Skip to main content

Sad but true? - How induced emotional states differentially bias self-rated Big Five personality traits

Abstract

Background

The Big Five are seen as stable personality traits. This study hypothesized that their measurement via self-ratings is differentially biased by participants’ emotions. The relationship between habitual emotions and personality should be mirrored in a patterned influence of emotional states upon personality scores.

Methods

We experimentally induced emotional states and compared baseline Big Five scores of ninety-eight German participants (67 female; mean age 22.2) to their scores after the induction of happiness or sadness. Manipulation checks included the induced emotion’s intensity and durability.

Results

The expected differential effect could be detected for neuroticism and extraversion and as a trend for agreeableness. Post-hoc analyses showed that only sadness led to increased neuroticism and decreased extraversion scores. Oppositely, happiness did not decrease neuroticism, but there was a trend for an elevation on extraversion scores.

Conclusion

Results suggest a specific effect of sadness on self-reported personality traits, particularly on neuroticism. Sadness may trigger different self-concepts in susceptible people, biasing perceived personality. This bias could be minimised by tracking participants’ emotional states prior to personality measurement.

Peer Review reports

Background

How are you? We regularly enquire about well-being and intuitively assume that emotional states may guide our thoughts and behaviour, moderating our personality. Although there are many different definitions of personality, it is widely accepted that personality traits are “habitual patterns of behaviour, thought, and emotion” (Kassin2003, p. 327). As we can see a lot of similarity between emotional states and personality traits – both influence the probability of exhibiting certain behaviours – it seems to be important to examine this relationship in more detail. This study investigates the effect of participants’ emotional states on personality testing.

Today’s most popular framework of personality traits are the Big Five (Costa and McCrae1985). The Big Five consist of five personality dimensions: neuroticism, extraversion, openness for experience, agreeableness and conscientiousness. Personality shows a moderate degree of stability over time (Hampson and Goldberg2006; Lucas and Donnellan2011) and even has a genetic basis (Tellegen et al.1988) whilst still changing dynamically in relation to life events conceptually similarly and to the same magnitude as income (Boyce et al.2013). Though, research shows that Big Five’s retest reliability is not perfect: A meta-analysis of 848 stability coefficients from different manuals measuring one or more of the Big Five dimensions reports average coefficients varying between .69 and .76 (Viswesvaran and Ones2000). These results indicate that the remaining 42-52% variance derives from other influencing factors. Some external factors have already been identified: Namely, the source of information, for example self ratings versus ratings by external observers (Allik et al.2010), and the interview process, for example a comparison of face-to-face interviews, telephone interviews and self-rated questionnaires (Lang et al.2011). But questions about the instability of personality traits within-subject remain. In Viswesvaran and One’s own words (2000, p. 227): “The stability of personality traits … [has] been a major source of consternation for personality psychology.”

The conceptualization of personality suggests that its testing should not be influenced by temporary moods: People should respond to how they think and behave in general rather than how they feel in the current situation. However, being a systematic but fluctuating source of measurement variance, it is possible that emotional states bias response as other personal states (e.g., the activation of a certain social role) do (Donahue and Harary1998). Emotional states should also be considered as a source of such “patterned” measurement bias, as evidence derived from related areas of study would suggest.

The influence of mood on self-attributes and self-conception has been studied (Sedikides1995). In a series of between-subject experiments, happy, neutral or negative mood was induced and a significant influence of mood on self-rated negative and positive behaviours was found for behaviours which subjects previously rated as rather unself-descriptive (Sedikides1995).

Recent affect-cognition theories suggest relationships between cognition and affect (Forgas2008). The affect infusion model (AIM, Forgas1995) states that affective influence may occur through inferential and memory based mechanisms depending on the processing style used in a respective situation (Forgas2008). Further, affect may influence the information processing strategy. In doing so, negative affect can even reduce judgement errors (Forgas1998,2008). For example, participants in a negative mood were more accurate in responding to their partner’s self-disclosure (Forgas2011). Therefore, altered information processing (Forgas2008) caused by emotions may result in an altered self-description. As personality assessment relies overly on self-descriptions, we deduce an effect of emotional states on personality testing.

Relationships between personality and habitual emotions

Research on habitual emotions acknowledges that individuals typically differ in how they experience emotions and that the frequency of emotions varies among people. In short, habitual emotions dispose of a trait quality, which makes them appropriate for integration in personality models (Watson and Clark1992). Empirically, relationships of discrete habitual emotions or the superior factors of negative and positive affect with the Big Five dimensions have been reported. Neuroticism correlates positively with anxiety and negative affect (Becker2001; Clark and Watson1999; Watson and Clark1992). Extraversion correlates positively with happiness and shows a moderately positive correlation with positive affect (Becker2001; Clark and Watson1999; Watson and Clark1992). Finally, agreeableness correlates negatively with annoyance/anger (Becker2001). Therefore, at least three of the Big Five traits are associated with habitual emotional experiences, neuroticism and extraversion in particular (Becker2001). Considering that frequent and intense experience of negative affect is associated with higher neuroticism, measuring neuroticism during negative affect may increase scores.

Habitual emotions systematically predict how often and intensely an individual experiences emotions. Those personality traits that are strongly associated with habitual emotions are able to predict emotional experiences as well. This emotional reactivity has been demonstrated by various studies (e.g., Hemenover2003; Smillie et al.2012).

Goals and hypotheses

We therefore conclude that the underlying pattern of the relationship between habitual emotion and personality traits should be mirrored in similarly patterned relations between emotional states and the measurement of personality traits. The purpose of this study is to show that 1) emotional states do have a systematic influence on personality measurement (not necessarily on personality itself), that 2) this influence also varies according to the construct-related similarity between the emotion and personality dimensions and according to 3) the valence of the emotion.

Hypothesis 1: Emotional states generally and differentially alter self-rated personality dimensions compared to base-line measurements.

Hypothesis 2: This differential effect occurs mainly for neuroticism, extraversion and agreeableness.

Hypothesis 3: According to the reported relationships between habitual emotion and personality factors, sadness as a negative emotion explains the differential effects on neuroticism and agreeableness, while the positive emotion happiness explains the effect on extraversion.

Regarding research on emotional reactivity, neuroticism and extraversion at baseline should also predict how intense and how long the respectively related emotion was experienced. In sum, we hypothesize that emotional states directly bias personality reporting.

Methods

Participants

From 107 participants, nine (8.4%) were excluded because of missing values. The remaining 98 participants were, on average, 22.2 years old (SD = 4.74; min = 14; max = 49), 70% were psychology students and the majority were female (67%). Participants gave written informed consent for participation. This study was exempt from ethical approval by the Review Board of the University of Bielefeld.

Treatment and measurement

All participants attended the experiment twice with a time lag of about one month (M = 33.7 days, SD = 4.58, min = 27; max = 44). Treatment was in accordance with APA ethical standards. At the beginning of the first session, each participant provided informed consent prior to the experiment. Importantly, before responding to the NEO-FFI at each measurement, participants were asked to describe their personality in general and as accurately as possible. Subsequently, all further instructions were given via computer to avoid instructor effects.

Treatment in the emotional condition

At the beginning of this condition the emotion was induced via a ten minute short film. To provide strong emotions with an unequivocal valence, we chose happiness as the positive and sadness as the negative emotion. Subsequently to the film, participants were asked to imagine happy or sad scenes from their own personal experience. Music was played in accordance with the emotion. Additionally, participants were asked to focus upon their physical reactions to the induced emotion, and increase them if possible. Participants then had three minutes to adopt the emotion. In this way we used visual, auditory, proprioceptive and cognitive means to induce the emotion.

As stimuli we chose an excerpt from the film ‘Philadelphia’ and Barber’s ‘Adagio pour cordes’ to stimulate the sad condition. A short report about the fall of the Berlin Wall including a reunion of a long divided family and Mozart’s ‘Eine kleine Nachtmusik’ was used for the happy condition. The same pieces of music were successfully used to induce emotions by Eich and Metcalfe (1989).

On the last slide of the power-point-presentation, participants were informed that the emotional induction was over and they were given the pen-and-paper part of the experiment. Before filling out the personality questionnaire, participants were asked to answer all items as honestly as possible to avoid biases caused by social desirability. At first, they responded to the item “Right now I feel very happy/sad”, ranging from 0 (strong disagree) to 6 (strong agree) as a manipulation check. This was repeated in the last item of the questionnaire to indicate the induced emotion’s durability. The emotion control items derived from the manipulation check were used as dependent variables for the correlative replication of emotional reactivity. The session ended with a debriefing; participants were asked how they felt and, especially in the sadness-group, we offered the possibility to talk about what they felt during the experiment.

Treatment in the neutral condition

The neutral (or baseline) condition contained a short film of ten minutes before participants filled out the questionnaire (without the emotion-control items). We decided to show a film about savants, humans with extraordinary skills, with the intention not to evoke any emotion. After the short film, the participants were asked to briefly give thought to their own strengths and weaknesses to encourage them to be more self-alert.

Measurement

The dependent variables were the Big Five personality scores measured with a German version of the NEO-FFI (Five factor inventory, Borkenau and Ostendorf1993; Costa and McCrae1992). This questionnaire consists of 60 items, which are summed up as the Big Five personality factors: Neuroticism, extraversion, openness to experience, agreeableness and conscientiousness (each 12 items). As the original NEO-FFI only uses a five-point Likert-scale, we increased measurement sensitivity by using a seven-point Likert-scale. As this approach was experimental, we computed the intercorrelations for the five factors of our sample to check for deviations from the model. As expected, the results matched those of the NEO-FFI manual: only the same three intercorrelated. However, our version revealed even higher intercorrelations (correlation between neuroticism and extraversion -.33 vs. -.38 in our sample; agreeableness and extraversion .16 vs. .31; conscientiousness and neuroticism -.31 vs. -.41).

Experimental design

Every participant was measured twice. Once in a neutral condition that served as a baseline measurement and about one month earlier or later in the emotional condition. We altered 1) the order of the conditions to avoid sequence effects, and 2) the sequence of the items (original sequence vs. opposite sequence, beginning with the original sequence’s last item), to avoid habituation effects. In sum, the participants were randomly assigned to 8 subgroups, each a combination of the following three dichotomous possibilities (see Table 1):

Table 1 Participants’ distribution for combinations of treatment condition, order and sequence of items
  • Induced emotion: A) happiness or B) sadness

  • Order of treatment condition: C) firstly emotional and secondly neutral, D) vice versa

  • Sequence of items: E) original sequence of the questionnaire in the emotional condition and opposite sequence in the neutral condition, F) vice versa

Statistical analysis

For the manipulation check of the induced emotions we performed ordinal Wilcoxon signed-rank tests to compare induction success. To examine shifts in reported personality scores, we conducted a three step design. First a multivariate 5 × 2 × 2 level repeated measurement ANOVA was performed to test for the global null hypothesis. Two within-subject factors (personality factor and treatment condition) and one between-subject factor (induced emotion) were included. Regarding Hypothesis 1, only the triple interaction was expected to be significant. If Mauchly’s Tests of Sphericity yielded significance, degrees of freedom were corrected according to Huyn-Feldt as Greenhouse-Geisser ϵ’s were above 0.75. Eta-squared (η2) was estimated to describe effect sizes, where η2 = 0.01 describes a small, η2 = 0.06 a medium and η2 = 0.14 a large effect (Cohen1988). In the second step we computed a 2 × 2 level repeated measurement ANOVA for each personality factor with treatment condition as within and induced emotion as between-subject factor, again we expected the interaction to be significant. In the third step, paired-sample t-tests were computed for every personality factor per induced emotion combination. Effect sizes and 95% confidence intervals for paired t-tests were calculated following Dunlap et al. (1996). Confidence interval of the effect sizes were calculated with PSY (http://www.psy.unsw.edu.au/research/research-tools/psy-statistical-program). Post-hoc power calculations showed a satisfying probability to detect reported effects (~73% for the largest reported effect size). In addition, percentages of participants with increasing vs. decreasing personality scores during treatment were displayed to estimate mean variability due to the respective mood induction. Finally, Pearson correlations were calculated between personality traits at baseline and emotional control items.

Randomization check

We controlled goodness of randomization by comparing the personality scores of the happiness and sadness group using t-tests for independent samples. The groups differed neither at baseline, nor after emotion induction (p s > 0.1). In order to exclude any possible sequence or habituation biases, we enlarged the 5 × 2 × 2 repeated measurement ANOVA by adding the two 2-level between-subject factors “order of condition” and “sequence of items”, expecting neither main effects nor interactions under involvement for one or both factors. According to this, the computed model showed no significant main effect as well as no significant interaction on every possible combination of the five factors (p s > 0.1).

Results

Manipulation check

Directly after emotion induction, 85% of the happiness group members agreed at least somewhat (values ≥ 4 of the total range of 0–6) to the item: “Right now I feel very happy” (M = 4.4) and 80% of the sadness group members agreed at least somewhat to the item: “Right now I feel very sad” (M = 4.1), with no mean rank differences between the conditions Z = -0.46, exact p = 0.65. After having filled out the questionnaire, 52% of the sadness group (M = 3.4) and 55% of the happiness group (M = 3.7) still agreed to the same item. Again, no mean rank differences between the conditions were obtained Z = -0.86, exact p = 0.39.

Descriptives

Table 2 shows the averaged Big Five factor scores per personality factor, treatment condition and induced emotion. For a summary of statistical analysis see Figure 1.

Table 2 Descriptive statistics: mean and standard deviations of the personality scores per altered Big Five trait, depending on treatment condition and induced emotion
Figure 1
figure 1

Mean differences of personality scores between emotional and neutral condition dependent on personality factor and induced emotion, results of 5 × 2 × 2 ANOVA, post-hoc 2 × 2-ANOVAs and paired-sample t-tests. Notes: *** = p < .001; ** = p < .01; * = p < .05; ° = p < .10 A: triple interaction of 5×2×2-ANOVA with factors personality trait (Big Five) × emotional condition (neutral and emotional) × induced emotion (sadness and happiness). B: interaction of 2×2-ANOVA with factors emotional condition (neutral and emotional) × induced emotion (sadness and happiness). C: paired-sample t-tests between neutral and emotional condition.

Repeated measures ANOVAs

In a first step, the 3-factor-interaction between treatment condition (within), induced emotion (between) and personality traits (within), tested by a 5 × 2 × 2 level repeated measurement ANOVA revealed a highly significant result with F(3.71, 356.02) = 6.10, p < .001, η2 = 0.06, but, as predicted, none of the 2-factor combinations were significant: 1) treatment condition * induced emotion: F(1, 96) = 0.04, p = 0.85, η2 < 0.01, 2) induced emotion * personality factor: F(3.19, 306.08) = 0.54, p = 0.67, η2 < 0.01, and 3) treatment condition * personality factor: F(3.71, 356.02) = 2.34, p = 0.06, η2 = 0.02. In accordance to the hypothesis, there was also no significant main effect for treatment condition: F(1, 96) = 0.98, p = 0.32, η2 = 0.01, and induced emotion: F(1, 96) = 0.72, p = 0.40, η2 < 0.01. This supported the hypothesis that emotional states generally and differentially alter self-rated personality dimensions compared to base-line measurements.

In a second step 2 × 2 level repeated measurement ANOVAs were computed for every Big Five factor. Significant interactions were shown for the factors neuroticism with F(1, 96) = 10.39, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.10, and extraversion: F(1, 96) = 6.19, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.06, as well as a trend-like interaction on agreeableness: F(1, 96) = 2.83, p = 0.10, η2 = 0.03. The hypothesis, that a differential effect only occurs for personality dimensions with construct-related similarity to habitual emotions was supported but was only a trend for agreeableness.

Post-hoc paired t-tests

In a third step, the three previously identified factors were chosen for post-hoc analyses with paired-sample t-tests (see Table 3). The hypothesis that emotional valence in accordance to the reported relationships of habitual emotion to personality factors shows stronger effects was partially supported: Negative emotion did result in higher neuroticism scores and a lower score for agreeableness, while the positive emotion did not affect any of them. As a contradiction to the hypothesis, happiness did not significantly increase the extraversion score, although sadness did lower the score. The percentages of participants with a higher score for a respective personality factor in the neutral or emotion conditions (see last columns of Table 3) indicate that changes in self-reported personality scores were not due to outlier effects. Instead, each effect is based upon the majority of the participants.

Table 3 Results of the post-hoc paired t-tests and percentages of changes between measurements

Correlation analyses on emotional reactivity

Neither neuroticism nor extraversion at baseline was able to predict the immediate intensity of the respective emotional experience. In contrast, people scoring high on neuroticism tended to display a higher durability of the negative emotion, as revealed by the second manipulation check r = .37, p < .01, N = 53, while more extraverted people tended to maintain happiness r = .30, p < .05, N = 45.

Discussion

The purpose of this study was to investigate if emotional states have a systematic influence on personality measurement. We hypothesized that such influence differs depending on the construct similarity between the habitual emotion and personality dimensions – as well as the valence of the induced emotion. As results show, this assumption was predominantly right. It seems that the well-known relationships between habitual emotions and personality traits are reflected in the influence of emotional states on personality measurement. Differential effects of sadness and happiness could be shown on the dimensions neuroticism and extraversion and as a trend for agreeableness. These results are in accordance with Becker (2001) and Clark and Watson (1999), who both examined habitual emotions. The post-hoc analysis of our study attracts attention as it reveals that mainly sadness induction led to these differential results. When sadness was induced, scores of three personality dimensions differed from their baseline measures.

The influence of sadness on personality traits

When sadness was induced, neuroticism went up considerably and extraversion and agreeableness decreased moderately. Compared to baseline, neuroticism scores increased for nearly two-thirds (66%) of the participants. Further, the 95% confidence interval of the effect size did not include 0.125, indicating a substantial effect (cf. Yarkoni2012) even though we verified the strong relationship between negative affect and neuroticism (Becker2001; Clark and Watson1999) as far as the within-person measurement level. A possible explanation for this finding is that negative affect may trigger negative experiences, which are linked as a component to elevated neuroticism scores (Ormel et al.2012). The AIM model states that participants may have conferred their actual emotional state onto their general feelings as well as emotions may have automatically primed associated ideas or memories (Forgas2008). Further, self-reported neuroticism could also have been influenced by the accommodative, externally focused reasoning strategy induced by negative affect (Forgas2008).

Regarding the large body of research which relates neuroticism to mostly negative outcomes, it seems to be increasingly important to assess neuroticism in an unbiased manner (Cuijpers et al.2010; Bowen et al.2012; Ready et al.2012). This measurement bias could be minimized by controlling for influencing emotional states (Viswesvaran and Ones2000), which may lead to an even stronger predictive power for subsequent behaviour.

The nonexistent influence of happiness on personality traits

In the happiness condition, no influence on personality traits’ measurement was detected - though an increase of extraversion scores could be descriptively observed. The first possible explanation is pragmatic: Unfortunately drop-out participants had all been randomly assigned to the happiness group, resulting in a smaller number of participants. Alternatively, one could be tempted to argue that the happiness induction was not effective; however, this is not supported by the emotion control items. As the manipulation check shows, the agreement to the emotion control item was high – and even slightly higher in the happiness group than in the sadness condition. This either indicates that participants could subjectively accept the happiness induction better, although it had a weaker impact on personality scores, or that most participants are happy anyway (Diener and Diener1996). Thus, participants’ personality scores at baseline may not significantly differ after induced happiness as they were happy without explicit induction.

A third explanation for the weaker effect of the happiness induction refers to Nesse (1990): “Emotional states not only motivate action, they are also goals that we seek to achieve. Most human thought, plans, and actions are intended to induce positive emotions or to avoid negative emotions.” (Nesse1990, p. 262). From this evolutionary point of view, a successful induction of sadness would be more relevant for participants’ behaviour because sadness indicates a situation that should be changed, while positive emotions indicate situations that should be maintained (Nesse1990). Change is more urgent than maintenance. Hence, we suggest that negative emotions may display stronger effects because they are largely stimulative and motivational.

Possible implications on theory

Using a correlative post-analysis of the emotion-control items, we tried to replicate emotional reactivity theory. In accordance with previous research (Hemenover2003; Smillie et al.2012), the neuroticism and extraversion baseline scores correlated with the change rate of the related emotion before and after filling out the questionnaire. While people scoring high on neuroticism tend to display a higher durability of the negative emotion, more extraverted people tend to maintain happiness.

Of course, the results of our study are only first hints. Still, they indicate that it could be reasonable to expand the well-known reactivity model of personality and emotional experience by a reciprocal element: personality determines the experience of emotions while emotional states vice versa impact personality self-ratings. The examination of how and under which conditions this reciprocity occurs and if it is moderated by baseline personality traits is subject to further research.

Limitations

We hypothesized that emotional states bias the measurement of personality traits, especially in experimental test situations. Emotional induction was unsuccessful in only one in five in the sadness group and one in seven members in the happiness group – at least on the conscious level. Although social desirability bias is possible, intimate knowledge of our hypotheses and the questionnaire would have been necessary to fake the Big Five self-ratings in any intended direction. Furthermore, this would not explain why significant effects only occurred after negative mood induction.

Conclusion

Inducing emotions and examining their influence on personality research seems to be a very fertile, powerful and promising approach. In the present study, induced sadness increased self-reported neuroticism while decreasing extraversion. Becoming aware of participants’ emotional state and paying attention to the possible implications on testing could lead to a notable increase in the stability of assessed personality traits.

References

  1. Allik J, Realo A, Mõttus R, Borkenau P, Kuppens P, Hřebíčková M: How people see others is different from how people see themselves: A replicable pattern across cultures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2010, 99 (5): 870-882.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Becker P: Struktur- und Zusammenhangsanalyse von Emotionen und Persönlichkeitseigenschaften [Structural and relational analyses of emotions and personality traits]. Zeitschrift für Differentielle und Diagnostische Psychologie. 2001, 22: 155-172.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Borkenau P, Ostendorf F: NEO- Fünf-Faktoren Inventar (NEO-FFI) nach Costa und McCrae [NEO-Five-factor Inventory, after Costa and McCrae]. 1993, Göttingen: Hogrefe

    Google Scholar 

  4. Bowen R, Balbuena L, Leuschen C, Baetz M: Mood instability is the distinctive feature of neuroticism. Results from the British Health and Lifestyle Study (HALS). Personality and Individual Differences. 2012, 53 (7): 896-900.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Boyce CJ, Wood AM, Powdthavee N: Is personality fixed? Personality changes as much as ‘variable’ economic factors and more strongly predicts changes to life satisfaction. Social Indicators Research. 2013, 111 (1): 287-305.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Clark LA, Watson D: Temperament: A new paradigm for trait psychology. Handbook of personality: theory and research. Edited by: Pervin LA, John OP. 1999, New York: Guilford Press, 399-423. 2

    Google Scholar 

  7. Cohen J: Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. 1988, Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc, 2

    Google Scholar 

  8. Costa PT, McCrae RR: The NEO Personality Inventory manual. 1985, Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources

    Google Scholar 

  9. Costa PT, McCrae RR: Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and the NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) professional manual. 1992, Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources

    Google Scholar 

  10. Cuijpers P, Smit F, Penninx BWJH, de Graaf R, ten Have M, Beekman ATF: Economic costs of neuroticism. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2010, 67 (10): 1086-1093.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Diener E, Diener C: Most people are happy. Psychological Science. 1996, 7 (3): 181-185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Donahue EM, Harary K: The patterened inconsistency of traits: Mapping the differential effects of social roles on self-perceptions of the Big Five. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 1998, 24 (6): 610-619.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Dunlap WP, Cortina JM, Vaslow JB, Burke MJ: Meta-analysis of experiments with matched groups or repeated measures designs. Psychological Methods. 1996, 1 (2): 170-177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Eich E, Metcalfe J: Mood dependent memory for internal versus external events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 1989, 15 (3): 443-455.

    Google Scholar 

  15. Forgas JP: Mood and judgment: The affect infusion model (AIM). Psychological Bulletin. 1995, 117 (1): 39-66.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Forgas JP: On being happy and mistaken: Mood effects on the fundamental attribution error. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1998, 75 (2): 318-331.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Forgas JP: Affect and cognition. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2008, 3 (2): 94-101.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Forgas JP: Affective influences on self-disclosure: Mood effects on the intimacy and reciprocity of disclosing personal information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2011, 100 (3): 449-461.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Hampson SE, Goldberg LR: A first large cohort study of personality trait stability over the 40 years between elementary school and midlife. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2006, 91 (4): 763-779.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Hemenover SH: Individual differences in rate of affect change: Studies in affective chronometry. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2003, 85 (1): 121-131.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Kassin S: Psychology. 2003, USA: Prentice-Hall, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  22. Lang FR, John D, Lüdtke O, Schupp J, Wagner GG: Short assessment of the Big Five: Robust across survey methods except telephone interviewing. Behavior Research Methods. 2011, 43 (2): 548-567.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Lucas RE, Donnellan MB: Personality development across the life span: Longitudinal analyses with a national sample from Germany. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2011, 101 (4): 847-861.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Nesse RM: Evolutionary explanations of emotions. Human Nature. 1990, 1 (3): 261-289.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Ormel J, Riese H, Rosmalen JGM: Interpreting neuroticism scores across the adult life course: Immutable or experience-dependent set points of negative affect?. Clinical Psychology Review. 2012, 32 (1): 71-79.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Ready RE, Åkerstedt AM, Mroczek DK: Emotional complexity and emotional well-being in older adults: Risks of high neuroticism. Aging & Mental Health. 2012, 16 (1): 17-26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Sedikides C: Central and peripheral self-conceptions are differentially influenced by mood: tests of the differential sensitivity hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1995, 69 (4): 759-777.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Smillie LD, Cooper AJ, Wilt J, Revelle W: Do extraverts get more bang for the buck? Refining the affective-reactivity hypothesis of extraversion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2012, 103 (2): 306-326.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Tellegen A, Lykken DT, Bouchard TJ, Wilcox KJ, Segal NL, Rich S: Personality similarity in twins reared apart and together. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1988, 54 (6): 1031-1039.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Viswesvaran C, Ones DS: Measurement error in ‘Big Five factors’ personality assessment: Reliability generalization across studies and measures. Educational and Psychological Measurement. 2000, 60 (2): 224-235.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Watson D, Clark LA: On traits and temperament: General and specific factors of emotional experience and their relation to the five-factor model. Journal of Personality. 1992, 60 (2): 441-476.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Yarkoni T: Sixteen is not magic: comment on Friston. 2012,http://www.talyarkoni.org/blog/2012/04/25/sixteen-is-not-magic-comment-on-friston-2012/,

    Google Scholar 

Pre-publication history

Download references

Acknowledgements

We acknowledge support for the Article Processing Charge by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft and the Open Access Publication Funds of Bielefeld University Library.

Special thanks go to the members of the student research group: T. Beyer, A. Caglar, S. Launer, A. Nagy, A. Plischke, C. Roth, S. Schlachter, P. Sora, L. Stamatescu, S. Strohmeier, M. Thomalla and C. Wolf. We would like to thank L. Thürmer and A. Whale for their help with editing and proof reading and W. Bongartz for providing the positive and supporting framework for this research project. Finally we would like to thank all participants.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Sebastian Schindler.

Additional information

Competing interest

The authors declared that they had no competing interest with respect to their authorship or the publication of this article.

Authors’ contributions

JQ contributed to the study design and carried out participant testing. JQ and SS performed statistical analysis and drafted the manuscript. JQ and SS revised the manuscript. Both authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.

Authors’ original file for figure 1

Authors’ original file for figure 2

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly credited. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Querengässer, J., Schindler, S. Sad but true? - How induced emotional states differentially bias self-rated Big Five personality traits. BMC Psychol 2, 14 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1186/2050-7283-2-14

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/2050-7283-2-14

Keywords