X14541444.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Fingerman KL, Yahirun JJ. Emerging adulthood in the context of family. In: Arnett JJ, editor. The Oxford handbook of emerging adulthood, vol. 1. Oxford University Press; 2015. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199795574.013.17.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  7. Jiao J, Segrin C. The development and validation of the overparenting scale—short form. Emerg Adulthood. 2021;8:9. https://doi.org/10.1177/21676968211052783.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Rocchi M, Pelletier L, Cheung S, Baxter D, Beaudry S. Assessing need-supportive and need-thwarting interpersonal behaviours: the Interpersonal Behaviours Questionnaire (IBQ). Personal Individ Differ. 2017;104:423–33. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.08.034.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Rocchi M, Pelletier L, Desmarais P. The validity of the Interpersonal Behaviors Questionnaire (IBQ) in sport. Meas Phys Educ Exerc Sci. 2017;21(1):15–25. https://doi.org/10.1080/1091367X.2016.1242488.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Ryan RM, Deci EL. Overview of self-determination theory: an organismic-dialectical perspective. In: Ryan RM, Deci EL, editors. Handbook of self-determination research. University of Rochester Press; 2002. p. 3–33.

    Google Scholar 

  11. Ryan RM, Deci EL. Self-determination theory: basic psychological needs in motivation, development, and wellness. Guilford Press; 2007.

    Google Scholar 

  12. Chen B, Vansteenkiste M, Beyers W, Boone L, Deci EL, van der Kaap-Deeder J, Duriez B, Lens W, Matos L, Mouratidis A, Ryan RM, Sheldon KM, Soenens B, van Petegem S, Verstuyf J. Basic psychological need satisfaction, need frustration, and need strength across four cultures. Motiv Emot. 2015;39(2):216–36. https://doi.org/10.1007/S11031-014-9450-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. van der Kaap-Deeder J, Brenning K, Neyrinck B. Emotion regulation and borderline personality features: the mediating role of basic psychological need frustration. Personal Individ Differ. 2021;168: 110365. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2020.110365.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Vansteenkiste M, Ryan RM, Soenens B. Basic psychological need theory: advancements, critical themes, and future directions. Motiv Emot. 2020;44(1):1–31. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-019-09818-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Niemiec CP, Soenens B, Vansteenkiste M. Is relatedness enough? On the importance of need support in different types of social experiences. In: Weinstein N, editor. Human motivation and interpersonal relationships: theory, research, and applications. New York: Springer; 2014. p. 77–96. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-017-8542-6_4.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  16. Buhl HM. Development of a model describing individuated adult child–parent relationships. Int J Behav Dev. 2008;32(5):381–9. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025408093656.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Kruse J, Walper S. Types of individuation in relation to parents: predictors and outcomes. Int J Behav Dev. 2008;32(5):390–400. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025408093657.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Lee B, Vondracek FW. Teenage goals and self-efficacy beliefs as precursors of adult career and family outcomes. J Vocat Behav. 2014;85(2):228–37. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.JVB.2014.06.003.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Reed K, Duncan JM, Lucier-Greer M, Fixelle C, Ferraro AJ. Helicopter parenting and emerging adult self-efficacy: implications for mental and physical health. J Child Fam Stud. 2016;25(10):3136–49. https://doi.org/10.1007/S10826-016-0466-X.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Scales PC, Benson PL, Oesterle S, Hill KG, Hawkins JD, Pashak TJ. The dimensions of successful young adult development: a conceptual and measurement framework. Appl Dev Sci. 2015;20(3):150–74. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888691.2015.1082429.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Soenens B, Vansteenkiste M, Lens W, Luyckx K, Goossens L, Beyers W, Ryan RM. Conceptualizing parental autonomy support: adolescent perceptions of promotion of independence versus promotion of volitional functioning. Dev Psychol. 2007;43(3):633–46. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.43.3.633.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Soenens B, Vansteenkiste M, Sierens E. How are parental psychological control and autonomy-support related? A cluster-analytic approach. J Marriage Fam. 2009;71(1):187–202. https://doi.org/10.1111/J.1741-3737.2008.00589.X.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Costa S, Soenens B, Gugliandolo MC, Cuzzocrea F, Larcan R. The mediating role of experiences of need satisfaction in associations between parental psychological control and internalizing problems: a study among Italian college students. J Child Fam Stud. 2015;24(4):1106–16. https://doi.org/10.1007/S10826-014-9919-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Ingoglia S, Inguglia C, Liga F, Coco A. Associations between perceived parental psychological control and internalizing difficulties in emerging adulthood: the role of identity. J Soc Pers Relat. 2016;34(8):1227–40. https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407516670760.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Zong X, Cheah CSL, Yu J, Lim HJ, Vu KTT, Opara N. Identity synthesis as a pathway linking parenting and emerging adults’ internalizing problems. J Child Fam Stud. 2019;28(4):1029–41. https://doi.org/10.1007/S10826-019-01330-X.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Inguglia C, Ingoglia S, Liga F, lo Coco A, lo Cricchio MG. Autonomy and relatedness in adolescence and emerging adulthood: relationships with parental support and psychological distress. J Adult Dev. 2014;22(1):1–13. https://doi.org/10.1007/S10804-014-9196-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Kins E, Soenens B, Beyers W. Parental psychological control and dysfunctional separation-individuation: a tale of two different dynamics. J Adolesc. 2012;35(5):1099–109. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.ADOLESCENCE.2012.02.017.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Soenens B, Vansteenkiste M, Luyten P, Duriez B, Goossens L. Maladaptive perfectionistic self-representations: the mediational link between psychological control and adjustment. Personal Individ Differ. 2005;38(2):487–98. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.PAID.2004.05.008.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Luyckx K, Soenens B, Vansteenkiste M, Goossens L, Berzonsky MD. Parental psychological control and dimensions of identity formation in emerging adulthood. J Fam Psychol. 2007;21(3):546–50. https://doi.org/10.1037/0893-3200.21.3.546.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Clark CM, Dahlen ER, Nicholson BC. The role of parenting in relational aggression and prosocial behavior among emerging adults. J Aggress Maltreat Trauma. 2015;24(2):185–202. https://doi.org/10.1080/10926771.2015.1002653.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Liga F, Coco A, le Musso P, Inguglia C, Costa S, le Cricchio MG, Ingoglia S. Parental psychological control, autonomy support and Italian emerging adult’s psychosocial wellbeing: a cluster analytic approach. Eur J Dev Psychol. 2018;17(1):37–55. https://doi.org/10.1080/17405629.2018.1532887.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Nelson LJ, Padilla-Walker LM, Christensen KJ, Evans CA, Carroll JS. Parenting in emerging adulthood: an examination of parenting clusters and correlates. J Youth Adolesc. 2010;40(6):730–43. https://doi.org/10.1007/S10964-010-9584-8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Kaye MP, Lowe K, Dorsch TE. Dyadic examination of parental support, basic needs satisfaction, and student–athlete development during emerging adulthood. J Fam Issues. 2018;40(2):240–63. https://doi.org/10.1177/0192513X18806557.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Erçelik P, Dost-Gözkan A. Basic psychological needs in relationships with parents and best friend and identity statuses among Turkish emerging adults. Curr Psychol. 2020. https://doi.org/10.1007/S12144-020-00710-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Wang Y, King RB, Wang F, Leung SO. Need-supportive teaching is positively associated with students’ wellbeing: a cross-cultural study. Learn Individ Differ. 2021;92: 102051. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.LINDIF.2021.102051.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Rodrigues F, Pelletier L, Neiva HP, Teixeira DS, Cid L, Monteiro D. Initial validation of the Portuguese version of the Interpersonal Behavior Questionnaire (IBQ & IBQ-Self) in the context of exercise: measurement invariance and latent mean differences. Curr Psychol. 2019;40:4040–51. https://doi.org/10.1007/S12144-019-00374-Y.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Tóth-Király I, Bőthe B, Orosz G, Rigó A. A new look on the representation and criterion validity of need fulfillment: application of the bifactor exploratory structural equation modeling framework. J Happiness Stud. 2018;20(5):1609–26. https://doi.org/10.1007/S10902-018-0015-Y.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Tóth-Király I, Morin AJS, Gillet N, Bőthe B, Nadon L, Rigó A, Orosz G. Refining the assessment of need supportive and need thwarting interpersonal behaviors using the bifactor exploratory structural equation modeling framework. Curr Psychol. 2020;1:15. https://doi.org/10.1007/S12144-020-00828-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Vosylis R, Sorgente A, Serido J, Lanz M, Raižienė S. Becoming financially self-sufficient: developing a need-supportive and need-thwarting scale for financial parenting of emerging adults. J Personal Assess. 2022;1:12. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2022.2075268.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Antony MM, Cox BJ, Enns MW, Bieling PJ, Swinson RP. Psychometric properties of the 42-item and 21-item versions of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales in clinical groups and a community sample. Psychol Assess. 1998;10(2):176–81. https://doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.10.2.176.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Su R, Tay L, Diener E. The development and validation of the comprehensive inventory of thriving (CIT) and the brief inventory of thriving (BIT). Appl Psychol Health Well Being. 2014;6(3):251–79. https://doi.org/10.1111/APHW.12027.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Reise S, Moore T, Maydeu-Olivares A. Target rotations and assessing the impact of model violations on the parameters of unidimensional item response theory models. Educ Psychol Meas. 2011;71(4):684–711. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013164410378690.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Morin AJS, Arens AK, Marsh HW. A bifactor exploratory structural equation modeling framework for the identification of distinct sources of construct-relevant psychometric multidimensionality. Struct Equ Model. 2015;23(1):116–39. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705511.2014.961800.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Bornovalova MA, Choate AM, Fatimah H, Petersen KJ, Wiernik BM. Appropriate use of bifactor analysis in psychopathology research: appreciating benefits and limitations. Biol Psychiat. 2020;88(1):18–27. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.BIOPSYCH.2020.01.013.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Marsh HW, Morin AJS, Parker PD, Kaur G. Exploratory structural equation modeling: an integration of the best features of exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2014;10:85–110. https://doi.org/10.1146/ANNUREV-CLINPSY-032813-153700.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Brown TA. Confirmatory for analysis for applied research, 2nd ed. The Guilford Press. 2015.

  47. Chen FF. Sensitivity of goodness of fit indexes to lack of measurement invariance. Struct Equ Model. 2007;14(3):464–504. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705510701301834.

  48. Haerens L, Aelterman N, Vansteenkiste M, Soenens B, van Petegem S. Do perceived autonomy-supportive and controlling teaching relate to physical education students’ motivational experiences through unique pathways? Distinguishing between the bright and dark side of motivation. Psychol Sport Exerc. 2015;16(P3):26–36. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2014.08.013.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

This research was funded by the European Social Fund under measure No. 09.3.3-LMT-K-712 “Development of Competencies of Scientists, Other Researchers, and Students through Practical Research Activities”. Grant recipient: Rimantas Vosylis. Grant no: 09.3.3-LMTK-712–19-0214. The funding body had no role in the design of the study and collection, analysis, and interpretation of data and the writing of the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

RV contributed to the conception, design, and acquisition; drafted the manuscript; critically revised the manuscript approved the final version of the manuscript; and agreed to be accountable for all aspects of work ensuring integrity and accuracy. RE contributed to drafting the manuscript; critically revised the manuscript; approved the final version of the manuscript; and agreed to be accountable for all aspects of work ensuring integrity and accuracy. Both authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Rimantas Vosylis.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

All procedures performed in the study were in accordance with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. The study received ethical approval from the committee of Psychological Research Ethics of Mykolas Romeris University (decision no. 1/-2021, February 12, 2021). All study participants provided informed consent to take part in the study.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The author declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1

. A document provides a detailed description of the characteristics of study participants and characteristics of study participants’ parental socioeconomic status.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Vosylis, R., Erentaitė, R. Interpersonal Behaviors Questionnaire (IBQ) applied to parenting of emerging adults: dimensional structure and criterion validity. BMC Psychol 10, 285 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-022-00983-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-022-00983-6

Keywords