Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Less efficient detection of positive facial expressions in parents at risk of engaging in child physical abuse

Abstract

Background

Parental physical punishment (e.g., spanking) of children can gradually escalate into child physical abuse (CPA). According to social-information processing (SIP) models of aggressive behaviors, distorted social cognitive mechanisms can increase the risk of maladaptive parenting behaviors by changing how parents detect, recognize, and act on information from their social environments. In this study, we aimed to identify differences between mothers with a low and high risk of CPA regarding how quickly they detect positive facial expressions.

Methods

Based on their use of spanking to discipline children, 52 mothers were assigned to a low- (n = 39) or high-CPA-risk group (n = 13). A single-target facial emotional search (face-in-the-crowd) task was used, which required participants to search through an array of faces to determine whether a target emotional face was present in a crowd of non-target neutral faces. Search efficiency index was computed by subtracting the search time for target-present trials from that for target-absent trials.

Results

The high-CPA-risk group searched significantly less efficiently for the happy, but not sad, faces, than did the low-CPA-risk group; meanwhile, self-reported emotional ratings (i.e., valence and arousal) of the faces did not differ between the groups.

Conclusions

Consistent with the SIP models, our findings suggest that low- and high-CPA-risk mothers differ in how they rapidly detect positive facial expressions, but not in how they explicitly evaluate them. On a CPA-risk continuum, less efficient detection of positive facial expressions in the initial processes of the SIP system may begin to occur in the physical-discipline stage, and decrease the likelihood of positive interpersonal experiences, consequently leading to an increased risk of CPA.

Peer Review reports

Background

A general definition of the physical punishment of children, such as spanking (i.e., open-handed swats to the buttocks or extremities), is “the use of physical force with the intention of causing a child to experience pain, but not injury, for the purpose of correction or control of the child’s behavior” [1]. However, for children, receiving physical punishment has been associated with cognitive-behavioral, physical, and mental health problems in later life [2,3,4,5,6]; further, it has also been suggested to alter the trajectories of brain development [7, 8]. Given such long-term adverse consequences, physical punishment (e.g., spanking) can be defined as a form of child maltreatment, which encompasses a spectrum of abusive actions (physical, emotional, sexual abuse) or lack of actions (i.e., neglect) by the parent or other caregivers. Indeed, spanking has empirically loaded on the same factor structure with physical and emotional abuse items which indicates a similar underlying construct to physical and emotional abuse [9].

In recent years, the traditional perceived dichotomy between physical punishment and child physical abuse (CPA) has begun to disappear [10], and physical punishment is beginning to be considered a risk factor of CPA. Specifically, it is believed to escalate gradually into CPA, following a continuum ranging from positive (effective) discipline, to physical punishment, to abusive treatment [11,12,13,14]. Today, physical punishment in all settings, including the home, is legally prohibited in 56 countries around the world [15]. However, to prevent child maltreatment and related problems (e.g., co-parental conflicts), it is of particular importance to better understand the social cognitive mechanisms that prompt a parent to progress from positive discipline along the continuum towards physical punishment and/or CPA.

According to social-information processing (SIP) models regarding CPA risk [16,17,18,19,20,21], distorted social cognitive mechanisms may increase the risk of maladaptive parenting behaviors by changing how parents detect, recognize, and act on information from social environments. In Milner’s [19, 20] studies, social cognitive mechanisms are assumed to encompass four stages: first, perceiving social behavior (e.g., facial expressions); second, interpreting and evaluating the meanings of the behavior; third, integrating the information and selecting a response; and fourth, implementing and monitoring the response. These cognitive processing stages are also assumed to be influenced by cognitive schemata that are developed through experience and stored in long-term memory. When encountering a discipline situation, a parent at risk of engaging in CPA is likely to inaccurately perceive the child’s behavior, consider the behavior to be hostile (aggressive) and construct a negative narrative regarding the causes of the behavior. For example, high-CPA-risk parents tend to view negative child behaviors as being due to internal, stable, and global child factors and being motivated by hostile (aggressive) intent [20]. Various sources [22] show that parents with a higher CPA risk are more likely to show greater processing of negative (i.e., angry, hostile) stimuli in the SIP system in regard to schema accessibility [23,24,25], attentional control [26], interpretation [27,28,29], attribution [30, 31], and subjective feelings [32], although a few studies [33] have found that less, rather than greater, accessibility to negative information is present in parents with a higher CPA risk. Overall, the main findings of prior research have suggested that greater processing of negative stimuli in the SIP system increase the likelihood of parents engaging in aggressive behaviors [22].

In addition to altered negative processing, the parents with a high CPA risk, relative to low-risk parents, also seem to exhibit less processing of positive (i.e., happy, benign) stimuli in the SIP system [23, 24, 27, 33]. Aggression may be associated with the twice the challenges, including both the altered processing of angry (hostile) stimuli and happy stimuli in the SIP system. However, relatively less attention has focused on the decreased processing of positive information, including schema accessibility [23, 24, 33] and interpretation [27]. For example, Crouch et al. [24] reported that, in a cued-recall task, high-CPA-risk parents, compared to low-CPA-risk parents, recalled less child-care information when cued by positive terms, indicating less accessibility of positive schema stored in long-term memory. Similarly, Dopke et al. [27] found that low-CPA-risk parents, unlike high-CPA-risk parents, have positive interpretive tendencies regarding child behaviors. As positive social information (e.g., a happy facial expression) has important adaptive functions, such as by facilitating interpersonal relationships [34, 35], efficiently perceiving and interpreting such critical can secure important interpersonal benefits (e.g., parent-child attachment formation). In a parent-child communicative setting, detection of a child’s happiness engenders happiness in the perceiving parent, facilitating a feedback loop, whereby the detecting of happiness leads to the parent having a happy experience, and the parent’s consequent expression of happiness elicits further happiness in the child.

In the current study, we mainly focused on detection efficiency (i.e., initial processes of the SIP system) of positive information in the low- and high-CPA risk parents, rather than the schema accessibility and interpretation focus of previous studies [23, 24, 27, 33]. In particular, we examined differences between parents (mothers) with low and high risks of engaging in CPA in relation to their speed of detection of positive (happy) facial expressions. We hypothesized that high-CPA-risk mothers would exhibit lower performance on the happy face detection task than on the low-CPA-risk mothers. To determine the CPA risk, we focused on the use of spanking (i.e., swatting a child’s buttocks or extremities with an open hand) as a form of discipline. Consequently, mothers who never spanked their children in order to discipline them were classified as low CPA risk, and mothers who spanked their children to discipline them were classified as high CPA risk, which was based on the Index of Child Care Environment (ICCE) [36] that was developed using the Home Observation for Measurement of Environment (HOME) [37]. As an experimental detection paradigm, a single-target face-search task (i.e., a face-in-the-crowd task) was used, which required participants to search through an array of schematic faces to determine whether a target happy face was present in a crowd of non-target neutral faces [38, 39]. As target-absent trials require an exhaustive search of the entire array before participants can indicate that the target is absent, the task responses provide an important baseline for the responses in target-present trials [38, 40]. The response differences between the target-present and target-absent trials indicate the level of efficiency regarding searching for happy faces, with higher values indicating greater search efficiency. In the single-target face-search tasks, we used not only the happy-face search task but also the sad-face search task, which allowed us to take into consideration visual (physical) saliency for the target face among the non-target faces. From an evolutionary perspective, mothers who could efficiently detect child’s sad expressions as signs of distress might provide a better chance of survival for the premature child [41,42,43]. In particular, greater processing of a child’s sad expressions has been shown in neglectful than non-neglectful parents [44], but not shown in physically abusive (high-CPA-risk) parents [45], suggesting differences in distorted social cognitive mechanisms underlying physically abusive and neglectful parenting behaviors. Based on previous studies [22, 44, 45], we hypothesized that higher CPA risk would not be associated with the altered processing of sad stimuli in the SIP system. If our hypothesis was correct, high-CPA-risk mothers would exhibit lower search efficiency for the target-happy, but not for the target-sad faces than the low-CPA-risk mothers. Conversely, if a higher CPA risk was associated with the altered detection of visual saliency for the target face among the non-target faces, high-CPA-risk, relative to low-CPA-risk mothers, would exhibit lower search efficiency for the target-happy and target-sad faces, respectively.

Methods

Participants

Fifty-two healthy Japanese mothers (age range = 27–46 years; mean age = 35.5 years; SD = 4.2 years) who were caring for one or more young children participated in this study, after providing written informed consent. The study protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Fukui and was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and the Ethical Guidelines for Clinical Studies published by the Ministry of Health, Labour, and Welfare of Japan. Almost all mothers (51 [98.1%]) were caring for at least one preschool child (one [1.9%] was caring for an elementary school child). All mothers had completed at least 12 years of education (non-compulsory secondary-level or post-school university-level education), which was categorized as a relatively high level of education [46]. Further, they were all living above the relative poverty line, which was set at 50% of the country’s median household income [47]. All had normal vision or corrected-to-normal vision. Moreover, through self-report questionnaires, they stated that they had no history of brain injury or neurological or psychiatric illness, and that were not currently using psychoactive medications.

Using the ICCE, the mothers were classified with respect to their CPA risk, based on their use of spanking to discipline children for misbehavior. In Japan, milder physical punishment such as spanking has been still considered a socially acceptable parental behavior [48]. For the ICCE subscale “avoidance of restriction” (two items: Q1 “what would you do if your child spilled milk on purpose?” and Q2 “how many times did you spank your child last week?”), the answers “I would not spank” and “I did not spank” were defined as low CPA risk, and the answers “I would spank” and/or “I did spank” were defined as high CPA risk. Of the 52 mothers, 39 (75%) were classified as low CPA risk, and the remaining 13 (25%) were classified as high CPA risk (approximately 8% of the High CPA risk group answered “I would spank” to Q1 and 92% answered “I did spank” to Q2).

Measures of maternal characteristics

The Buss-Perry Aggression Questionnaire (BPAQ) [49, 50] was used to measure the mothers’ aggression; this consists of four subscales: anger, hostility, physical aggression, and verbal aggression. Meanwhile, to assess empathic ability, the Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI) [51, 52] was used, which is composed of four subscales (Empathic Concern, Personal Distress, Perspective-Taking, and Fantasy). Further, the Japanese version of the Parenting Stress Index (J-PSI) [53], which is an adaptation of the PSI [54], was used to evaluate the mothers’ parenting stress. The J-PSI is comprised of items on Child (reinforces parent, mood, acceptability, distractibility/hyperactivity, demandingness, problems/worries, and sensitivity to stimuli) and Parent domains (role restriction, social isolation, relationship with spouse, competence, depression, sad/uneasy feelings after leaving hospital, attachment, and health). The Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II) [55, 56] was used to measure the mothers’ depressive symptoms, and the Parental Bonding Instrument (PBI) [57, 58] was used to obtain retrospective information on the parental caregiving behaviors the participants perceived during their first 16 years of life. The PBI is comprised of two fundamental dimensions of parental behaviors: parental emotional support (care) and parental protectiveness (protection).

Stimuli

The stimuli were three schematic images of facial emotions (happy, sad, neutral) (Fig. 1a) taken from the Wong-Baker Faces Pain Rating Scale (WBFS) [59]. The faces, including the outline, eyebrows, eyes, and mouth, were depicted using black lines on a white background. The happy face used in this study was taken from the WBFS smiling face representing “no hurt” (Face 0), while the sad face was taken from the WBFS sad face representing “hurts a whole lot” (Face 8). For the neutral face, Face 4 from the WBFS was used. Each face image was pasted onto a white background that was 175 × 165 pixels in size and assigned to any of 12 possible locations on a 4 × 3 array.

Fig. 1
figure 1

a Emotional schematic faces (i.e., happy, neutral, sad) selected from the Wong-Baker Faces [59]. b Mean valence and arousal ratings of the faces for the CPA-risk groups. Error bars represent the standard errors of the mean

Face ratings

Participants rated, using a nine-point Likert scale, each face image in terms of valence and level of arousal [60, 61]. For Valence ratings, the scale ranged from extremely unpleasant (1) to extremely pleasant (9), and for Arousal, the scale ranged from extreme sleepiness (1) to extremely high arousal [9]. On the arousal-valence orthogonal dimension of the circumplex model of affect [61], happiness is high in pleasantness and high in arousal, whereas sadness is low in pleasantness but low in arousal.

Face-search task

The stimuli were displayed on a 14-in. monitor with a refresh rate of 60 Hz and a screen resolution of 1024 × 768 pixels and were presented using Presentation software (Neurobehavioral Systems, Albany, CA) running on a Windows computer. Participants were seated approximately 70 cm away from the monitor and gave responses using the left and right arrow keys on the computer’s keyboard. Before beginning the experiment, all participants received instructions and performed a short practice task.

Participants were instructed to perform, as quickly and accurately as possible, two visual search tasks (happy, sad); each task had three set-size conditions (1, 6, and 12); similar visual-search-task paradigms involving emotional schematic faces have been applied in several previous experimental psychological studies [38, 39]. In each task, our participants indicated whether a target face was present on the display by pressing the right or left direction arrow key. The right direction arrow key was associated with target-present detection, whereas the left direction arrow key was associated with target-absent (non-target) detection. In one of the visual search tasks, the target they searched for was always a happy face, and in the other, the target was always a sad face. For the first set size (set of 1), a target face or a distracting non-target neutral face was presented in only one of the 12 possible locations in a 4 × 3 array. For the second set size (set of 6), a target face and five non-target faces, or six non-target faces, were presented in six of the 12 possible locations. Finally, for the third set size (set of 12), a target face and 11 non-target faces, or 12 non-target faces, were presented in the 12 possible locations.

Participants completed six task blocks, each consisting of 24 trials, giving a total of 144 trials. Within each task block, half were target-present trials and half were target-absent trials. The task blocks were presented in order of ascending set size (1, 6, and 12). Each trial began with a black fixation cross presented in the middle of the screen, which remained on screen for 1000 ms. The face stimuli were then presented for 5000 ms or until the participant responded by pressing one of the two keys with the index or middle finger of the right hand. The next trial commenced after an inter-trial interval of 1000 ms.

Visual saliency

A total of 144 visual scene images, including 36 happy-face-present, 36 sad-face-present, and 36 target-absent neutral (twice) scenes, were used for the face-search task experiment. For the three types of visual scenes (happy, sad, and neutral), visual saliency maps were computed according to the Graph-Based Visual Saliency (GBVS) model [62]. The GBVS algorithms extract low-level visual features (e.g., intensity, orientation), generate individual feature maps by extracting locations of distinctive features, and integrate these maps to generate an overall saliency map. The values of the saliency maps range from 0 to 1, depicting the distribution of visual saliency across the scene image. The saliency maps of the three types of visual scenes had comparable mean values (F(2, 105) = 0.13, p > .87), indicating control for the visual saliency among the three types of visual scenes (happy-face-present scenes, mean value [SD] = 0.152 [0.097]; sad-face-present scenes, mean value [SD] = 0.142 [0.095]; target-absent neutral scenes, mean value [SD] = 0.153 [0.097]).

Data analysis

The mean response time (RT) and accuracy (percentage of correct responses) were calculated individually, using separate measures for the two trial types (target-present, target-absent), the two target emotions (happy, sad), and the three set sizes (1, 6, and 12). RTs were only analyzed for correct responses. Data for measures for which participants had an error rate in excess of 25% were excluded from each analysis. Search slope was calculated for each task by fitting a linear function to the mean RTs for the three set sizes. An increasing slope with more set sizes (distractors) indicated a serial exhaustive search strategy, whereas a flattened slope indicated a pre-attentive parallel search strategy. As target-absent trials require participants to perform an exhaustive search of the entire array before they can indicate that the target is absent, the RTs and slopes for these trials provided an important “baseline” against which the RTs and slopes for the target-present trials could be interpreted [38, 40]. Thus, differences in RT (Δ RT) and search slope (Δ search slope), which would reflect a search advantage (i.e., efficiency) regarding target-present over target-absent trial types, were calculated by subtracting the RTs and slopes of the target-present trial types from those of the target-absent (baseline) trial types. The RT differences (Δ RT) reflected the search efficiency at a specific set size, whereas the search slope differences (Δ search slope) reflected the overall search efficiency across the three set sizes. These differences in search efficiency create indexes with positive values when there is a search advantage (efficiency), and with negative values when there is a search disadvantage (inefficiency) regarding target-present relative to target-absent trial types. All statistical analyses were performed using SPSS Statistics (version 22; IBM Japan, Tokyo, Japan). The accuracy and Δ RT data were analyzed using a two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) with one between-subjects factor (CPA risk: low, high) and one within-subject factor (set size: 1, 6, and 12). The Δ search slope data for the 2 CPA risk groups were analyzed using a two-tailed t-test. An alpha level of .05, with Bonferroni correction, when appropriate, was used for all significance tests.

Results

Demographic and psychological characteristic data

The demographic and psychological characteristics of the CPA groups are listed in Table 1. There were significant differences between the 2 CPA-risk groups for five measures: number of children, t(50) = 3.61, p < .001, d = 1.03; BPAQ Anger scores, t(50) = 2.48, p = .016, d = 0.75; J-PSI Child domain Mood subscore, t(50) = 2.78, p = .007, d = 0.92; J-PSI Child domain Acceptability subscore, t(50) = 2.68, p = .009, d = 0.78, and J-PSI Parent domain Attachment subscore, t(50) = 3.15, p = .002, d = 0.92. There were no differences between the remaining scores (all ps > .07).

Table 1 The Child Physical Abuse (CPA)-risk group characteristics

Face ratings data

As shown in Fig. 1b, the low- and high-CPA-risk groups gave similar Valence and Arousal ratings for all three face images (happy, sad, neutral) (all ps > .24). Overall, the happy face image was rated high in pleasantness and high in arousal, whereas the sad face image was low in pleasantness but low in arousal.

Face-search-task data

Accuracy

Both the low- and high-CPA-risk groups showed over 90% accuracy for all trials (Table 2). For the happy-face search task, a two-way ANOVA was conducted on the target-present trial type with one between-subjects factor (CPA risk: low, high) and one within-subject factor (set size: 1, 6, and 12); it was determined that CPA risk had no effect on accuracy (F(1, 48) = 2.15, p > .14). There was a main effect of set size (F(2, 96) = 11.42, p < .001, η2p = .192) and an interaction between the two factors (F(2, 96) = 4.66, p = .012, η2p = .088). Subsequent comparisons for the simple main effect indicated that the high CPA risk participants showed less accuracy in the set of six than did those with low CPA risk (t(48) = 2.70, p = .009, d = 0.84). For the target-absent trial type, there were no effects for CPA risk (F(1, 48) = 1.37, p > .24), set size (F < 1), or an interaction effect (F < 1).

Table 2 Mean accuracy of the happy- and sad-face search tasks for the two Child Physical Abuse (CPA)-risk groups

For the sad-face search task, the target-present trial type was again analyzed using an ANOVA. Here, there was neither a main effect of CPA risk (F < 1), a main effect of set size (F < 1), nor an interaction effect (F(2, 94) = 1.15, p > .32). However, for the target-absent trial type, there was a main effect of CPA risk (F(1, 47) = 5.64, p = .022, η2p = .107). The subsequent comparisons for the simple main effect indicated that the overall accuracy of the high-CPA-risk group was significantly less than that of the low-CPA-risk group (t(44) = 2.37, p = .021, d = 0.42). Neither the effect of set size (F(2, 94) = 2.99, p = .055) nor the interaction effect (F(2, 94) = 1.45, p > .23) were significant.

RT differences (Δ RT)

For the happy-face search task, the differences in RT (Δ RT) between the target-absent and -present trial types were analyzed using an ANOVA. Here, there were main effects of CPA risk (F(1, 48) = 4.44, p = .040, η2p = .085) and of set size (F(2, 96) = 67.84, p < .001, η2p = .586), as well as an interaction effect (F(2, 96) = 4.79, p = .010, η2p = .091). As indicated by subsequent comparisons for the simple main effect, the high-CPA-risk group showed significantly less-efficiency performing the visual search for the happy face in the set of 12 than did the low-CPA-risk group (Fig. 2a; t(48) = 2.38, p = .021, d = 0.91). On the other hand, for the sad-face search task (Fig. 2b), an ANOVA of the Δ RT showed that there was a main effect of set size (F(2, 94) = 56.48, p < .001, η2p = .546). Neither the effect of CPA risk (F < 1) nor the interaction effect (F(2, 94) = 1.60, p > .20) were significant.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Mean differences in Reaction Time (Δ RT) and search slope (Δ search slope) between target-absent and -present trial types for the Child Physical Abuse (CPA)-risk groups. a For the happy-face search task, the high-CPA-risk group searched significantly less efficiently than the low-CPA-risk group. b For the sad-face search task, there was no inter-group difference in search efficiency. Error bars represent the standard errors of the mean

Search slope differences (Δ search slope)

As shown in Fig. 2a and b, the differences in search slopes (Δ search slope) for the happy-face search task differed significantly between the CPA-risk groups (t(48) = 2.35, p = .023, d = 0.88), but not for the sad-face search task (t(47) = 1.44, p > .15, d = 0.49). This indicates that the high-CPA-risk group (mean Δ search slope [SD] = 27.83 [17.53]) had significantly lower search efficiency for the happy face than the low-CPA-risk group (mean Δ search slope [SD] = 47.13 [25.54]).

To further explore the relationship between the demographic and psychological characteristic data, the Δ search slopes for the happy-face search task, and the CPA-risk, we performed logistic regression analyses with the CPA-risk groups (i.e., low, high) as the binary outcomes. The Δ search slopes as well as five measures that showed significant between-group differences (i.e., number of children, the BPAQ Anger scores, the J-PSI Child domain subscores for mood and acceptability, and the J-PSI Parent domain subscores for attachment) were the predictors. The analyses showed that the Δ search slopes for happy faces (Wald = 4.63, p = .031, OR = 1.06, 95% CI [1.01, 1.11]) and number of children (Wald = 4.53, p = .033, OR = 0.22, 95% CI [0.05 to 0.89]) were significant predictors for being in the high-CPA-risk group. As confirmed by supplementary analyses using the mediational model, the two variables, Δ search slopes and the number of children, each had direct, but not indirect, effects on CPA-risk. Moreover, none of these five measures were significantly correlated with the Δ search slopes for the happy-face search task (all ps > .43).

Search strategies

To further explore the search strategies of the target-happy and -sad faces, the search slopes for the target-present trial type of the face search tasks were compared to zero in a one-sample t-test using a Bonferroni correction for multiple tests. For the target-happy faces, significantly increasing search slopes with more set sizes were shown in both the low- (t(38) = 12.14, p < .001) and high-CPA-risk groups (t(10) = 9.84, p < .001). On the other hand, for the target-sad faces, there were neither significant search slopes in the low- (t(35) = 1.07, p > .58) nor the high-CPA-risk groups (t(12) = 2.39, p = .068).

Discussion

The current study examined how individual differences in CPA risk are associated with the rapid detection of positive (happy) facial expressions during a single-target face search (face-in-the-crowd) task. Based on the Δ RT and Δ search slopes between the target-absent and -present trial types for each face-search task, the high-CPA-risk group was found to be significantly less efficient at searching for a happy, but not sad, face than were the low-CPA-risk group. The self-reported face ratings of valence and arousal did not differ between the 2 CPA-risk groups. The happy and sad faces that were rated in this study were consistent with happiness and sadness on the emotional expressions distributed in the arousal-valence orthogonal dimension of the circumplex model of affect [61]. On this dimension, happiness is high in pleasantness and high in arousal, whereas sadness is low in pleasantness but low in arousal, which was what our findings showed. The current study presented evidence that higher CPA risk was associated with less efficient detection of happy facial expressions in the face-search task rather than the visual saliency of the target face among the non-target faces.

Consistent with existing SIP models regarding CPA risk, the results of the current study suggest that showing less efficient detection of positive facial expressions in the SIP system is associated with a higher CPA risk. In particular, low- and high-CPA-risk mothers differed in how they rapidly detected happy facial expressions, but not in how they explicitly evaluated them. This less-efficient detection of happy facial expressions in high-CPA-risk mothers is likely to indicate a deficiency in the initial stages of their SIP, as characterized by the four-stage model [19, 20]. In previous studies involving verbal-stimulus input [23, 24, 27, 33], such decreased processing of positive information in the SIP system were shown across several processing stages. For example, in a cued-recall task, high-CPA-risk parents recalled less positive information when cued by positive words, indicating less accessibility of positive schema during the second or third processing stage [24]. Although the current study, applying visual (non-verbal) materials, differs from previous studies in terms of its experimental paradigm, it suggests that the distorted social cognitive mechanisms underlying CPA risk are associated with early processing (detection) of visual facial expressions rather than later processing (evaluation) of the emotions depicted by the facial expressions.

Based on the results of the search strategies, the overall search slope of the target-happy, but not target-sad, faces in this study increased with more set sizes, indicating serial exhaustive search processes that were different from parallel search processes [38]. Combined with these findings, the influence of CPA risk on the efficient detection of visual facial expressions appears to vary depending on the visual search strategies (i.e., parallel or serial). According to models of visual searches [63], it is assumed that information about the presence of task-relevant features is accumulated in parallel searches (spatially global guidance) and is then used to control the allocation of spatial attention to possible target objects (spatially focal selection). A choice between parallel and serial selection strategies is assumed to be determined by the nature of a particular search task. Thus, the influence of CPA risk on the happy-face search efficiency may occur under conditions where processing demands of the task are greater; in that case, a serial selection strategy is chosen. As considered from one evolutionary perspective, mothers who could efficiently detect children’s negative signals (e.g., sad or crying expressions) as signs of distress provide a greater chance of survival for the children and, over time, a parent-child communication system developed in which children’s stylized distress signals triggered maternal attention and care [41,42,43]. Although detection of another’s distress generally encourages empathic (prosocial) responses, such distress signals can also often produce aversive responses, including anger, horror, and even physical abuse [64,65,66]; further, subclinically distressed mothers have been found to generally have lower brain function regarding their interpretation of social signals [67]. On the other hand, given that positive social signals have an important adaptive function facilitating interpersonal relationships [34, 35], less-efficient detection of happy facial expressions may decrease the likelihood of a mother having positive interpersonal experiences, consequently leading to a relatively increased probability of detecting children’s distress signals and an increased probability of experiencing frustration and stress in such situations [68, 69]. Taken together, it is possible that the serial search of happy signals may be relatively vulnerable to CPA risk, while the parallel search of sad signals may be relatively resilient to CPA risk.

Moreover, inefficient detection of happy facial expressions can also influence interpersonal experiences with other adults and children in parental caregiving contexts. Parental caregiving commonly involves social cooperation with others (i.e., co-parenting, which refers to coordination between individuals responsible for the care and upbringing of children) [70, 71]. When a person is perceived to be happy, the positivity typically spreads to the perceiver (interpersonal warmth) and, consequently, the perceiver becomes more inclined to cooperate with the person [72, 73]. In a co-parental setting, when a parent detects their partner (or social supporter) to be happy, it may cause herself/himself to selectively focus on the partner’s co-parental efforts, which may lead to improved co-parenting. Conversely, lower positive biases in the SIP system can interfere with positive co-parental experiences. For example, high-CPA-risk parents with inefficient detection of positive information may have more difficulty feeling interpersonal warmth and associating it with cooperativeness, consequently preventing themselves from fully engaging in problem-solving of family matters with their partner, which would, in turn, lead to childrearing disagreements and heightened co-parental conflicts. Children’s exposure to intense parental/co-parental conflicts has been reported to be associated with an increased risk of altered brain-development trajectories during childhood [74, 75] and into adulthood [76]. Thus, to prevent child maltreatment and related problems (e.g., co-parental conflicts), further studies are needed to identify the social cognitive mechanisms that prompt a parent to progress from positive toward negative interpersonal relationships with children and other adults in parenting/co-parenting contexts.

To date, SIP models concerning CPA risk and related study paradigms have mainly focused on explicit late-stage processes rather than implicit early-stage processes. Consequently, scientific understanding of distorted late-stage processes in at-risk parents has been applied to the design of cognitive-behavioral interventions designed to modify interpretive bias [17, 77,78,79]. On the other hand, the current study suggests that distorted early-stage processes in the SIP system are associated with high CPA risk. The application of this scientific evidence in parenting programs focusing on attentional bias modification (ABM) may enhance tailored interventions targeting the specific bias profiles shown by individual parents. In other research fields, it has been indicated that ABM training, which encourages positively-focused attention-search modes, reduces self-reported stress and physiological (e.g., cortisol) measures of stress reactivity [80, 81]. Such tailored interventions (e.g., ABM training) might benefit the prevention of interpersonal problems (e.g., child maltreatment), as well as providing support to families with a large number of children [82]. Although whether parenting programs for ABM effectively modify not only the attentional biases but also the parenting stress and maladaptive parenting behaviors of at-risk parents is still not fully understood, further studies of the SIP models regarding CPA risk may present avenues for the early identification and prevention of child maltreatment and related problems.

A few potential limitations of the current study should be noted. First, our high-CPA-risk group was modest in size. A post-hoc sample size calculation [83] for a two-sample t-test as a main analysis indicated a minimum sample size of 26 for each group (calculated effect size = .80; alpha level = .05; power = .80), and therefore, this study was slightly underpowered, thus other potentially significant findings may have been missed. Studies involving a larger number of participants are essential for generalizing our results. Second, schematic faces used here have reduced ecological validity, although many visual search studies have used schematic faces to eliminate low-level perceptual variations found in actual faces (e.g., photographs). Given this tradeoff between experimental control and ecological validity [84], future studies are needed to examine whether similar results would be obtained using photographed faces. In this study, it was important that self-reported emotional ratings of the schematic faces were fit with the emotional expressions distributed in the arousal-valence orthogonal dimension of the circumplex model of affect [61]. On this dimension, happiness is high in pleasantness and high in arousal, whereas sadness is low in pleasantness but low in arousal. Finally, the positive stimuli used in this study were only limited to happy faces (i.e., genuine smiles). As previously shown, even in the absence of happy eyes, a smiling mouth face (i.e., a nongenuine or fake smiling face) was likely to bias the judgment of the expression as being happy [85], and was associated with an increased inclination to cooperate with the smiling person [72, 73]. Further studies using an ambiguous happy-face search task with fake smiling faces would be helpful to better understanding the social cognitive mechanisms associated with CPA risk and maladaptive parenting behaviors.

Conclusions

In this study, we found that high-CPA-risk, compared to low-CPA-risk, mothers showed less efficiency when searching for happy facial expressions; meanwhile, self-reported emotional ratings of the faces did not differ. Consistent with SIP models, our findings suggest that low- and high-CPA-risk mothers differ regarding the speed by which they detect positive facial expressions, but not in how they explicitly evaluate them. On the CPA-risk continuum, less efficient detection of positive facial expressions in the initial processes of the SIP system may begin to manifest in the mild physical discipline (punishment) stage and decrease the likelihood of producing positive interpersonal experiences, consequently leading to an increased risk of CPA and communication conflicts with others in parental caregiving settings.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

ABM:

Attentional bias modification

ANOVA:

Analysis of variance

BDI-II:

Beck Depression Inventory-II

BPAQ:

Buss-Perry Aggression Questionnaire

CPA:

Child physical abuse

GBVS:

Graph-Based Visual Saliency

HOME:

Home Observation for Measurement of Environment

ICCE:

Index of Child Care Environment

IRI:

Interpersonal Reactivity Index

J-PSI:

Japanese version of the Parenting Stress Index

PBI:

Parental Bonding Instrument

RT:

Response time

SIP:

Social-information processing

WBFS:

Wong-Baker Faces Pain Rating Scale

References

  1. Straus MA, Sugarman DB, Giles-Sims J. Spanking by parents and subsequent antisocial behavior of children. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 1997;151:761–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Afifi TO, Mota NP, Dasiewicz P, MacMillan HL, Sareen J. Physical punishment and mental disorders: results from a nationally representative US sample. Pediatrics. 2012;130:184–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Afifi TO, Mota N, MacMillan HL, Sareen J. Harsh physical punishment in childhood and adult physical health. Pediatrics. 2013;132(2):e333–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Ferguson CJ. Spanking, corporal punishment and negative long-term outcomes: a meta-analytic review of longitudinal studies. Clin Psychol Rev. 2013;33:196–208.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Gershoff ET, Grogan-Kaylor A. Spanking and child outcomes: old controversies and new meta-analyses. J Fam Psychol. 2016;30:453–69.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Okuzono S, Fujiwara T, Kato T, Kawachi I. Spanking and subsequent behavioral problems in toddlers: a propensity score-matched, prospective study in Japan. Child Abuse Negl. 2017;69:62–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Sheu YS, Polcari A, Anderson CM, Teicher MH. Harsh corporal punishment is associated with increased T2 relaxation time in dopamine-rich regions. Neuroimage. 2010;53:412–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Tomoda A, Suzuki H, Rabi K, Sheu YS, Polcari A, Teicher MH. Reduced prefrontal cortical gray matter volume in young adults exposed to harsh corporal punishment. Neuroimage. 2009;47:T66–71.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Afifi TO, Ford D, Gershoff ET, Merrick M, Grogan-Kaylor A, Ports KA, et al. Spanking and adult mental health impairment: the case for the designation of spanking as an adverse childhood experience. Child Abuse Negl. 2017;71:24–31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Durrant J, Ensom R. Physical punishment of children: lessons from 20 years of research. Can Med Assoc J. 2012;184:1373–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Fergusson DM, Lynskey MT. Physical punishment/maltreatment during childhood and adjustment in young adulthood. Child Abuse Negl. 1997;21:617–30.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Österman K, Björkqvist K, Wahlbeck K. Twenty-eight years after the complete ban on the physical punishment of children in Finland: trends and psychosocial concomitants. Aggress Behav. 2014;40:568–81.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Rodriguez CM. Parent-child aggression: association with child abuse potential and parenting styles. Violence Vict. 2010;25:728–41.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Zolotor AJ, Theodore AD, Chang JJ, Berkoff MC, Runyan DK. Speak softly—and forget the stick: corporal punishment and child physical abuse. Am J Prev Med. 2008;35:364–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Global Initiative to End All Corporal Punishment of Children. States prohibiting all corporal punishment of children. 2019 https://endcorporalpunishment.org/global-progress Accessed 3 July 2019.

  16. Azar ST, Weinzierl KM. Child maltreatment and childhood injury research: a cognitive behavioral approach. J Pediatr Psychol. 2005;30:598–614.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Azar ST, Reitz EB, Goslin MC. Mothering: thinking is part of the job description: application of cognitive views to understanding maladaptive parenting and doing intervention and prevention work. J Appl Dev Psychol. 2008;29:295–304.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Dodge KA. Translational science in action: hostile attributional style and the development of aggressive behavior problems. Dev Psychopathol. 2006;18:791–814.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Milner JS. Social information processing and physical child abuse. Clin Psychol Rev. 1993;13:275–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Milner JS. Social information processing in high-risk and physically abusive parents. Child Abuse Negl. 2003;27:7–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Pettit GS, Lansford JE, Malone PS, Dodge KA, Bates JE. Domain specificity in relationship history, social-information processing, and violent behavior in early adulthood. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2010;98:190–200.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. Camilo C, Garrido MV, Calheiros MM. Implicit measures of child abuse and neglect: a systematic review. Aggress Violent Behav. 2016;29:43–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Zelli A, Rowell Huesmann L, Cervone D. Social inference and individual differences in aggression: evidence for spontaneous judgments of hostility. Aggress Behav. 1995;21:405–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Crouch JL, Milner JS, Skowronski JJ, Farc MM, Irwin LM, Neese A. Automatic encoding of ambiguous child behavior in high and low risk for child physical abuse parents. J Fam Violence. 2010;25:73–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Hiraoka R, Crouch JL, Reo G, Wagner M, Milner JS, Skowronski JJ. Pain tolerance, pain sensitivity, and accessibility of aggression-related schemata in parents at-risk for child physical abuse. Child Abuse Negl. 2014;38:1840–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Crouch JL, Shelton CR, Bardeen JR, Hiraoka R, Milner JS, Skowronski JJ. Does attentional control mediate the association between adverse early experiences and child physical abuse risk? J Fam Violence. 2012;27(2):97–103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Dopke CA, Lundahl BW, Dunsterville E, Lovejoy MC. Interpretations of child compliance in individuals at high- and low-risk for child physical abuse. Child Abuse Negl. 2003;27:285–302.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Farc MM, Crouch JL, Skowronski JJ, Milner JS. Hostility ratings by parents at risk for child abuse: impact of chronic and temporary schema activation. Child Abuse Negl. 2008;32:177–93.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. McCarthy RJ, Crouch JL, Skowronski JJ, Milner JS, Hiraoka R, Rutledge E, et al. Child physical abuse risk moderates spontaneously inferred traits from ambiguous child behaviors. Child Abuse Negl. 2013;37:1142–51.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Crouch JL, Irwin LM, Milner JS, Skowronski JJ, Rutledge E, Davila AL. Do hostile attributions and negative affect explain the association between authoritarian beliefs and harsh parenting? Child Abuse Negl. 2017;67:13–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Rodriguez CM, Cook AE, Jedrziewski CT. Reading between the lines: implicit assessment of the association of parental attributions and empathy with abuse risk. Child Abuse Negl. 2012;36:564–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Crouch JL, Skowronski JJ, Milner JS, Harris B. Parental responses to infant crying: the influence of child physical abuse risk and hostile priming. Child Abuse Negl. 2008;32:702–10.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Crouch JL, Risser HJ, Skowronski JJ, Milner JS, Farc MM, Irwin LM. Does accessibility of positive and negative schema vary by child physical abuse risk? Child Abuse Negl. 2010;34:886–95.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Miles L, Johnston L. Detecting happiness: perceiver sensitivity to enjoyment and non-enjoyment smiles. JNB. 2007;31:259–75.

    Google Scholar 

  35. Johnston L, Miles L, Macrae CN. Why are you smiling at me? Social functions of enjoyment and non-enjoyment smiles. Br J Soc Psychol. 2010;49:107–27.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Anme T, Tanaka E, Watanabe T, Tomisaki E, Mochizuki Y, Tokutake K. Validity and reliability of the index of child care environment (ICCE). Public Health Front. 2013;2:141–5.

    Google Scholar 

  37. Caldwell BM, Bradley RH. HOME inventory and administration manual. Little Rock (AR): University of Arkansas for Medical Sciences/University of Arkansas at Little Rock; 2001.

    Google Scholar 

  38. Becker DV, Anderson US, Mortensen CR, Neufeld SL, Neel R. The face in the crowd effect unconfounded: happy faces, not angry faces, are more efficiently detected in single- and multiple-target visual search tasks. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2011;140:637–59.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Craig BM, Becker SI, Lipp OV. Different faces in the crowd: a happiness superiority effect for schematic faces in heterogeneous backgrounds. Emotion. 2014;14:794–803.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Grubert A, Indino M, Krummenacher J. From features to dimensions: cognitive and motor development in pop-out search in children and young adults. Front Psychol. 2014;5:519.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Decety J, Bartal IB, Uzefovsky F, Knafo-Noam A. Empathy as a driver of prosocial behaviour: highly conserved neurobehavioural mechanisms across species. Philos Trans R Soc Lond Ser B Biol Sci. 2016;371(1686):20150077.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Dulac C, O’Connell LA, Wu Z. Neural control of maternal and paternal behaviors. Science. 2014;345:765–70.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  43. Rilling JK, Young LJ. The biology of mammalian parenting and its effect on offspring social development. Science. 2014;345:771–6.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  44. Hildyard K, Wolfe D. Cognitive processes associated with child neglect. Child Abuse Negl. 2007;31:895–907.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Francis KJ, Wolfe DA. Cognitive and emotional differences between abusive and non-abusive fathers. Child Abuse Negl. 2008;32:1127–37.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Pearson RM, Evans J, Kounali D, Lewis G, Heron J, Ramchandani PG, et al. Maternal depression during pregnancy and the postnatal period: risks and possible mechanisms for offspring depression at age 18 years. JAMA Psychiatry. 2013;70:1312–9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  47. Organization for Economic Cooperation and Development. Society at a glance: OECD social indicators. Paris: OECD Publishing; 2016.

  48. Save the Children Japan. Toward a society without punishment to hurt the body and mind of children. 2018 http://www.savechildren.or.jp/jpnem/jpn/pdf/php_report201802.pdf Accessed 3 July 2019.

  49. Ando A, Soga S, Yamasaki K, Shimai S, Shimada H, Utsuki N, et al. Development of the Japanese version of the Buss-Perry aggression questionnaire (BAQ). Jap J Psychol. 1999;70:384–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Buss AH, Perry M. The aggression questionnaire. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1992;63(3):452–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Davis MH. Measuring individual differences in empathy: evidence for a multidimensional approach. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1983;44(1):113–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Sakurai S. The relationship between empathy and helping behavior in college students. Bull Nara Univ Educ Cult Soc Sci. 1988;37:149–54.

    Google Scholar 

  53. Narama M, Kanematsu Y, Araki A, Maru M, Nakamura N, Takeda J, et al. Validity and reliability of the Japanese version of the parenting stress index. J Child Health. 1999;58:610–6.

    Google Scholar 

  54. Abidin RR. Parenting stress index professional manual. Psychological Assessment Resources: Lutz (FL); 1995.

    Google Scholar 

  55. Beck AT, Steer RA, Brown GK. Manual for the Beck depression inventory-II. San Antonio (TX): Psychological Corporation; 1996.

    Google Scholar 

  56. Kojima M, Furukawa TA, Takahashi H, Kawai M, Nagaya T, Tokudome S. Cross-cultural validation of the Beck depression inventory-II in Japan. Psychiatry Res. 2002;110:291–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Kitamura T, Suzuki T. A validation study of the parental bonding instrument in a Japanese population. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1993;47:29–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Parker G, Tupling H, Brown LB. A parental bonding instrument. Br J Med Psychol. 1979;52:1–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Wong DL, Baker CM. Pain in children: comparison of assessment scales. Pediatr Nurs. 1988;14:9–17.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Russell JA. A circumplex model of affect. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1980;39:1161–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  61. Posner J, Russell JA, Peterson BS. The circumplex model of affect: an integrative approach to affective neuroscience, cognitive development, and psychopathology. Dev Psychopathol. 2005;17:715–34.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  62. Harel J, Koch C, Perona P. Graph-based visual saliency. Adv Neural Inf Process Syst. 2007;19:545–52.

    Google Scholar 

  63. Eimer M. The neural basis of attentional control in visual search. Trends Cogn Sci. 2014;18:526–35.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Potegal M, Knutson JF. The dynamics of aggression: biological and social processes in dyads and groups. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc: Hillsdale, NJ, US; 1994.

    Google Scholar 

  65. Preston SD, Hofelich AJ. The many faces of empathy: parsing empathic phenomena through a proximate, dynamic-systems view of representing the other in the self. Emot Rev. 2012;4:24–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  66. Preston SD. The origins of altruism in offspring care. Psychol Bull. 2013;139:1305–41.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Shimada K, Kasaba R, Fujisawa TX, Sakakibara N, Takiguchi S, Tomoda A. Subclinical maternal depressive symptoms modulate right inferior frontal response to inferring affective mental states of adults but not of infants. J Affect Disord. 2018;229:32–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Andreoni J. Impure altruism and donations to public goods: a theory of warm-glow giving. Econ J. 1990;100:464–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  69. de Waal FB, Preston SD. Mammalian empathy: behavioural manifestations and neural basis. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2017;18:498–509.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Majdandžić M, de Vente W, Feinberg ME, Aktar E, Bögels SM. Bidirectional associations between coparenting relations and family member anxiety: a review and conceptual model. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. 2012;15:28–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. McHale JP, Kuersten-Hogan R, Rao N. Growing points for coparenting theory and research. J Adult Dev. 2004;11:221–34.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  72. Fiske ST, Cuddy AJ, Glick P. Universal dimensions of social cognition: warmth and competence. Trends Cogn Sci. 2007;11:77–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Van Kleef GA, De Dreu CK, Manstead AS. An interpersonal approach to emotion in social decision making: the emotions as social information model. In: Zanna MP, editor. Adv Exp Soc Psychol. Oxford: Academic Press; 2010. p. 45–96.

    Google Scholar 

  74. Shimada K, Takiguchi S, Mizushima S, Fujisawa TX, Saito DN, Kosaka H, et al. Reduced visual cortex grey matter volume in children and adolescents with reactive attachment disorder. Neuroimage Clin. 2015;9:13–9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  75. Takiguchi S, Fujisawa TX, Mizushima S, Saito DN, Okamoto Y, Shimada K, et al. Ventral striatum dysfunction in children and adolescents with reactive attachment disorder: functional MRI study. BJPsych Open. 2015;1:121–8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  76. Tomoda A, Polcari A, Anderson CM, Teicher MH. Reduced visual cortex gray matter volume and thickness in young adults who witnessed domestic violence during childhood. PLoS One. 2012;7(12):e52528.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  77. Prinz RJ, Sanders MR, Shapiro CJ, Whitaker DJ, Lutzker JR. Population-based prevention of child maltreatment: the U.S. triple p system population trial. Prev Sci. 2009;10:1–12.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  78. Sanders MR, Pidgeon AM, Gravestock F, Connors MD, Brown S, Young RW. Does parental attributional retraining and anger management enhance the effects of the triple P-positive parenting program with parents at risk of child maltreatment? Behav Ther. 2004;35:513–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  79. Sanders MR, Kirby JN, Tellegen CL, Day JJ. The triple P-positive parenting program: a systematic review and meta-analysis of a multi-level system of parenting support. Clin Psychol Rev. 2014;34:337–57.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. Dandeneau SD, Baldwin MW, Baccus JR, Sakellaropoulo M, Pruessner JC. Cutting stress off at the pass: reducing vigilance and responsiveness to social threat by manipulating attention. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2007;93:651–66.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  81. Waters AM, Zimmer-Gembeck MJ, Craske MG, Pine DS, Bradley BP, Mogg K. A preliminary evaluation of a home-based, computer-delivered attention training treatment for anxious children living in regional communities. J Exp Psychopath. 2016;7:511–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  82. Witte S, Fegert JM, Walper S. Risk of maltreatment for siblings: factors associated with similar and different childhood experiences in a dyadic sample of adult siblings. Child Abuse Negl. 2018;76:321–33.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  83. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang AG, Buchner AG. *power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods 2007;39:175–191.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  84. Stein T, Sterzer P. Not just another face in the crowd: detecting emotional schematic faces during continuous flash suppression. Emotion. 2012;12:988–96.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  85. Calvo MG, Fernández-Martín A. Can the eyes reveal a person’s emotions? Biasing role of the mouth expression. Motiv Emot. 2013;37:202–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank all of the mothers who participated in our study, and also the staff at the Research Center for Child Mental Development for their cooperation.

Funding

This study was supported, in part, by Grants-in-Aid for Young Scientists (B) (JP16K16622), Early-Career Scientists (JP19K14174) and Scientific Research (A) (JP19H00617) from the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (JSPS), and a Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research on Innovative Areas (JP16H01637) from the Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science, and Technology (MEXT) of Japan. This study was also partially supported by a Grant-in-Aid for “Creating a Safe and Secure Living Environment in the Changing Public and Private Spheres” from the Japan Science and Technology Corporation (JST)/Research Institute of Science and Technology for Society (RISTEX) and a research grant from the Takeda Science Foundation. The funders had no role in study design, data collection, analysis, interpretation, writing up nor the decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

KS conceptualized and designed the study. KS and RK collected the data. KS and RK analyzed the data. KS wrote the first draft of the manuscript. KS, RK, AY and AT edited and revised subsequent drafts of the manuscript. All authors approved the final version of the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Koji Shimada.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The study protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Fukui, and was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and the Ethical Guidelines for Clinical Studies published by the Ministry of Health, Labour, and Welfare of Japan. All participants signed an informed consent form.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Shimada, K., Kasaba, R., Yao, A. et al. Less efficient detection of positive facial expressions in parents at risk of engaging in child physical abuse. BMC Psychol 7, 56 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-019-0333-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-019-0333-9

Keywords