Skip to main content

Development and validation of the Japanese version of the Bedtime Procrastination Scale (BPS-J)

Abstract

Background

The average sleep duration of Japanese people is shorter than that of people from other countries, and bedtime procrastination is suspected to be one of the factors contributing to this issue. This study aimed to develop and validate the Japanese version of the Bedtime Procrastination Scale (BPS-J).

Methods

The BPS-J was developed through procedures including the translation and back-translation of the scale, cognitive interviews with 100 participants who reported having experiences of being diagnosed with insufficient sleep syndrome (ISS) or receiving treatment for ISS using open-ended online questionnaires, and expert checking. To investigate the scale’s validity and reliability, an online survey was conducted with daytime workers aged 20 − 65 years without a history of sleep disorders other than ISS. Half the participants were retested using the same survey after 14 days. Participants’ responses to the Brief Self-Control Scale (BSCS), General Procrastination Scale (GPS), and Munich ChronoType Questionnaire (MCTQ), and data on sleep-related variables such as sleep duration on workdays and the days per week of fatigue or sleep loss, sex, and age, were collected.

Results

We analyzed data from 574 participants to assess scale validity. We then analyzed data from 280 participants to determine test–retest reliability. Confirmatory factor analyses revealed that the two-factor model without Item 2 was most suitable for the BPS-J, unlike other language versions. Regardless of the full-item model or the model with Item 2 eliminated, sufficient reliability and significant correlations with the BSCS, GPS, MCTQ, and sleep-related variables such as sleep duration per night on work days, days per week of feeling fatigued, and days per week of sleep loss were observed. Logistic and linear regressions showed that the relationships between the BPS-J, sleep-related variables, and MCTQ were maintained after adjusting for sex and age.

Conclusion

The BPS-J had sufficient validity and reliability. Further, eliminating Item 2 from the original version of the BPS strengthened the ability to survey Japanese daytime workers.

Peer Review reports

Background

Getting sufficient sleep at the appropriate time is important for maintaining physical and mental health and performance. It has been well established that chronic sleep deprivation or delay in the sleep phase increases the prevalence of diabetes, obesity, and cardiovascular diseases [1,2,3]. It has also been reported that various mental disorders, including major depression, are associated with sleep problems [3,4,5]. In addition, sleep loss interferes with cognitive function [6,7,8,9] and emotion regulation [10,11,12,13], resulting in difficulties in social adjustment [14]. Therefore, getting sufficient sleep at the appropriate time is essential not only for individuals’ health but also for socioeconomic benefits.

Bedtime procrastination, the behavioral tendency of staying up later at night than intended or needed to go to bed, is suggested to be one of the factors promoting shorter sleep duration [15, 16]. Several cross-sectional studies have indicated a relationship between bedtime procrastination and sleep loss. In the general population, individuals with bedtime procrastination tend to have insufficiently short sleep duration, especially on workdays [17]. Adolescents who procrastinated their bedtime tended to have short sleep durations, and those with evening chronotypes showed more severe sleep deprivation [18]. In addition, bedtime procrastination has been suggested to share a core concept of low self-control and general procrastination. Several studies have indicated that self-control and general procrastination are associated with bedtime procrastination [15, 19,20,21,22].

Among the Japanese, bedtime procrastination are suggested to results in a shorter sleep duration and being more prone to an evening chronotype than among non-Japanese individuals. Specifically, the average sleep duration of Japanese people is much shorter than that of people from 63 other countries [23]. Further, the evening chronotype is more prevalent in Japanese young adults than in young adults from other countries [24, 25].

To measure the tendency toward bedtime procrastination, the Bedtime Procrastination Scale (BPS) was developed and validated by Kroese et al. [15]. This self-reported outcome measurement consists of nine items measuring subjective bedtime procrastination. To our knowledge, it is the first scale developed to assess bedtime procrastination. The BPS is a major measurement tool translated and validated in several languages, such as Dutch, Spanish, English, and Korean [15, 19, 20, 22]. However, a Japanese version of the BPS has not yet been developed. The coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) pandemic has particularly brought out the burdens of sleep loss and delayed sleep–wake phase on sleep health worldwide [26,27,28,29]. Bedtime procrastination has been regarded as a factor causing sleep problems during the COVID-19 pandemic [30,31,32]. In addition, the development of the Japanese version of the BPS may help explain the shorter sleep duration of Japanese people in comparison to people from other countries [23,24,25]. Therefore, this study aimed to develop and confirm the validity and reliability of the Japanese version of the Bedtime Procrastination Scale (BPS-J).

Methods

Translation and cross-cultural adaptation processes in the development of the BPS-J

After obtaining permission from the developer of the original version of the BPS, the BPS-J was developed according to the COSMIN reporting guideline in four steps: forward translation, back-translation, cognitive interviews using the questionnaire, and expert checking [33].

First, forward translation was performed by a sleep expert and a non-expert, both being native Japanese speakers. Then, two other sleep experts integrated the two translations by comparing and adopting the expressions.

Second, the BPS-J was back-translated by three non-experts who were bilingual in English and Japanese, unlike the non-expert translator mentioned above. After the BPS-J was back-translated by the first bilingual non-expert, the second bilingual non-expert independently cross-checked it, and the third bilingual non-expert performed the final confirmation. In instances where the back-translated English expressions were very different from the original version, two sleep experts who integrated the draft translations revised the forward-translated version, and the back-translation process was repeated. This process was repeated until the English expressions in the back-translated BPS-J completely matched those in the original BPS.

Linguistic validation of the BPS-J

We conducted cognitive interviews with individuals with suspected insufficient sleep syndrome (ISS) for the linguistic validation of the BPS-J [34], as individuals with ISS were suspected to have bedtime procrastination tendency based on individuals with high BPS scores are reported to have less sleep [15]. A total of 100 participants (male = 53%, mean age = 43.56) who reported having experiences of being diagnosed with ISS or receiving treatment for ISS were recruited through an Internet survey agency (Rakuten Insight, Tokyo, Japan). They were asked to write their opinions about the comprehensibility, comprehensiveness, and relevance of the BPS-J in open-ended fields. The instruction statements were as follows: “Did you find the instructional text and each question item on this scale easy to understand? Please indicate any instructional text or questions that were difficult to understand. If there are alternatives, please indicate them as well”; “Do you think this scale includes the experiences associated with your bedtime procrastination behaviors? If you think any important experiences or characteristics often associated with bedtime procrastination are missing, please describe them (if not, please do not fill in the blanks)”; and “Does each item on this scale correspond to your experiences with bedtime procrastination? If there are items that you think are not related to your bedtime procrastination behaviors, please indicate so (questions 2, 3, 7, and 9 ask about the behavior of those who do not procrastinate going to bed).” Each statement was accompanied by the note “If you think there is no particular problem, please leave this field blank.

The expressions or items in the BPS-J pointed out by two or more participants were revised based on discussion. To improve comprehension, Japanese translations of “rarely” and “frequent” were revised based on the suggestions by five participants. Revisions in terms of the comprehensiveness and relevance of the items were not made because there were no participant suggestions regarding points that they felt should be modified. Further details regarding the suggestions in terms of comprehensibility, relevance, and comprehensiveness are presented in Additional file 1: Appendix 1.

After three sleep experts performed these checks, they approved the final version of the BPS-J (Additional file 2: Appendix 2).

Testing the validity, contribution, and reliability of the BPS-J

Participants

We recruited 600 individuals who reported meeting the following criteria: a) between the ages of 20 and 65 years, b) daytime worker (working four or more days a week), and c) no history of sleep disorders other than ISS. People whose waking times and social constraints on activities varied from day to day were excluded, in accordance with the definition of bedtime procrastination.

Among the participants who completed the questionnaire, those who met the following criteria based on the answers were excluded from the analyses: a) inconsistency between answers (e.g., overlap between sleeping and working time), and b) those who selected incorrect answers to the dummy question.

Survey design

All participants’ data were collected employing a cross-sectional design to estimate structural and criterion validities, internal consistency, measurement error, and relation of the BPS-J. To confirm the test–retest reliability, half of the participants were asked to answer the same questionnaire again after 14 days [35]. All participants were asked to complete the test–retest reliability questionnaire on a first-come, first-served basis, and data collection was stopped when half of the participants had completed the questionnaire.

Online surveys were conducted through Rakuten Insight, Inc., one of the largest online research service companies in Japan. Participants were awarded points by the company after completing the entire questionnaire; the points could be used for shopping and other purposes. Once candidates received a request to participate in the survey, they voluntarily accessed the online page of the survey system. If they agreed to participate after receiving informed consent, they answered screening questions to ensure that they met the inclusion criteria for study participants. When they met the criteria, they completed the entire questionnaire.

Measurements

BPS-J

The BPS measures the tendency of bedtime procrastination, which has been confirmed to have sufficient validity and reliability in the original and other language versions [15, 17, 20, 22]. This tool consists of nine items, and each item is evaluated on a 5-point scale: 1 (almost never) to 5 (almost always). The BPS total score can be calculated in two ways: by calculating the average of the items [15, 17] or by simply summing them [20, 22]. Higher total scores indicate a greater tendency of bedtime procrastination. The values for the alternatives to inverted items, such as Items 2, 3, 7, and 9, were reversed before calculation.

Brief self-control scale

The Brief Self-Control Scale (BSCS) [36] measures the degree of self-control, which is the ability to control one’s own emotions and desires to achieve more important long-term benefits [37, 38], and consists of 13 items. Each item is evaluated from 1 (not at all like me) to 5 (just like me), and higher scores indicate greater difficulty in self-control. Validity and reliability were confirmed in both the original [36] and the Japanese versions [39].

General procrastination scale

The General Procrastination Scale (GPS) [40] measures the tendency of general procrastination, which is the most common and ubiquitous behavioral pattern of laying off things that need to be done in all aspects of life [41, 42]. Validity and reliability were confirmed for both the original [40] and the Japanese [43] versions. The Japanese version of this scale consists of 13 items [43], and each item is evaluated on a scale from 1 (never) to 5 (almost always). A higher total score indicates a stronger tendency for procrastination.

Sleep-related variables

Data on the sleep and related subjective variables were collected based on the items used in the study by Kroese et al. (2014) [15]. These items were generated originally by Kroese et al. [15], and items directly translated from the original were used in this study. Therefore, these items do not have established validity or reliability. Thus, we also adopted the Munich ChronoType Questionnaire (MCTQ) to clarify the relationship between the BPS-J and sleep–wake phase delay.

Sleep duration on workdays was measured using the item “How long is your sleep duration on workdays?” and evaluated with the following alternatives: less than 5 h, less than 6 h, less than 7 h, less than 8 h, less than 9 h, less than 10 h, and 10 h or over. Number of days per week feeling fatigued during the daytime was measured with the item “How many days do you experience fatigue per week?” Number of days per week of feeling sleep loss was measured with the item, “How many days do you experience insufficient sleep per week?” The answers to these two questions were categorized as 0 days, 1 − 2 days, 3 − 4 days, 5 − 6 days, and 7 days. The tendency to view bedtime procrastination as a problem was measured with the item “To what extent do you find it problematic that you go to bed later than you would like to?” on a scale of 1 (not at all) to 5 (very much).

Munich chronotype questionnaire

MCTQ was used to measure participant chronotype by assessing the midpoint of the sleep in work-free days correcting sleep loss. Although the MCTQ has not been examined in previous validation studies of the BPS, it was adopted in this study to understand the relationship between the BPS-J and sleep–wake phase delay.

The MCTQ measures the propensity to sleep at a particular time during a 24-h period, which is potentially regulated by individual circadian rhythmicity. Validity and reliability were confirmed in the original and the Japanese versions [44, 45]. Among the 17 items regarding sleep habits, the following items on workdays and work-free days were used: local time of going to bed, local time of preparing to sleep, sleep latency, and sleep end. With these items, several variables were calculated in line with validation studies [44, 45]. Sleep duration (the interval of sleep onset [local time of preparing to sleep + sleep latency] and sleep end) and midsleep (time [sleep duration ÷ 2] minutes elapsed after sleep onset) on workdays and work-free days as well as average sleep duration per week ([sleep duration on workdays × workdays per week + sleep duration on work-free days × {7-workdays per week}]/7) were calculated. Furthermore, absolute social jetlag (midsleep on weekdays—midsleep on work-free days) and weekly sleep loss ( [average sleep duration per week—sleep duration on workdays] × workdays per week; [average sleep duration per week—sleep duration on work-free days] × [7—workdays per week]) were calculated. Additionally, absolute social jetlag was divided into groups of < 120 min and ≥ 120 min, based on the cut-off point of the existing studies [46,47,48]. Work start time and work end time, which are parts of the seven items in the domain of work details, were also used for calculating working hours.

Statistical analyses

The results of the analyses were defined as statistically significant when the p-value was < 0.05. Analyses of criterion validity were performed using SPSS AMOS version 25 (IBM Japan, Tokyo), and the other analyses were performed using SPSS Statistics version 28 (IBM Japan, Tokyo). Missing values were handled by multiple imputation.

Participants’ characteristics and each item of the BPS-J

The median and Interquartile range (IQR) for continuous variables and the number and proportion for categorical variables were calculated.

To examine whether conditions have changed between Time 1 (T1) and Time 2 (T2), Friedman’s tests for continuous variables and McNemar’s tests for categorical variables were performed using data from individuals who participated in both T1 and T2. The mean, SD, median, and interquartile range (IQR) of each BPS-J item at T1 were also calculated.

Structural validity

Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) for the one-factor model [15, 17, 20, 22] was performed after the Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin (KMO) test and Bartlett’s test of Sphericity. For goodness of fit, χ2/df, the Tucker-Lewis index (TLI), comparative fit index (CFI), and root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) were calculated. We determined the model to be good when TLI, CFI, and RMSEA were > 0.90, > 0.90, and < 0.06, respectively [49]. When the model did not meet these criteria, we eliminated items with low factor loadings (< 0.50) and analyzed the model again [50].

Additionally, exploratory factor analysis (EFA) with maximum-likelihood and Promax rotation was performed to confirm whether the one-factor model was appropriate for the BPS-J, and CFA was performed again based on the results of the EFA. The appropriate number of factors was determined by the factor number with eigenvalues greater than 1 in the EFA scree plot. The mean, SD, median, and IQR of the total BPS-J scores were calculated based on the structural validity results. These analyses were performed on the sum and average scores.

Criterion validity

Bivariate Spearman’s correlation analyses between the BPS-J total score and the following variables were performed: age, BSCS, GPS, sleep duration on workdays, days of feeling fatigue during daytime, days of feeling sleep loss during daytime, degree of viewing bedtime procrastination as a problem, and variables of MCTQ such as sleep duration on workdays, sleep duration on work-free days, midsleep on workdays, midsleep on work-free days, absolute social jetlag, absolute social jetlag ≥ 120 min, weekly sleep loss, working time, work start time, and work end time.

Reliability

Reliability was assessed using data from individuals who participated in both T1 and T2 in three ways: internal consistency, measurement error, and test–retest reliability. The internal consistency of each item and the BPS-J total score were confirmed using Cronbach’s α, MacDonald’s ω, and item total (I-T) correlation. The measurement error was confirmed by calculating the standard error of measurement (SEM) and the smallest detectable change (SDC) of the total score [51, 52]. Test–retest reliability was confirmed using intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) and Pearson’s correlation coefficient between T1 and T2.

Relation of the BPS-J to sleep-related variables

To examine the association of the BPS-J to sleep loss, sleep–wake phase delay, daytime fatigue, and view of bedtime procrastination as a problem, with reference to prior literature [15], the linear regressions for continuous outcomes and logistic regression for categorical outcomes were performed. The MCTQ and sleep-related variables [15] were used as the dependent variables. The GPS and BSCS, which were suggested to be associated with both the BPS and sleep-related variables in a previous study [15], were also included in the analysis models as covariates in addition to demographic information such as sex and age.

Results

Participants’ characteristics and the BPS-J

The participant selection process is shown in Fig. 1. Out of 600 participants, data from 574 participants were analyzed to estimate the structural and criterion validities, relation of the BPS-J to sleep-related variables, internal consistency, and SEM and SDC of the BPS-J. Among them, data from 280 participants were retested and analyzed to estimate the test–retest reliability of the BPS-J. No missing values were observed.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Flow chart of the participant selection process

Table 1 shows the demographic characteristics of the participants at T1 and T2. At T1, the mean age was 44.25 (SD = 12.84), and the proportion of males was 50%. Mean sleep duration on workdays and work-free days were 393.09 (SD = 69.33) min and 440.89 (SD = 93.10) min, respectively. At T2, the mean age was 44.34 (SD = 12.74), and the proportion of males was 51.4%. Mean sleep duration on workdays and work-free days were 394.50 (SD = 67.84) min and 443.11 (SD = 72.46) min, respectively. The characteristics of the participants at T1 and T2 were comparable, except in terms of the Self-Control Scale, the number of feeling fatigue, and the number of days feeling sleep loss.

Table 1 Participants’ characteristics

Table 2 shows the mean, SD, median, and IQR for each item of the BPS-J.

Table 2 Item characteristics of the BPS-J

Flowchart of the selection process of participants from participant recruitment to data analysis at Time 1 and Time 2.

Structural validity

Table 3 shows the results of the CFA. As the result of the KMO test was 0.86 (p < 0.001) and the result of Bartlett’s test was significant (χ2 = 2221.26, df = 28, p < 0.001), CFA was interpreted as appropriate. The values of the factor loading and fit indices of the one-factor model are listed in Table 3. Although the factor loadings for all items were p < 0.001, the fit indices were poor (χ2/df = 15.55, p < 0.001, TLI = 0.77, CFI = 0.83, RMSEA = 0.16). The factor loadings of Item 2, “I go to bed early if I have to get up early in the morning (invert scale),” and Item 3, “If it is time to turn off the lights at night I do it immediately (invert scale),” were under 0.50. Therefore, based on the value of factor loading, CFAs of the models with Item 2 eliminated (KMO = 0.86, p < 0.001; χ2 = 2221.26, df = 28, p < 0.001) and with Items 2 and 3 eliminated (KMO = 0.85, p < 0.001; χ2 = 2041.16, df = 21, p < 0.001) were additionally performed. The fit indices of the model with Item 2 eliminated were slightly better than those of the full-item model, except for the RMSEA (χ2/df = 18.92, p < 0.001, TLI = 0.77, CFI = 0.84, RMSEA = 0.18).

Table 3 Confirmatory factor analyses results of the BPS-J

As other models were suggested to be appropriate from the fit indices of the one-factor model, an EFA was performed. The eigenvalues of the EFA showed that the two-factor model was appropriate (Fig. 2). CFAs of the two-factor model were performed (KMO = 0.86, p < 0.001; χ2 = 2279.04, df = 36, p < 0.001). Although the two-factor full-item model was better than the one-factor model (χ2/df = 9.10, p < 0.001, TLI = 0.87, CFI = 0.91, RMSEA = 0.12), Item 2 was eliminated and confirmed again because of low factor loading (KMO = 0.86, p < 0.001; χ2 = 2221.26, df = 28, p < 0.001; χ2/df = 10.70, p < 0.001, TLI = 0.88, CFI = 0.92, RMSEA = 0.13). Factor 1 and 2 were interpreted as “Preparing for bedtime” and “Adherence to bedtime”, respectively.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Scree plot of the exploratory factor analysis

Based on the results of the CFA and EFA, the mean of mean score and mean of sum score of the BPS-J for the models with all the items, with Item 2 eliminated, and with Items 2 and 3 eliminated were calculated (Table 4). The mean sum scores were 24.32 (SD = 7.36), 21.69 (SD = 7.05), and 19.07 (SD = 6.36), and the median values were 25 (IQR = 19 – 29), 22 (IQR = 16 – 26), and 19 (IQR = 14 – 23), respectively. The mean average scores were 2.71 (SD = 0.81), 2.72 (SD = 0.87), 2.74 (SD = 0.91), and the median values were 2.78 (IQR = 2.11—3.22), 2.75 (IQR = 2.0 – 3.25), and 2.79 (IQR = 2.0 -3.29), respectively.

Table 4 Characteristics and reliability of the BPS-J total score

The scree plot of the exploratory factor analysis shows that only the eigenvalues of Factors 1 and 2 are greater than 1. Thus, the two-factor model is considered to be appropriate for the Japanese version of the Bedtime Procrastination Scale (BPS-J).

Criterion validity

Table 5 shows the bivariate correlation between the BPS-J and the variables. Regardless of the model, significant correlations between the BPS-J and the variables were shown, except for the degree of viewing bedtime procrastination as a problem, working time, and work start time.

Table 5 Bivariate correlation between the BPS-J and variables

Reliability

As presented in Table 4, Cronbach’s α was 0.86, 0.87, and 0.88, and McDonald's ω was 0.86, 0.87, and 0.88, respectively. Table 6 shows the I-T correlation, Cronbach’s α and McDonald's ω when each item is eliminated. The average I-T correlations of the models with all the items, with Item 2 eliminated, and with Items 2 and 3 eliminated were 0.58, 0.73, and 0.76, respectively. Cronbach’s α and McDonald's ω of factor 1, factor 2 with all items, factor 2 with Item 2 eliminated were 0.86 and 0.86, 0.76 and 76, and 0.79 and 0.79.

Table 6 I-T correlation, Cronbach’s α, and McDonald’s ω when each item is eliminated

Table 4 shows the SEM and SDC of the models with all the items, with Item 2 eliminated, and with Items 2 and 3 eliminated. The SEM values of the average score were 0.38, 0.41, and 0.42, and those of the MDC were 1.06, 1.13, and 1.18, respectively. The SEM values of the sum score were 3.51, 3.28, and 2.98, and those of the MDC were 9.74, 9.08, and 8.28, respectively.

As Table 4 shows, ICC and Pearson’s correlation were almost comparable between the versions (ICC = 0.77, 0.77, and 0.78; r = 0.78, 0.78, and 0.78, respectively). Each item of ICC was between 0.46 and 0.68 (Table 6).

Relation of the BPS-J to sleep-related variable

Table 7 shows the results of the multivariate linear and logistic regressions. BPS-J score was significantly related to the sleep-related variables defined by Kroese et al. [15, 19]: shorter sleep duration on workdays, more frequent days of feeling daytime fatigue, more frequent days of feeling sleep loss, and viewing bedtime procrastination as a problem less seriously.

Table 7 Results of the regression analyses between the BPS-J sum score and sleep-related variables

Significant relationships between the BPS-J and MCTQ variables were also found: shorter sleep duration on workdays, shorter sleep duration on work-free days, delayed midsleep on workdays, delayed midsleep on work-free days, and weekly sleep loss. There was no significant relationship between the BPS-J and absolute social jetlag.

Discussion

Except for structural validity, we confirmed that the BPS-J has acceptable validity and reliability, similar to other language versions. The two-factor version that eliminated Item 2 indicated slightly better validity than that of the full-item version. The factor loading of Items 2 and 3, especially Item 2, was not as high as that of other items, resulting in our consideration of eliminating Item 2 from the BPS-J scale. Behavioral patterns such as “promptly turning off the lights when getting up early the next morning” were suspected to be related to factors other than bedtime procrastination.

The structure of the two-factor model without Item 2 was better than that of the one-factor model or the full-item model for the BPS-J. The fit indices seemed to be lower than those of the other language versions, even in the two-factor model, in which the factor loading of items was eliminated. The factor loadings of Item 2 in the Korean version also seemed to be lower than those of the other items [20], similar to the trend noted for the BPS-J. Considering the fact that sleep duration among Japanese and Korean participants seemed to be shorter than those from other countries in the world [53], the trend implied by the poor fitting profile of Item 2 could be influenced by a mutually shared value of under-evaluation regarding getting sufficient sleep among societies with extremely short sleep duration40. The fact that the two-factor model after removing Item 2 fitted better than the one-factor model may not be peculiar to the BPS-J. The eigenvalues of the two components also reached 1 in the Polish and Korean versions, [17, 20] and the fit indices of the two-factor model were also better than the one-factor model for the Korean version20 as well as the BPS-J.

The mean total BPS-J score was almost the same as that of the original version. However, it seemed to be lower than that in the other language versions. Differences in the characteristics of the study participants could have caused discrepancies in the BPS scores. The participants of studies examining the BPS-J and BPS original versions were daytime workers [15], whereas those of the other language versions were students [17, 20, 22]. Considering the social constraint effect on bedtime procrastination [54], daytime workers may undertake more severe social responsibility than students [15, 17, 20, 22].

The criterion validity was consistent with other language versions of the BPS, considering that the BPS-J is associated with general procrastination, self-control, and sleep variables.

The reliability of the BPS-J was found to be sufficient, based on the Cronbach's α [55], McDonald's ω, ICC, [56] and correlation [57] between T1 and T2. The level of Cronbach's α was similar to that of the other language versions [17, 19, 20, 22, 58], but lower than that of the original version [15]. McDonald's ω, SEM, and MDC, which had previously only been calculated in the Polish version, were almost the same as those reported in the Polish version [17]. The correlation between T1 and T2, which was calculated in the Polish and Spanish versions, appears to be almost comparable to the Spanish version [22] and higher than the Polish version [17]. The correlation coefficient in the BPS-J was higher than that in the Polish version, possibly because the interval between T1 and T2 in the BPS-J was shorter than that in the Polish version [17].

The BPS-J is believed to enhance the assessment of underlying factors that contribute to ISS among daytime workers in Japan. This facilitates the implementation of interventions specifically targeting BP for those with ISS. This approach may be more effective than often ineffective traditional advice of simply telling individuals to go to bed earlier [59]. Such an approach allows for the implementation of specific behavioral interventions aimed at curbing BP by behavior replacement [60], thereby potentially reducing the prevalence of ISS.

This study has some limitations. The validity of the ISS diagnosis is subject to potential inaccuracies as it relies solely on participants’ self-reported accounts, although we devised a way to exclude dishonest respondents by eliminating those who responded inappropriately to the dummy question. Participants were limited to daytime workers, similar to the original version of the BPS. The BPS-J should be applied to other populations (e.g., students) with caution, based on the difference noted in the results of the BPS-J with daytime workers and other language versions of the BPS with students [20, 22]. Individuals whose bedtime procrastination tendency was so severe that they could not perform daytime work were not included in this study. Additional validation studies including such individuals are necessary for further clarification.

Regression analyses indicated that the BPS-J was associated with sleep-related variables, as with other language versions [15, 17, 20, 22]. The degree of “viewing bedtime procrastination as a problem,” the dependent variable used in Kroese et al.’s study [19], was similar to that used in a previous study [19]. This relationship may explain why patients with ISS hardly changed their sleep habits despite advice from sleep professionals [59]. Therefore, in the context of ISS, awareness of bedtime procrastination problems should be increased among people with the condition. Bedtime procrastination was also related to a delayed sleep–wake phase tendency and short sleep duration. These relationships are consistent with those reported in previous studies [61].

Conclusion

We developed and validated the BPS-J, and constructed a two-factor model with Item 2 eliminated; the version with Item 2 eliminated was found to be more suitable for Japanese daytime workers. This study sheds light on the relationship between bedtime procrastination and sleep duration in Japanese populations, while also highlighting differences in bedtime procrastination tendencies between other countries and Japan. High BPS-J scores were indicative of increased harmful sleep-related variables in Japanese daytime workers, although the causal relationship remained unclear given the cross-sectional study design. Further studies are needed to clarify the causal relationships and degree of necessity of changing bedtime procrastination.

Availability of data and materials

The data will be made available by the corresponding author upon request.

Abbreviations

BPS-J:

Japanese Version of the Bedtime Procrastination Scale

BSCS:

Brief Self-Control Scale

CFA:

Confirmatory Factor Analysis

CFI:

Comparative Fit Index

COVID-19:

Coronavirus Disease 2019

EFA:

Exploratory Factor Analysis

GPS:

General Procrastination Scale

ICC:

Interclass Correlation Coefficient

IQR:

Interquartile Range

ISS:

Insufficient Sleep Syndrome

I-T:

Item Total

KMO:

Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin

MCTQ:

Munich ChronoType Questionnaire

RMSEA:

Root Mean Square Error of Approximation

SD:

Standard Deviation

SDC:

Smallest Detectable Change

SEM:

Standard Error of Measurement

T1:

Time 1

T2:

Time 2

TLI:

Tucker–Lewis index

References

  1. Zhu B, Shi C, Park CG, Zhao X, Reutrakul S. Effects of sleep restriction on metabolism-related parameters in healthy adults: A comprehensive review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Sleep Med Rev. 2019;45:18–30 W.B. Saunders Ltd.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Itani O, Jike M, Watanabe N, Kaneita Y. Short sleep duration and health outcomes: a systematic review, meta-analysis, and meta-regression. Sleep Med. 2017;1(32):246–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Chaput JP, Dutil C, Featherstone R, Ross R, Giangregorio L, Saunders TJ, et al. Sleep timing, sleep consistency, and health in adults: a systematic review. Appl Physiol Nutr Metab. 2020;45(10):S232–47.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Scott J, Kallestad H, Vedaa O, Sivertsen B, Etain B. Sleep disturbances and first onset of major mental disorders in adolescence and early adulthood: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Sleep Med Rev. 2021;57:101429 W.B. Saunders Ltd.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Hu B, Liu C, Mou T, Luo F, Lv T, Qian C, et al. Meta-Analysis of Sleep Deprivation Effects on Patients With Depression. Front Psychiatry. 2021;12(November):1–9.

    Google Scholar 

  6. Devore EE, Grodstein F, Schernhammer ES. Sleep duration in relation to cognitive function among older adults: a systematic review of observational studies. Neuroepidemiology. 2016;46:57–78 S. Karger AG.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Lowe CJ, Safati A, Hall PA. The neurocognitive consequences of sleep restriction: a meta-analytic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2017;80:586–604 Elsevier Ltd.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Boardman JM, Porcheret K, Clark JW, Andrillon T, Cai AWT, Anderson C, et al. The impact of sleep loss on performance monitoring and error-monitoring: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Sleep Med Rev. 2021;58:101490 W.B. Saunders Ltd.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Takaesu Y, Kanda Y, Nagahama Y, Shiroma A, Ishii M, Hashimoto T, et al. Delayed sleep-wake rhythm is associated with cognitive dysfunction, social dysfunction, and deteriorated quality of life in patients with major depressive disorder. Front Psychiatry. 2022;7:13.

    Google Scholar 

  10. Palmer CA, Alfano CA. Sleep and emotion regulation: An organizing, integrative review. Sleep Med Rev. 2017;31:6–16 W.B. Saunders Ltd.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Tomaso CC, Johnson AB, Nelson TD. The effect of sleep deprivation and restriction on mood, emotion, and emotion regulation: Three meta-analyses in one. Sleep. 2021;44(6):1–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Pires GN, Bezerra AG, Tufik S, Andersen ML. Effects of acute sleep deprivation on state anxiety levels: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Sleep Med. 2016;24:109–18 Elsevier B.V.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. van Veen MM, Lancel M, Şener O, Verkes RJ, Bouman EJ, Rutters F. Observational and experimental studies on sleep duration and aggression: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Sleep Med Rev. 2022;64:101661.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Palmer CA, John-Henderson NA, Bawden H, Massey A, Powell SL, Hilton A, et al. Sleep Restriction Reduces Positive Social Emotions and Desire to Connect with Others. Sleep. 2022 https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/zsac265

  15. Kroese FM, de Ridder DTD, Evers C, Adriaanse MA. Bedtime procrastination: Introducing a new area of procrastination. Front Psychol. 2014;5(JUN):1–8.

    Google Scholar 

  16. Hill VM, Rebar AL, Ferguson SA, Shriane AE, Vincent GE. Go to bed! A systematic review and meta-analysis of bedtime procrastination correlates and sleep outcomes. Sleep Med Rev. 2022;110:131932. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tet.2021.131932.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Herzog-Krzywoszanska R, Krzywoszanski L. Bedtime Procrastination, Sleep-Related Behaviors, and Demographic Factors in an Online Survey on a Polish Sample. Front Neurosci. 2019;13(September):1–15. Available from: https://research.bowdoin.edu/rothbart-temperament-questionnaires/instrument-descriptions/the-childrens-behavior-questionnaire/.

    Google Scholar 

  18. Kadzikowska-Wrzosek R. Insufficient sleep among adolescents: the role of bedtime procrastination, Chronotype and Autonomous vs. Controlled Motivational Regulations. Curr Psychol. 2020;39(3):1031–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Kroese FM, Evers C, Adriaanse MA, de Ridder DTD. Bedtime procrastination: A self-regulation perspective on sleep insufficiency in the general population. J Health Psychol. 2016;21(5):853–62.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. An H, Chung S, Suh S. Validation of the Korean bedtime procrastination scale in young adults. J Sleep Med. 2019;16(1):41–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Exelmans L, van den Bulck J. “Glued to the Tube”: the interplay between self-control, evening television viewing, and bedtime procrastination. Communic Res. 2017;48(4):594–616.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Brando-Garrido C, Montes-Hidalgo J, Limonero JT, Gómez-Romero, María J Tomás-Sábado J. Spanish Version of the Bedtime Procrastination Scale: Cross-Cultural Adaptation and Psychometric Evaluation in Sample of Nursing Students. Psychol Rep. 2021;0(0):1–15. Available from: https://research.bowdoin.edu/rothbart-temperament-questionnaires/instrument-descriptions/the-childrens-behavior-questionnaire/.

  23. Coutrot A, Lazar AS, Richards M, Manley E, Wiener JM, Dalton RC, et al. Reported sleep duration reveals segmentation of the adult life-course into three phases. Nat Commun. 2022;13(1):7697. Available from: https://www.nature.com/articles/s41467-022-34624-8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. Sivertsen B, Harvey AG, Gradisar M, Pallesen S, Hysing M. Delayed sleep–wake phase disorder in young adults: prevalence and correlates from a national survey of Norwegian university students. Sleep Med. 2021;1(77):184–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Tomishima S, Komada Y, Tanioka K, Okajima I, Inoue Y. Prevalence and factors associated with the risk of delayed sleep-wake phase disorder in japanese youth. Front Psychiatry. 2022;13:13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Otsuki R, Matsui K, Yoshiike T, Nagao K, Utsumi T, Tsuru A, et al. Decrease in Social Zeitgebers Is Associated With Worsened Delayed Sleep-Wake Phase Disorder: Findings During the Pandemic in Japan. Front Psychiatry. 2022;9:13.

    Google Scholar 

  27. Yuksel D, McKee GB, Perrin PB, Alzueta E, Caffarra S, Ramos-Usuga D, et al. Sleeping when the world locks down: Correlates of sleep health during the COVID-19 pandemic across 59 countries. Sleep Health. 2021;7(2):134–42.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Al-Ajlouni YA, al Ta’ani O, Shamaileh G, Mushasha R, Makarem N, Duncan DT. Effects of the COVID-19 pandemic on sleep health among Middle Eastern and North African (MENA) populations: a systematic review of the literature. BMJ Open. 2022;12(12):e066964.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Salehinejad MA, Azarkolah A, Ghanavati E, Nitsche MA. Circadian disturbances, sleep difficulties and the COVID-19 pandemic. Sleep Med. 2022;1(91):246–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Oliveira A, Pereira B, Rosário P, Magalhães P. Bedtime procrastination during the second COVID-19 lockdown in Portugal. Sleep Science. 2022;15(1):105–11.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Deng Y, Ye B, Yang Q. COVID-19 related emotional stress and bedtime procrastination among college students in China: a moderated mediation model. Nat Sci Sleep. 2022;14:1437–47.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Son SM. Correlation Study among the Bedtime Procrastination and Sleep Disorder, and Depression of University Students during COVID-19 Pandemic in Korea. 2021;25. Available from: http://annalsofrscb.ro.

  33. Gagnier JJ, Lai J, Mokkink LB, Terwee CB. COSMIN reporting guideline for studies on measurement properties of patient-reported outcome measures. Qual Life Res. 2021;30(8):2197–218.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. American Academy of Sleep Medicine. The International Classification of Sleep Disorders – Third Edition (ICSD-3). Am Acad Sleep Med. 2014

  35. Marx RG, Menezes A, Horovitz L, Jones EC, Warren RF. A comparison of two time intervals for test-retest reliability of health status instruments. J Clin Epidemiol. 2003;56(8):730–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Tangney JP, Baumeister RF, Boone AL. High self-control predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success. J Pers. 2004;72(2):271–322.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Duckworth AL. The significance of self-control. Proc Natl Acade Sci U S A. 2011;108:2639–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Willems YE, Boesen N, Li J, Finkenauer C, Bartels M. The heritability of self-control: A meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2019;100:324–34 Elsevier Ltd.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Ozaki Y, Goto T, Kobayashi M, Kutsuzawa G. Reliability and validity of the Japanese translation of Brief Self-Control Scale (BSCS-J). Japanese J Psychol. 2016;87(2):144–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Lay CH. At last, my research article on procrastination. J Res Pers. 1986;20(4):474–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Steel P. Arousal, avoidant and decisional procrastinators: Do they exist? Pers Individ Dif. 2010;48(8):926–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Ferrari JR, Tice DM. Procrastination as a Self-Handicap for Men and Women: A Task-Avoidance Strategy in a Laboratory Setting. J Res Pers. 2000;34(1):73–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Hayashi J. Development of Japanese Version of General Procrastination Scale. Japanese J Pers. 2007;15(2):246–8. Available from: https://ci.nii.ac.jp/naid/110006318033#cit%0Ahttps://www.jstage.jst.go.jp/article/personality/15/2/15_2_246/_article/-char/ja/.

    Google Scholar 

  44. Roenneberg T, Wirz-Justice A, Merrow M. Life between clocks: Daily temporal patterns of human chronotypes. J Biol Rhythms. 2003;18(1):80–90.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Kitamura S, Hida A, Aritake S, Higuchi S, Enomoto M, Kato M, et al. Validity of the Japanese version of the Munich ChronoType Questionnaire. Chronobiol Int. 2014;31(7):845–50. Available from: https://doi.org/10.3109/07420528.2014.914035.

  46. Chang CS, Chang LY, Wu CC, Chang HY. Associations between social jetlag trajectories and body mass index among young adults. Sleep. 2024;47(1):zsad270.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Feng J, Meng X, Wang C, Ke P, Tian Q, He Y, et al. Associations of social jetlag with depressive symptoms among adults in Southern China: a cross-sectional study. Chronobiol Int. 2023;40(6):783–94.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Gamboa Madeira S, Reis C, Paiva T, Moreira CS, Nogueira P, Roenneberg T. Social jetlag, a novel predictor for high cardiovascular risk in blue-collar workers following permanent atypical work schedules. J Sleep Res. 2021;30(6):e13380.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  49. Hu LT, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct Equ Model Struc Equ Model. 1999;6(1):1–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Hair JFJ, Black WC, Babin BJ, Anderson RE. Multivariate Data Analysis (7th Edition). Upper Saddle River: NJ: Pearson Prentice Hall; 2009.

  51. Ries JD, Echternach JL, Nof L, Gagnon Blodgett M. Test-Retest Reliability and Minimal Detectable Change Scores for the Timed ‘Up & Go’ Test, the Six-Minute Walk Test, and Gait Speed in People With Alzheimer Disease. 2009. Available from: www.ptjournal.org.

  52. de Vet HCW, Terwee CB, Knol DL, Bouter LM. When to use agreement versus reliability measures. J Clin Epidemiol. 2006;59(10):1033–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Organization for Economic Co-Operation and Development. Time Use Across the World. Gend Data PORTAL. Available from: https://www.oecd.org/gender/data/OECD_1564_TUSupdatePortal.xlsx. [Cited 2022 Dec 15].

  54. Kühnel J, Syrek CJ, Dreher A. Why don’t you go to bed on time? A daily diary study on the relationships between chronotype, self-control resources and the phenomenon of bedtime procrastination. Front Psychol. 2018;9(FEB):1–11.

    Google Scholar 

  55. Taber KS. The Use of Cronbach’s Alpha When Developing and Reporting Research Instruments in Science Education. Res Sci Educ. 2018;48(6):1273–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Koo TK, Li MY. A Guideline of Selecting and Reporting Intraclass Correlation Coefficients for Reliability Research. J Chiropr Med. 2016;15(2):155–63.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  57. Calamia M, Markon K, Tranel D. The robust reliability of neuropsychological measures: meta-analyses of test-retest correlations. Clin Neuropsychol. 2013;27(7):1077–105.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Sirois FM, Nauts S, Molnar DS. Self-compassion and bedtime procrastination: an emotion regulation perspective. Mindfulness (N Y). 2019;10(3):434–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Baumann-Vogel H, Hoff S, Valko PO, Poryazova R, Werth E, Baumann CR. Extending sleep to confirm insufficient sleep syndrome is challenging. J Sleep Res. 2021;30(3):13109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Jeoung S, Jeon H, Yang HC, An H, Suh S. A randomized controlled trial of a behavioral intervention for decreasing bedtime procrastination using a wait-list control group in a non-clinical sample of young adults. Sleep Med. 2023;108:114–23.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Zhu L, Meng D, Ma X, Guo J, Mu L. Sleep timing and hygiene practices of high bedtime procrastinators: A direct observational study. Fam Pract. 2021;37(6):779–84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

This work was supported by the Intramural Research Grant for Neurological and Psychiatric Disorders of NCNP (2–1), the Ministry of Health, Labour and Welfare, Government of Japan (Grant numbers #21FA1002 and #22FA2001), and a Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research (22K13847 and 22K07590).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Concept and design: KK and MH. Analysis: MH. Interpretation of analysis: KK, AK, and MH. Drafting of the manuscript: MH. Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: MH, AK, TY, KM, SK, AT, KN, NA, TU, MI, MS, ET, McF, MmF, SO, TE, DN, and KK. Supervision: KK and DN. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kenichi Kuriyama.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

This study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the NCNP in Japan (A2021-111) and was performed in accordance with the Helsinki Declaration. Informed consent was obtained online from all participants. All participants were asked to express their consent to participate by clicking the URL on the questionnaire after reading the explanatory description regarding ethical considerations. They were allowed to stop responding to the questionnaire and withdraw from the study even after agreeing to participate.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

Payments were made to DN from AIG General Insurance Company, Ltd, Startia, Inc., en-power, Inc., and MD.net outside the submitted work. MH, AK, TY, KM, SK, AT, KN, NA, TU, MI, MS, ET, McF, MmF, SO, TE, and KK have no financial conflicts of interest disclosure concerning the study.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1: Appendix 1.

Suggestions about comprehensibility, relevance, and comprehensiveness.

Additional file 2: Appendix 2.

Translation of Instructions and items of the Bedtime Procrastination Scale.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Hazumi, M., Kawamura, A., Yoshiike, T. et al. Development and validation of the Japanese version of the Bedtime Procrastination Scale (BPS-J). BMC Psychol 12, 56 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-024-01557-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-024-01557-4

Keywords