Skip to main content

Validation of the South Korean Version of the Beliefs about Emotions Scale

Abstract

Background

Beliefs about the unacceptability of experiencing or expressing negative emotions can contribute to diverse psychological symptoms and associated with poor treatment outcomes and low treatment attempts. The Beliefs about Emotions Scale (BES) was developed to assess such beliefs based on the cognitive-behavioral models; however, no study has reported on the psychometric properties of the BES in Korea. The present study aimed to cross-culturally adapt and validate the BES for the Korean population (BES-K).

Methods

The BES-K was administered to 592 Korean adults (323 men and 269 women) aged 20–59 years. Exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis were used to assess the factor model of the scale. Pearson correlation coefficients were used to evaluate the relationships between the BES-K and other psychological measures.

Results

The result showed a two-factor model of the BES-K, with Factor 1 relating to Interpersonal and Factor 2 representing Intrapersonal aspects. The scale had significant yet moderately low correlations with measures of depression, anxiety, and difficulties in emotion regulation.

Conclusion

The BES-K is a useful instrument in evaluating the beliefs about emotions in the Korean population.

Peer Review reports

Background

Processing emotional states has a crucial role in a variety of health conditions. There is growing evidence that beliefs about the unacceptability of experiencing and expressing negative emotions are related to diverse clinical problems and symptoms such as depression [1], anxiety [2], somatic symptoms [3], eating disorders [4], chronic fatigue syndrome [5], and irritable bowel syndrome [6]. Cognitive behavioral models propose that such beliefs contribute to the etiology and maintenance of various symptoms and disorders [7,8,9]. In addition, it has been argued that these beliefs are a transdiagnostic vulnerability factor that gives rise to various problems and is associated with poor treatment outcomes and low treatment attempts [10, 11].

Cognitive behavioral models of chronic fatigue syndrome suggest that these beliefs may develop in some venerable individuals nurtured in an environment where the display of negative emotions and difficulties was met with punishment or lack of positive feedback [4, 8, 10]. Believing in positive attitude such as “being happy” and “never giving up” is overvalued, while displaying any negative thoughts and emotions is not tolerated [4]. This emotionally invalidating environment leads to the beliefs that experiencing and expressing negative thoughts or emotions are unhelpful and unacceptable, cause adverse social consequences, and thus should be avoided, suppressed, or at least not overtly revealed [4].

These beliefs contribute to difficulties in many ways [10]. People who have such beliefs have difficulty in properly understanding and taking care of themselves, as there is substantial evidence that emotions help people to reorganize, understand, and interpret their subjective state [12]. Moreover, while experiencing distress, these individuals may engage in self-silencing behaviors to create or maintain safe and close relationships by “putting on a brave face”. Silencing certain negative thoughts and emotions and not seeking help have a counterproductive effect of blocking social support that may otherwise be beneficial [4, 10, 13, 14]. Additionally, suppression or avoidance of unwanted thoughts and emotions ironically increases them, which in turn results in higher distress and associated mental and physical symptoms such as numbness, fatigue, and bowel disturbance [10].

Few studies have attempted to measure beliefs about emotions. There are several scales measuring behavioral responses that are closely related to beliefs about emotions, such as avoidance and suppression [15], but few studies have evaluated the underlying beliefs that lead to those behaviors [10]. It is true that some emotion regulation questionnaires that measure individuals’ strategies to modulate negative emotions partly contain items evaluating beliefs about emotions [16, 17]. However, as most of the items assess behavioral strategies in time of experiencing negative emotions, such as “engaging in goal-directed behaviors” and “refraining from impulsive behaviors”, these scales have rarely been used to measure beliefs about the emotions themselves. Therefore, a new questionnaire that can briefly assess such beliefs has been required.

Rimes and Chalder [10] developed the Beliefs about Emotions Scale (BES) to assess negative beliefs about experiencing and expressing negative thoughts and emotions based on cognitive behavioral models in the United Kingdom. The BES items were designed and selected to illustrate the types of such beliefs identified in clinical reports and cognitive behavioral models [10]. The BES is a 12-item scale, preferably brief and easy to use. It is unidimensional with adequate internal consistency (Cronbach’s α = 0.91) and validity in its development study [10]. The BES was cross-culturally validated in another cultural context—Brazil, and the Brazilian Portuguese version [11] showed a two-factor model with fair internal consistency (Cronbach’s α = 0.86) and validity. The first factor (Item 1, 2, 3, 5, 6, 8, 9, 10, 11) accounted for “emotions and their expressions as signs of weakness”, and the second factor (Item 4, 7, 12) was linked to “emotional self-control”. The disagreement in the factor structures of the two versions may reflect cultural differences [11], which lays the groundwork for the discussion on cultural differences in beliefs about emotions.

The BES has been utilized in various studies exploring and intervening in individuals with chronic fatigue syndrome [5, 18,19,20], irritable bowel syndrome [6, 14, 21], anorexia and bulimia nervosa [4], perfectionism [22,23,24], depression [20, 23, 24], anxiety [20], and fibromyalgia [21, 25].

According to Rimes and Chalder [10], the benefit of measuring these beliefs that derive behavioral responses (e.g., suppression and avoidance), rather than evaluating behaviors themselves, is that individuals can easily assess and self-report their beliefs. For instance, individuals may not recognize the extent of their suppression or avoidance behaviors if these behaviors are overlearned and become relatively automatic behavioral responses. Another advantage is that measuring beliefs is more clinically useful as different forms of modern cognitive psychotherapy and psychoeducation try to address and modify these negative beliefs about emotions and increase psychological flexibility [10]. For example, third wave cognitive behavioral approaches based on mindfulness such as MBSR (Mindfulness Based Stress [26, 27]), DBT (Dialectical Behavior therapy [28]), ACT (Acceptance and Commitment Therapy [29]), and MBCT (Mindfulness Based Cognitive Therapy [30]) develop nonjudgmental awareness and radical acceptance of emotions, thoughts, and bodily sensations.

To date, no studies have reported on the psychometric properties of the BES in Korea. This study aimed to cross-culturally adapt and validate the BES in a Korean population sample (BES-K). In Korea, without a reliable and valid scale to measure beliefs about emotions, relevant studies have been quite limited. For instance, given that modern cognitive behavioral models state such beliefs are key variables in the etiology and maintenance of a variety of symptoms and disorders including chronic fatigue syndrome, irritable bowel syndrome, and eating disorders [7,8,9], it is surprising that no study exploring such beliefs in individuals with these symptoms can be identified in the existing Korean literature. Thus, there has been a need for a proper scale to evaluate beliefs about emotions that can encourage relevant studies and interventions. Further, to the best of our knowledge, there is only one study analyzing the factorial structure of the BES for other cultural contexts, the Brazilian Portuguese version [11]. This study also aimed to contribute to the discussion on the cultural differences of beliefs about emotions by providing additional cultural data on such beliefs.

Therefore, the objectives of this study were: (1) to investigate the psychometric properties of the BES-K using both exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses (EFA and CFA). Performing EFA was necessary instead of doing CFA directly, considering that the factor structures of the previous two versions (the original English and the Brazilian version) are not congruent, and people in different cultures differ in their emotional experiences and expressions [31,32,33]; (2) to compare factor structure models found in the previous studies [10, 11] with the BES-K model; and (3) to examine the convergent validity of the BES-K by examining associations with other criterion scales.

Methods

Participants and sampling

As a part of a two-year government funded research project, the national online survey was implemented according to the Korean population census standard, considering sociodemographic factors such as sex, age, and residential area. The inclusion criteria for the sample were: 1) age between 20 and 59 years and 2) reading and writing proficiency in Korean. Originally, 845 participants enrolled in the survey; 608 participants completed the survey (72.0%). The exclusion criterion was insufficient effort responding (e.g., “leaving items unanswered” and “using the same response repeatedly”) [34]. The inclusion of these responses into the dataset can have various unexpected and unwanted effects on relationships being examined; thus, the removal of such responses is suggested [35, 36]. As our Internet-based survey was designed such that the participants could not skip any questions without ticking their responses, there were no participants who left items unanswered. However, 16 participants ticked the same response (number) consecutively and were therefore excluded. The final sample consisted of 592 participants (54.6% men and 45.4% women).

This study received ethical approval from the Institutional Review Board (IRB) to which the researchers are affiliated. All study protocols were performed in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. A survey was conducted between July and August 2019 by a confidential Internet-based survey company that utilizes a firewall (WAF) and secure socket layer (SSL) on its securities and encryption. The survey took approximately 30 min to complete, and vouchers worth two dollars were provided to the participants as compensation.

Instruments

The Beliefs about Emotions Scale (BES)

The original BES [10] has 12 items that assess beliefs about the unacceptability of experiencing and expressing negative thoughts and emotions (e.g., “I should be able to control my emotions”). Items are rated on a 7-point Likert scale from 0 to 6, with high scores demonstrating more unacceptability of negative thoughts and emotions. The original BES has a single factor with good internal consistency (Cronbach’s α = 0.91).

To develop the Korean version of the BES (BES-K), the following steps were taken based on the cross-cultural adaptation process guidelines: (a) translation, (b) back-translation, (c) committee review, (d) pretesting, and (e) final consensus [37, 38]. (a) After obtaining permission to validate the BES from the original authors, two Korean researchers fluent in both languages translated the original English BES into Korean. (b) A bilingual researcher familiar with both cultures and languages performed back-translations. (c) The research team comprising 20 trained counselors with master’s and Ph.D. degrees in counseling psychology compared it to the original English version to discuss whether there were any discrepancies. There was a semantic discrepancy on idiom “think less of me” in Item 3. Modification was made to this item until there was no feedback from researchers. Further, some researchers suggested a more natural Korean word for “a sign of weakness” in Item 6 and Item 11. The research team came to a consensus on the suggested word, so modifications were made to the items. (d) A pilot test was conducted on a sample of 30 undergraduate and graduate students to ensure readability and comprehensibility of the scale. After completing the questionnaire, they were also asked about their thoughts or responses to the items. Based on these comments, minor revisions were made to improve the sentence fluency of the items until the research team reached a consensus. (e) Finally, a professor at an American university (Ph.D. in rehabilitation psychology) and one at a Korean university (Ph.D. in counseling psychology) confirmed the final version tested in the present study.

The Brief Symptom Inventory-18 (BSI-18)

The BSI-18 [39] has 18 items that assess psychological distress during the past seven days. It includes items assessing depression (e.g., “Feeling no interest in things”), anxiety (e.g., “Feeling tense or keyed up”), and somatization (e.g., “Nausea or upset stomach”). Each item is rated on a 5-point Likert scale from 0 to 4, and all three dimensions have six items each. Possible total scores vary from 0 to 72 points, with high scores indicating greater psychological distress. The BSI-18 showed fair internal consistency (Cronbach’s α = 0.84, 0.79, and 0.74 for depression, anxiety, and somatization, respectively). The Korean version of the BSI-18 [40] also reported fair internal consistency (Cronbach’s α = 0.80, 0.81, and 0.73 for depression, anxiety, and somatization, respectively). Based on the significant positive relationship of the BES with depression and anxiety from previous studies [10, 11], only these two subscales were used in the present study. Cronbach’s α of the BSI-18 in this study was 0.90 for depression and 0.91 for anxiety.

The Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale-16 (DERS-16)

The DERS-16 [41] has 16 items that assess an individual’s typical level of emotion regulation. It assesses five dimensions of emotion regulation difficulties: non-acceptance of negative emotions (e.g., “When I am upset, I feel like I am weak”), inability to engage in goal-directed behaviors when distressed (e.g., “ When I am upset, I have difficulty thinking about anything else”), difficulties controlling impulsive behaviors when distressed (e.g., “When I am upset, I become out of control”), limited access to emotion regulation strategies perceived as effective (e.g., “When I am upset, I start to feel very bad about myself”), and lack of emotional clarity (e.g., “I am confused about how I feel”). Items are rated on a 5-point Likert scale from 1 to 5, and possible total scores vary from 16 to 80, with high scores reflecting more difficulty in emotion regulation. The DERS-16 showed good internal consistency (Cronbach’s α = 0.92). The psychometric properties of the Korean version of the DERS-16 [42] were as strong as the original (Cronbach’s α = 0.92). Cronbach’s α of the DERS-16 in this study was 0.93.

Statistical analysis

Data analyses in this study were performed using the SPSS version 21.0 and AMOS version 21.0. First, the distribution, central tendency, and dispersion of all variables were inspected. Kline [43] recommended that none of variables should exceed the standard value of skewness (≤|2.0|) and kurtosis (≤|4.0|) to verify a fair level of normality of the data. Second, a split-half method [44, 45] was chosen to allow for independent EFA and CFA. The dataset was randomly split into two halves. One half (subsample I) was a training sample where EFA would be performed to discover the number and nature of latent factors inherent to the BES, and the other half (subsample II) was a testing sample where the structural model identified from EFA would be tested through CFA. Third, EFA using maximum likelihood estimation (MLE) was performed on the 12 BES items in subsample I (n = 270). Oblique rotation of the items was done because it has advantages if one or more components are somehow related rather than independent [46]. Scree test [47] and eigenvalue criterion [48] were employed to determine the appropriate number of factors to retain. Items with a factor loading above 0.40 were retained. Fourth, CFA was performed on the retained BES items in subsample II (n = 322) and other alternative models proposed in previous studies. Model fit was evaluated based on chi-square (χ2/df), comparative fit index (CFI), Tucker Lewis index (TLI), normed fit index (NFI), standardized root mean square residual (SRMR), and root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), following the standards of previous studies [46, 49,50,51]. Akaike information criterion (AIC) was also employed to compare several non-nested models. Finally, we examined the validity of the scale. The convergent validity of the scale was evaluated by Pearson correlation coefficients.

Results

Socio-demographic characteristics

The participants were 592 Korean adults (323 men and 269 women) aged between 20 and 59 years (M = 39.7, SD = 9.7). The age distribution was as follows: 20.6% were 20–29 years, 27.7% were 30–39 years, 31.3% were 40–49 years, and 20.4% were 50–59 years. Most of the participants (87.0%) had received or had been receiving post-secondary education (e.g., college or university education), with only 12.5% having received secondary education. Moreover, most of the participants (79.4%) were employed or self-employed, 7.3% were housewives, 5.9% were unemployed, and 5.6% were students. Subsample I consisted of 270 participants (55.2% men and 44.8% women) with a mean age of 40.6 years (SD = 9.8), and Subsample II consisted of 322 participants (54.0% men and 46.0% women) with a mean age of 39 years (SD = 9.6).

Preliminary analysis

All variables were first examined on normal distribution. None of them exceeded the standard value of skewness (≤|2.0|) and kurtosis (≤|4.0|).

Exploratory factor analysis

The suitability of the data for factor analysis was inspected by assessing the sample fits of the Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin and the Bartlett’s sphericity test. The Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin sample fit was 0.89, exceeding the acceptable value of 0.6, and Bartlett’s sphericity test reached statistical significance (χ2 = 1,497.097, df = 66, p = 0.00). The results indicated the factorability of the correlation matrix. The 12 items of the BES were subjected to maximum likelihood estimation (MLE) and Oblimin rotation. The following criteria were used to determine the most appropriate number of factors: (a) a minimum eigenvalue of 1.0 criterion, (b) a minimum of four items in each factor, (c) deletion of items with factor loadings less than 0.40, and (d) conceptual coherence of each factor [52]. As shown in Table 1, a two-factor structure was suggested, with a total explained variance of 57.6%. Factor loadings of Item 1 were less than 0.40 on both factors, and it was therefore deleted. This resulted in seven items in the first subscale and four items in the second subscale. However, the two-factor model of the BES-K was not consistent with the dimensionality of the original English version. Factor 1 of the BES-K explained the beliefs about emotions at the interpersonal and social level, emphasizing that individuals often regulate and control emotions considering the negative social consequences of their emotions; Factor 2 explained the beliefs at the intrapersonal and personal level, focusing on whether people should be able to control or cope with negative thoughts and emotions. Therefore, Factor 1 was labeled as Interpersonal, and Factor 2 was labeled as Intrapersonal.

Table 1 The two-factor structure from exploratory factor analysis (EFA) in sample I (n = 270)

Confirmatory factor analysis

Confirmatory factor analysis was performed on the BES-K model (11 items, with two factors, e.g., Interpersonal and Intrapersonal), as well as other alternative models. Alternative models were included to check whether they might better represent our data. Table 2 shows that only the BES-K model had acceptable model fit indices. For the comparison of the four models, we used AIC value, with lower value representing a better fit. The lowest AIC value was observed in the BES-K model. Further, Fig. 1 shows that the standardized regression weights of all 11 items of the BES-K were between 0.65 and 0.82, and each of them reached statistical significance (p < 0.001).

Table 2 Confirmatory factor analysis for four forms of the BES
Fig. 1
figure 1

Confirmatory factor analysis of the BES-K

Internal consistency

Internal consistency of the BES-K was calculated for the scale as a whole (Cronbach’s α = 0.90) as well as for each subscale (Cronbach’s α = 0.89 for Interpersonal and 0.78 for Intrapersonal), indicating fair reliability.

Validity

Convergent validity was evaluated by analyzing Pearson correlation coefficients between the BES-K and other questionnaires, namely, the BSI-18, and the DERS-16. The BES-K showed significant yet moderately low correlations with the BSI depression (r = 0.27, p < 0.01), the BSI anxiety (r = 0.21, p < 0.01), and the DERS-16 (r = 0.36, p < 0.01). Interpersonal subscale also showed significant positive correlations with the BSI depression, (r = 0.33, p < 0.01), the BSI anxiety, (r = 0.29, p < 0.01), and the DERS-16 (r = 0.47, p < 0.01), whereas Intrapersonal subscale was not significantly related to them.

Discussion

The BES, a brief measure of beliefs about experiencing and expressing negative thoughts and emotions, was developed in the United Kingdom and widely used in various studies, including chronic fatigue syndrome, irritable bowel syndrome, anorexia nervosa, bulimia nervosa, perfectionism, depression, anxiety, and fibromyalgia. The BES was cross-culturally adapted to another culture, Brazil, and the factor structure of this Brazilian Portuguese version was found to be different from the original English version, laying the groundwork for the discussion on cultural differences in beliefs about emotions. The present study aimed to contribute to the discussion on cultural differences in beliefs about emotions by evaluating the scale in the Korean context.

Running both EFA and CFA was necessary because they would identify and verify the nature of latent factors of the scale in the Korean context. Our result demonstrates that the BES-K is divided into two factors: Interpersonal, consisting of seven items (Item 2, 3, 5, 6, 9, 11, 12) and Intrapersonal, consisting of four items (Item 4, 7, 8, 10). The Interpersonal factor evaluates the beliefs about emotions at the social and interpersonal level, focusing on the negative social consequences of emotions (e.g., “If I lose control of my emotions in front of others, they will think less of me”, “To be acceptable to others, I must keep any difficulties or negative feelings to myself”); and Intrapersonal factor measures the beliefs at the personal and intrapersonal level, focusing on whether people should be able to control or cope with negative thoughts and emotions (e.g., “I should be able to control my emotions”, “I should not let myself give in to negative feelings”).

Previous studies reported that emotional experiences and expressions fundamentally differ among cultures [31,32,33]. In collectivist cultures, emotions are regarded as relational phenomena reflecting the state of relationships rather than a unique personal inner state; thus, emotional expression is adjusted and sometimes suppressed according to the social context and self-other relationships [53,54,55]. For example, there are two Korean words, “Nun-chi (meaning being conscious to others)” and “Che-myon (meaning having a social face within a group)”, which describe adjusting personal emotions and behaviors in accordance with social context and relationship. Within the Korean society, one is considered socially inept and awkward when one is described to lack “Nun-chi” or “Che-myon”. Koreans are educated to control their emotions based on social cues and others’ expectations and are highly concerned about potential negative consequences of expressing their emotions. These relationship-oriented characteristics of Koreans might explain why the BES-K is divided into two different constructs: Interpersonal and Intrapersonal.

In comparison to the original English and its Brazilian adaptation, all three versions have different factor structures. Unlike the unidimensionality of the original English version, a two-factor model was shown in the Brazilian Portuguese version, where the first factor (Item 1, 2, 3, 5, 6, 8, 9, 10, 11; considering emotions and their expressions as signs of weakness) is distinctive from the second factor (Item 4, 7, 12; emotional self-control). The authors explained that emotional control can be a different aspect of emotional expression in a Latin American culture, which favors emotional expression and is more expressive than European culture [11]. In the Korean version, another two-factor model was confirmed. Two versions differ in Item 1, 8, 10, and 12. Further, the interpretation of each factor in the two versions was somewhat different. In the Korean version, the cause of regulating, controlling, or coping with emotions (e.g., whether it is an interpersonal cause or intrapersonal cause) was an important criterion between the two factors. However, in the Brazilian version, although the interpretation was not explained in detail, we can assume emotional self-control was a critical criterion between the two factors. For instance, in Item 12 (“Others expect me to always be in control of my emotions”), Korean focused on “others expect”, whereas Brazilian focused on “control”.

The relationship between the BES-K and the criterion scales was largely in line with previous studies. The correlations for the Korean sample were significant yet lower than the original BES and similar to the Brazilian version, particularly in terms of depression and anxiety. All three versions showed small-to-medium correlations with the criterion scales. Some researchers explained that these significant yet not strong correlations between the BES and other psychological symptoms imply the idea that negative beliefs about emotions may represent a transdiagnostic vulnerability factor contributing to diverse clinical problems rather than having a specific association with a certain problem [10, 11]. When examining the two subscales of the BES-K, Intrapersonal subscale was not significantly related to the criterion scales.

To understand this discrepancy, the research team to which authors are affiliated re-checked the three versions of the BES. We found that some researchers interpreted Item 4 (“I should be able to control my emotions”) and Item 7 (“I should not let myself give in to negative feelings”) as their adaptive ability to manage and regulate negative emotions not only in the Korean version but also in the original English version. This led us to suggest a possible assumption that these items might be open to interpretation among cultures due to cultural differences in emotional processing. As mentioned above, in Korea whose values and mentalities still heavily rely on the ideas of Confucianism such as harmony, people are socialized to control personal emotions and behaviors in accordance with social context and others. People are considered as “mature” if they maintain social harmony and good relationship by controlling and adjusting their emotions. Thus, in Korea, where emotional control is highly valued and recommended, Item 4 and 7 might be understood as desirable and adaptive ability to control and manage emotions, rather than maladaptive emotional suppression or avoidance, thereby diminishing the correlations with the criterion scales. This discrepancy was also found in Factor 2 (emotional self-control; Item 4, 7, 12) in the Brazilian version, and the authors of the Brazilian version also explained that this might be related to cultural and contextual differences [11]. Therefore, we assumed the fact that the Intrapersonal factor did not correlate with anxiety and depression was related to the cultural differences in emotional processing, but this issue needs to be further investigated in future studies.

Some limitations of the present study should be addressed. First, participants in the present study were recruited online and the age range of the participants was limited to 20–59 years. Therefore, it is hard to generalize and apply the results to different age groups and clinical populations. Further studies are needed to validate the BES-K in different age groups (e.g., adolescents, university students, and senior citizens) and diverse clinical sample (e.g., individuals with chronic fatigue syndrome, and irritable bowel syndromes, and eating disorders). Second, causal inferences between the BES-K and other psychological variables could not be drawn given the cross-sectional design of the study. Experimental or longitudinal designs are required to understand the cause-and-effect relationships between variables.

This study was the first to our knowledge to adapt and psychometrically evaluate beliefs about emotions in Korea. As such beliefs represent a transdiagnostic vulnerability factor contributing to diverse clinical problems [10], the BES-K would allow better understanding of various symptoms and encourage relevant studies in Korea. Furthermore, in therapeutic settings, as various current cognitive behavior psychotherapies focus on addressing and making interventions for such beliefs, the BES-K would be practically useful in addressing the beliefs that drive maladaptive behaviors, helping developing alternative behavior responses, and evaluating the effectiveness of therapeutic interventions. In addition, as emotions are understood and experienced in a fashion analogous to the dominating ideas and values of each culture wherein emotions occur [33], the BES-K would contribute to further discussion on cultural differences in beliefs about emotions, as well as emotional processing.

Availability of data and materials

The data used in the present study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Abbreviations

BES:

Beliefs about Emotions Scale

BES-K:

Korean version of the Beliefs about the Emotions Scale

EFA:

Exploratory factor analysis

CFA:

Confirmative factor analysis

DERS-16:

Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale-16

BSI-18:

The Brief Symptom Inventory-18

MLE:

Maximum likelihood estimation

CFI:

Comparative fit index

TLI:

Tucker–Lewis index

NFI:

Normed fit index

SRMR:

Standardized root mean square residual

RMSEA:

Root mean square error of approximation

AIC:

Akaike information criterion

References

  1. Cramer KM, Gallant MD, Langlois MW. Self-silencing and depression in women and men: comparative structural equation models. Personal Individ Differ. 2005;39(3):581–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. De Castella K, Goldin P, Jazaieri H, Ziv M, Heimberg RG, Gross JJ. Emotion beliefs in social anxiety disorder: associations with stress, anxiety, and well-being. Aust J Psychol. 2014;66(2):139–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Woolfolk RL, Allen LA. Treating somatization: a cognitive-behavioral approach. New York: Guilford Press; 2007.

    Google Scholar 

  4. Hambrook D, Oldershaw A, Rimes K, Schmidt U, Tchanturia K, Treasure J, et al. Emotional expression, self-silencing, and distress tolerance in anorexia nervosa and chronic fatigue syndrome. Br J Clin Psychol. 2011;50(3):310–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Loades ME, Rimes K, Lievesley K, Ali S, Chalder T. Cognitive and behavioural responses to symptoms in adolescents with chronic fatigue syndrome: a case-control study nested within a cohort. Clin Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2019;24(3):564–79.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Sibelli A, Chalder T, Everitt H, Chilcot J, Moss-Morris R. Positive and negative affect mediate the bidirectional relationship between emotional processing and symptom severity and impact in irritable bowel syndrome. J Psychosom Res. 2018;105:1–13.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Corstorphine E. Cognitive–emotional–behavioural therapy for the eating disorders: Working with beliefs about emotions. Eur Eat Disord Rev Prof J Eat Disord Assoc. 2006;14(6):448–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Surawy C, Hackmann A, Hawton K, Sharpe M. Chronic fatigue syndrome: a cognitive approach. Behav Res Ther. 1995;33(5):535–44.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Schmidt U, Treasure J. Anorexia nervosa: valued and visible. A cognitive-interpersonal maintenance model and its implications for research and practice. Br J Clin Psychol. 2006;45(3):343–66.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Rimes KA, Chalder T. The Beliefs about Emotions Scale: validity, reliability and sensitivity to change. J Psychosom Res. 2010;68(3):285–92.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Mograbi DC, Indelli P, Lage CA, Tebyriça V, Landeira-Fernandez J, Rimes KA. Cross-cultural adaptation and validation of the Brazilian version of the Beliefs about Emotions Scale. Trends Psychiatry Psychother. 2018;40:21–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Kennedy-Moore E, Watson JC. How and when does emotional expression help? Rev Gen Psychol. 2001;5(3):187–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Ali A, Toner BB, Stuckless N, Gallop R, Diamant NE, Gould MI, et al. Emotional abuse, self-blame, and self-silencing in women with irritable bowel syndrome. Psychosom Med. 2000;62(1):76–82.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Bowers H, Wroe A. Beliefs about emotions mediate the relationship between emotional suppression and quality of life in irritable bowel syndrome. J Ment Health. 2016;25(2):154–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Gámez W, Chmielewski M, Kotov R, Ruggero C, Suzuki N, Watson D. The brief experiential avoidance questionnaire: development and initial validation. Psychol Assess. 2014;26(1):35.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Gratz KL, Roemer L. Multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation and dysregulation: development, factor structure, and initial validation of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. 2004;26(1):41–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Gross JJ, John OP. Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2003;85(2):348.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Rimes KA, Wingrove J. Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for people with chronic fatigue syndrome still experiencing excessive fatigue after cognitive behaviour therapy: a pilot randomized study. Clin Psychol Psychother. 2013;20(2):107–17.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Brooks SK, Chalder T, Rimes KA. Chronic fatigue syndrome: cognitive, behavioural and emotional processing vulnerability factors. Behav Cogn Psychother. 2017;45(2):156–69.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Sydenham M, Beardwood J, Rimes KA. Beliefs about emotions, depression, anxiety and fatigue: a mediational analysis. Behav Cogn Psychother. 2017;45(1):73–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Bowers H, Wroe AL, Pincus T. ‘Isn’t it ironic?’Beliefs about the unacceptability of emotions and emotional suppression relate to worse outcomes in fibromyalgia. Clin Rheumatol. 2017;36(5):1121.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. James K, Verplanken B, Rimes KA. Self-criticism as a mediator in the relationship between unhealthy perfectionism and distress. Personal Individ Differ. 2015;79:123–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Tran L, Rimes KA. Unhealthy perfectionism, negative beliefs about emotions, emotional suppression, and depression in students: a mediational analysis. Personality Individ Differ. 2017;110:144–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Trusty WT, Richardson CM. Self-critical perfectionism, beliefs about emotions, emotional disclosure, and depression in young adults: a mediational analysis. Br J Guid Counsell. 2021:1–12.

  25. Bowers HM, Pincus T, Jones G, Wroe AL. Investigating the role of beliefs about emotions, emotional suppression and distress within a pain management programme for fibromyalgia. Br J Pain. 2019;13(2):112–20.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Kabat-Zinn J. An outpatient program in behavioral medicine for chronic pain patients based on the practice of mindfulness meditation: theoretical considerations and preliminary results. Gen Hosp Psychiatry. 1982;4(1):33–47.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Kabat-Zinn J. Mindfulness-based interventions in context: past, present, and future. 2003.

  28. Linehan M. Skills training manual for treating borderline personality disorder. New York: Guilford Press; 1993.

    Google Scholar 

  29. Hayes S, Strosahl K, Wilson K. Acceptance and commitment therapy: understanding and treating human suffering. New York: Guilford Press; 1999.

    Google Scholar 

  30. Segal ZV, Teasdale JD, Williams JM, Gemar MC. The mindfulness-based cognitive therapy adherence scale: inter-rater reliability, adherence to protocol and treatment distinctiveness. Clin Psychol Psychother. 2002;9(2):131–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Barrett LF, Mesquita B, Ochsner KN, Gross JJ. The experience of emotion. Annu Rev Psychol. 2007;58:373–403.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Markus HR, Kitayama S. Culture and the self: implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychol Rev. 1991;98(2):224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Mesquita B. Emotions in collectivist and individualist contexts. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2001;80(1):68.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Johnson JA. Ascertaining the validity of individual protocols from web-based personality inventories. J Res Pers. 2005;39(1):103–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Barends AJ, de Vries RE. Noncompliant responding: comparing exclusion criteria in MTurk personality research to improve data quality. Personal Individ Differ. 2019;143:84–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Huang JL, Liu M, Bowling NA. Insufficient effort responding: examining an insidious confound in survey data. J Appl Psychol. 2015;100(3):828.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Guillemin F, Bombardier C, Beaton D. Cross-cultural adaptation of health-related quality of life measures: literature review and proposed guidelines. J Clin Epidemiol. 1993;46(12):1417–32.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Beaton DE, Bombardier C, Guillemin F, Ferraz MB. Guidelines for the process of cross-cultural adaptation of self-report measures. Spine. 2000;25(24):3186–91.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Derogatis LR. The brief symptom inventory-18 (BSI-18): administration. scoring, and procedures manual (3rd ed), Minneapolis: National Computer Systems. 2000.

  40. Park KP, Woo SW, Jang MS. Validational study of Brief Symptoms Inventory-18 (BSI-18) in college students. Korean J Clin Psychol. 2012;31(2):507–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Bjureberg J, Ljótsson B, Tull MT, Hedman E, Sahlin H, Lundh L-G, et al. Development and validation of a brief version of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale: the DERS-16. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. 2016;38(2):284–96.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Cho YR. Assessing emotion dysregulation: psychometric properties of the Korean version of the difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale. Korean J Clin Psychol. 2007;26(4):1015–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Kline T. Psychological testing: a practical approach to design and evaluation. Thousand Oaks: Sage; 2005.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  44. Anderson JC, Gerbing DW. Structural equation modeling in practice: a review and recommended two-step approach. Psychol Bull. 1988;103(3):411.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Lloret-Segura S, Ferreres-Traver A, Hernandez-Baeza A, Tomas-Marco I. Exploratory item factor analysis: a practical guide revised and updated. Anales de Psicología. 2014;30(3):1151–69.

    Google Scholar 

  46. Tabachnick BG, Fidell LS, Ullman JB. Using multivariate statistics. Boston: Pearson; 2007.

    Google Scholar 

  47. Cattell RB. The scree test for the number of factors. Multivar Behav Res. 1966;1(2):245–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Kaiser HF. The application of electronic computers to factor analysis. Educ Psychol Measur. 1960;20(1):141–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Browne MW, Cudeck R. Alternative ways of assessing model fit. Sociol Methods Res. 1992;21(2):230–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Hoyle RH, Panter AT. Writing about structural equation models in structural equation modeling. Structural equation modeling: concepts, issues, and applications. 1995:158–76.

  51. Hu L, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct Equ Model Multidiscip J. 1999;6(1):1–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Raylu N, Oei TP. The Gambling Related Cognitions Scale (GRCS): development, confirmatory factor validation and psychometric properties. Addiction. 2004;99(6):757–69.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Markus HR, Kitayama S. Cultures and selves: a cycle of mutual constitution. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2010;5(4):420–30.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Safdar S, Friedlmeier W, Matsumoto D, Yoo SH, Kwantes CT, Kakai H, et al. Variations of emotional display rules within and across cultures: a comparison between Canada, USA, and Japan. Can J Behav Sci. 2009;41(1):1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Eid M, Diener E. Norms for experiencing emotions in different cultures: inter-and intranational differences. Culture and well-being. Berlin: Springer; 2009. p. 169–202.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

This work was supported by the Ministry of Education of the Republic of Korea and the National Research Foundation of Korea [Grant Number: NRF-2021S1A3A2A02089682].

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

GP conducted the literature search and the statistical analysis, wrote the first draft of the paper, and edited the final version. JK conducted the statistical analysis and provided expert opinions on this study. DL was the Principal Investigator, designed this study, collected data, supervised statistical analysis, provided review and feedback on the paper, and gave expert opinions on the subject. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Dong Hun Lee.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

This study received ethical approval from the Institutional Review Board (IRB) of SungKyunKwan University to which the researchers are affiliated [SKKU 2015-01-006-020], and all methods were carried out in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants with a detailed survey orientation, including the purpose and procedures of this study. All participants were informed that the information they provided would remain private and confidential, and they could withdraw from the study should they feel any discomfort during the survey.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Park, G., Kim, J.H. & Lee, D.H. Validation of the South Korean Version of the Beliefs about Emotions Scale. BMC Psychol 10, 7 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-021-00713-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-021-00713-4

Keywords