Skip to main content

Attachment insecurities, emotion dynamics and stress in intimate relationships during the transition to parenthood

Abstract

Background

In intimate relationships, which are characterized by emotional interdependence, partners act as attachment figures which serve emotion regulation functions. The experience of emotions as well as the strategies that partners use to regulate them and to respond to relational experiences, especially during stressful periods, differ greatly according to their attachment orientation. An important aspect in emotion dynamics is emotional inertia, which reflects the degree to which a person’s current affective state is resistant to change on a moment-to-moment basis. Inertia has been related to maladaptive emotion regulation strategies, like suppression and rumination, preferentially used by highly anxious and avoidant individuals. The aim of this study is to examine associations between attachment orientations and reports on the experience of positive and negative affect, and their dynamics in daily life across the transition to parenthood.

Methods

Longitudinal data from a sample of 152 mixed-gender couples collected across the transition to parenthood was analyzed. We predicted that individuals with a more insecure attachment would report more negative and less positive affect, and that their emotional experience would be more resistant to change over time. We explored effects when participants reported feeling stressed.

Results

The data suggested that attachment anxiety was associated with less positive and more negative affect and that attachment avoidance was associated with more positive affect. Anxious individuals showed lower emotional inertia and not higher as we expected. Reported stress for anxious and avoidant individuals was significantly associated with more negative but not less positive affect.

Conclusions

Results are discussed in the light of their impact on couples during stressful periods. Differences between anxiety and avoidance are found, emphasizing the importance of attachment insecurities on the experience of emotion. Furthermore, our findings on momentary fluctuating affect offer complementary insight into the emotional functioning of individuals with different attachment orientations.

Peer Review reports

Introduction

Emotions are a central aspect of how individuals experience their intimate relationships [1]. They help signaling and communicating personal needs and concerns to oneself and to the partner [2, 3]. Emotions are not static. Rather, they emerge and dissolve across time [4] and their expression and experience during interactions with intimate partners are predictive of relationship functioning and development [5]. The factors that shape emotional experience in intimate relationships are therefore important for individual well-being and relationship functioning, particularly during important life transitions that challenge couples, such as the transition to parenthood. An important factor influencing both emotion regulation and relationship functioning is adult attachment orientation [6]. When individuals enter new relationships, they bring with them so-called internal working models of relationships– including assumptions and expectations about how people behave in relationships– shaped by their history of interpersonal experiences [7]. Such attachment orientations guide relationships [8], and shape how individuals feel about and respond to their intimate partners [9]. Differences in attachment orientations affect patterns of emotional experience, as well as the strategies individuals use to regulate their emotions, especially when dealing with stress [10]. Attachment orientations thus, should shape daily emotional experiences in intimate relationships [11]. The current study examines this possibility during the transition to parenthood, examining the link between attachment orientations, momentary emotional experiences and stress.

In the remainder of the introduction, we first discuss the literature on emotion and emotion dynamics as related to individual and relational functioning. We then discuss attachment orientations, and how they may shape the experience of emotions in intimate romantic relationships.

Emotion dynamics

Emotions are characterized by frequent change [12]. They fluctuate over time and are thought to be continuously regulated to optimally fit with the current desired state [13]. People differ greatly in their emotional responses to environmental challenges [14], and some individuals show a relatively high degree of resistance to change of their emotions, commonly referred to as emotional inertia [15]. High emotional inertia reflects a pattern of emotions characterized by a high degree of moment-to-moment stability of emotions, with emotions being resistant to external (changes in the environment) and internal (regulation efforts) influences. Low emotional inertia reflects a pattern of emotions characterized by high moment-to-moment instability of emotions [15]. High emotional inertia may reflect a lack of emotion regulation [16], such that emotions carry over from one moment to the next, possibly because people tend to perceive and interpret the world around them in ways congruent with their current affective state [13]. Some possible explanations have been proposed for the occurrence of static emotions. For example, emotional inertia may result from the inability to regulate emotions in an efficient way even if there is a motivation to do so [17]. Also, exposure to intense events eliciting strong emotional reactions and difficulty downregulating elicited emotions, can result in an increased emotional dependency over time [18]. Studies suggest that elevated inertia not only of negative, but also of positive emotion is reliably associated with poorer well-being [15, 19]. Specifically, emotional inertia of both positive and negative emotions was associated with low self-esteem and depression [15]. Although these findings need further examination and replication, they point to the possibility that positive emotions that are slow to change across contexts may also be reflective of an affective system with limited responsiveness to environmental cues, possibly hampering regulatory processes [20]. Inert patterns of emotion may point to a lack of emotional flexibility and restrict an individual’s capacity to actively respond to situational challenges across varying conditions. They have also been associated with poorer psychological adjustment [21], impaired social functioning [22], and lower relationship quality [23]. Highly inert emotion patterns over time may therefore serve as a relevant proxy of maladjustment in everyday life.

Attachment and emotion in romantic relationships across the transition to parenthood

Attachment theory provides a framework for explaining how early relational dynamics with caregivers add to emotional stability in adulthood [24]. In intimate relationships, the partner or spouse represents an important attachment figure [25] and when people face challenges and feel stressed, they often attend to their closest relations to maintain or re-establish security and comfort [26]. They seek emotional or instrumental support from their romantic partners [27] although the specific strategies people will use to achieve this goal vary as a function of their attachment history [26].

Individuals with a secure attachment orientation can rely on others during times of need [28], they attribute less hostile intent to others when tensions arise [29] and they experience more positive emotions in their relationships [30]. Individuals with an anxious attachment orientation tend to focus on negative emotions [11] and overemphasize their helplessness [31]. They experience sadness more frequently [6] and report less positive emotions in the relationship [30]. Furthermore, they appear to be more distressed [32], ruminate, and harbor more anger [29], probably resulting from the expectation that people are unpredictable and inconsistent [24]. Individuals with an avoidant attachment orientation mask or inhibit expressions of emotion [11], to avoid appearing vulnerable and experiencing further rejection-related distress [33]. They tend to believe that their needs will not be met in a particular relationship [24], so they minimize experiences of negative affect [34, 35], and engage in emotional withdrawal [36]. At the same time, individuals higher in avoidance orientation, have also been found to report more frequent negative and fewer positive emotions in their relationship [30, 37, 38], more intense negative emotional reactions after the occurrence of negative events [39], more intense physiological recordings of anger, as well as higher cortisol levels during conflict [40].

The transition to parenthood constitutes a major challenge in one’s life and is for many marked by periods of stress and emotional turmoil, which can negatively impact relationship quality [41,42,43,44]. Parenthood brings new roles and responsibilities for mothers and fathers, and both partners need to adjust to a multitude of changes and stressors during the transition to parenthood [45]. Indeed, such changes in daily life tend to contribute to higher levels of stress during the transition to parenthood [41,42,43,44, 46]. As a result, these changes may undermine personal and relational well-being [47]. It may well be a time when attachment processes are particularly activated [48], and how the becoming parents adjust to this transition is shaped by their attachment orientation. As a consequence, their relationships will also differ in their emotional tone [30]. Specifically, highly anxious women enter the transition to parenthood perceiving lower levels of spousal support, leading to declines in marital satisfaction [49] and increases in depressive symptoms [50]. Parents higher in avoidance report more difficulties in adjusting to parenthood [51] and higher parenting stress [52].

In sum, the literature underscores the importance of attachment orientations for individual adjustment, shaping the experience of emotions in intimate relationships [53]. Individual differences in attachment orientations are reflected in different perceptions of oneself and others, shaping the experience of emotions in intimate relationships, particularly during times of stress, like the transition to parenthood.

Attachment, emotion, and emotion dynamics in romantic relationships

Research has indicated that individuals exhibit variations in their average levels of positive and negative emotions [54], highlighting how people are distinguished based on their typical emotional experiences. Particularly, people differ widely in their preferences for the strategies they use to regulate their own and their partner’s emotions. For instance, rumination, a maladaptive process involving repetitively thinking about negative emotions [55] was found to be associated with emotional inertia, and individuals with an anxious attachment orientation tend to use this strategy more than their securely attached counterparts [21, 56]. The stronger and more persistent repetitive thoughts occur on a particular day, the greater the impact on affective states that persist across different occasions [57] and result in intense negative feelings, such as anger [58], and in distressed close relationships [59, 60].

Suppression of emotion expression is another maladaptive emotion regulation strategy that has been linked to emotional inertia. Individuals with an avoidant attachment orientation tend to use suppression more than individuals with a secure or anxious attachment orientation [56, 57] probably for self-protective reasons [33]. Suppression does not alleviate negative emotions [61]. Indeed, individuals who tend to use suppression as a strategy report lower levels of intimacy and more negative emotional experiences [9, 62].

Overall, a high degree of emotional inertia may mirror a lack of emotional flexibility [12] and is associated with emotion regulation strategies preferentially used by anxious and avoidant individuals [63]. Therefore, since individuals high in anxiety and avoidance use emotion regulation strategies associated with emotional inertia, this study examines whether people exhibiting higher levels of insecurity will experience more negative and less positive affect and also exhibit higher emotional inertia.

The current study

The objective of this study was to investigate potential associations between attachment orientations and distinct patterns of momentary emotional experiences, emotional responses and emotional inertia in a daily life context during the transition to parenthood. The exploration of the relationships between attachment orientations and individuals’ emotional experiences, responses, and the dynamic patterns of change in daily life is of considerable significance because both insecure attachment orientations and emotional dynamics have previously been correlated with psychological maladjustment and mental health issues [15, 49].

The attachment system is activated in stressful and threatening instances helping to regulate stress that is associated with perceived threat by controlling proximity to the attachment figure [26, [7]–in the case of intimate relationships, the partner. In other words, the attachment system serves stress and emotion regulation purposes, and therefore the effects of an insecure attachment style should become apparent at the level of emotional responses and dynamics when people experience stress. Having a baby can be a joyful but also chronically stressful experience and attachment insecurities make individuals more vulnerable to personal and interpersonal problems during this time [64]. Bowlby (1988) [65] believed that the transition to parenthood should be a critical period for systematic changes in the attachment system.

The transition to parenthood is a major life transition characterized by challenges that often affect relationships [45] sometimes leading to marked decreases in marital satisfaction and personal well-being [66]. Highly anxious women enter the transition to parenthood perceiving less spousal support and with steeper declines in their marital satisfaction [66] prior to the baby’s birth [67]. It has also been found within a sample of couples expecting their first baby, that partners experience more negative affect due to increased stress [45], more negative perceptions, and decreases in relationship satisfaction [68]. We assessed individuals’ emotional experiences in daily life using a momentary assessment approach during the transition to parenthood [69].

We hypothesized that individuals higher in attachment anxiety (H1a) and individuals higher in avoidance (H1b) would generally experience less positive and more negative affect. On more exploratory grounds, we further specified this hypothesis such that individuals higher in attachment anxiety and avoidancewould report more negative and less positive emotional experiences than their more secure counterparts, in response to momentary stress experiences (i.e. when they reported feeling stressed). Finally, we hypothesized that individuals with higher levels of anxiety (H2a), as well as individuals with higher levels of avoidance (H2b) would exhibit more inert positive and negative emotional experiences than relatively secure individuals.

Method

Participants

Participants were recruited from flyers and word-of-mouth advertisement. Eligible couples had to speak the study languages, be over 18 years old, live in the same household and expecting their first child. The current sample consisted of 152 mixed-gender couples (n = 304). The mean age was 31.55 for women (SD = 3.66) and 33.19 for men (SD = 4.06). The mean relationship duration was 6.73 years (SD = 3.01). The sample size was determined before recruitment started, based on a priori power estimation of a medium sized between-subject effect (effect size r =.25) on a within-subject slope, which suggested statistical power of 0.8 for 286 participants from 143 couples.

Procedure

The data for this study are part of a longitudinal study with four measurements (pregnancy, 6, 12 and 18 months after the birth of the first child). The data used for the current analyses were collected at the first measurement - in the second or third trimester of pregnancy. First, each participant completed an online survey that included questions on demographic characteristics, mental health, well-being, interpersonal dispositions, attachment orientation, and on evaluations of the relationship. Second, participants completed a smartphone-based momentary assessment procedure during seven consecutive days, four times per day (8h00, 12h00, 18h00 and 21h30). It included sets of questions related to participants’ momentary emotional states and interpersonal experiences. Participants received a message in order to complete the assessment at each time point of the day and they could complete it in the next ninety minutes. Before starting the daily assessment, all participants were provided with detailed instructions on the use of the momentary assessment. Other assessments of the study included a diagnostic telephone interview on mental health, home visits with interaction tasks, physiological measures and an evaluation week, which included a three-day assessment of physiological measures and the seven-day momentary assessment. All participants completed and signed the informed consent form and after each assessment point, they received approximately 180 $ per couple. The project was approved by the ethics review board of the regional government. The study and hypotheses were not pre-registered.

Measures

Attachment orientation

The Experiences in Close Relationships-Revised [70] French version [71] and German version [72] was used to assess individual attachment on two dimensions: anxiety and avoidance. The questionnaire includes 36 items, of which 18 assess attachment anxiety, the degree to which individuals feel insecure about the availability and responsiveness of their partner (e.g., I need confirmation that my partner loves me; females: α = 0.867; males: α = 0.878), and another 18 items assess attachment avoidance, the degree to which individuals avoid closeness and intimacy (e.g., I try to avoid getting too close to my partner; females: α = 0.873; males: α = 0.815). Participants responded to each item using a 7-point scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree), rating the extent to which each item is descriptive of how they usually feel and behave in romantic relationships. Ratings were averaged to compute scores for each dimension. Higher scores reflected more anxious and more avoidant attachment orientations.

Emotional states

Emotions were assessed by daily diaries and participants rated the degree to which they felt “happy”, in a “good mood”, “depressed”, “irritated”, and “worried”. A continuous slider on a 10-point-scale was used, ranging from 1 (not at all) to 10 (extremely). The ratings of the items “happy” and “good mood” were averaged to form a Positive Affect (PA) measure and the two items were highly consistent within person (Ω = 0.858) and between individuals (Ω = 0.976). The ratings of the items “depressed”, “irritated”, and “worried”, were averaged to reflect negative affect (NA). This momentary NA measure showed satisfactory internal consistency within person (Ω = 0.652) and between individuals (Ω = 0.853). Between individuals internal consistency was high for PA (females: α = 0.965; males: α = 0.969) and for NA (females: α = 0.853; males: α = 0.836).

Emotional inertia

Emotional inertia is defined as resistance to emotional change, commonly formalized as the degree to which a person’s current emotional state can be predicted by his or her emotional state at a prior moment. Thus, high emotional inertia suggests that a person’s current emotional experience is likely to persist from one moment to the next and compromise an individual’s capacity to respond adaptively across varying conditions and demands. On the contrary, moderate emotional inertia indicates that a person’s emotional states are more changeable across time [15]. Low levels of emotional inertia may indicate emotional instability and undermine adaptive behavior [73]. Emotional inertia as addressed in this study, reflects an important aspect of a person’s emotion dynamics across hours in daily life, and is operationalized as the first-order autocorrelation among repeated measures of emotional states.

Stress

Stress was assessed by participants’ daily ratings of the degree to which they felt stress at a specific moment (“Currently, how stressed do you feel?”). A continuous slider on a 10-point-scale was used to measure the extent that participants felt stressed, ranging from 1 (not at all) to 10 (extremely).

Data analysis

We used a mixed modelling approach to analyze the data, as it allows to model dependencies of individuals’ repeated reports within time points and couples. Our sample included mixed-gender couples, so dyads’ members were distinguishable by their reported gender [74]. However, model comparisons suggested a better fit for models with pooled estimates instead of separate coefficients for the two genders (likelihood ratios > 540.795, df = 16, p <.001), and we therefore present models with pooled estimates.

We set up a dyadic model that captures both partners’ reports sampled at the same time points repeatedly and clustered within couples. Models were run with the package nlme [75] in R Studio [76]. Level 1 predictors (lagged emotion and stress) were centered at the person mean and Level 2 predictors (anxiety and avoidance) were centered at the grand mean. We estimated random intercepts and allowed for random variation of lagged emotion slopes and stress slopes across couples.

We ran two different models, one predicting PA reports and one predicting NA reports, to test whether attachment orientations predicted emotional states, and also emotional inertia and fluctuation of emotional states as a function of stress reports. Specifically, we ran models testing anxiety (H1a) and avoidance (H1b) as predictors of current PA and NA. We also included interactions with stress reports to test whether anxiety and avoidance predicted positive and negative affect as associated with stress reports. And to test our predictions that anxiety (H2a) and avoidance (H2b) were associated with greater emotional inertia, we included interaction effects between attachment anxiety and attachment avoidance and lagged PA reports and between attachment anxiety and avoidance and lagged NA reports respectively, as predictors of concurrent PA or NA.

Equation 1 shows the model, examining within person fluctuations of PA or NA as a function of lagged PA or NA, and stress, as associated with attachment anxiety and avoidance (and adjusted for a general trend across repeated measurements). Within person predictors: The coefficient for lagged PA or NA (π1i) reflects the extent to which the current emotional state is a function of the emotional state reported at the prior measurement (t-1), and therefore represents emotional inertia, while the coefficient for stress (π2i) reflects the extent to which stress reports predicted less positive or more negative emotional states. The coefficient for π3ti captures the general trend in PA or NA across the reporting period.

$$\eqalign{{\rm{Emotio}}{{\rm{n}}_{ti}}\,{\rm{ = }} & {{\rm{\pi }}_{0i}}\,{\rm{ + }}\,{{\rm{\pi }}_{1ti}}\,\left( {{\rm{emotio}}{{\rm{n}}_{\rm{t}}}{{\rm{ - }}_{{\rm{1i}}}}} \right)\,{\rm{ + }}\,{{\rm{\pi }}_{2ti}}\,\left( {{\rm{stress}}} \right) \cr & {\rm{ + }}\,{{\rm{\pi }}_{3ti}}\,\left( {{\rm{trend}}} \right)\,{\rm{ + }}\,{{\rm{\pi }}_{4i}}\,\left( {{\rm{Anxiety}}} \right) \cr & {\rm{ + }}\,{{\rm{\pi }}_{5i}}\,\left( {{\rm{Avoidance}}} \right)\,{\rm{ + }}\,{{\rm{\pi }}_{6i}}\left( {{\rm{Anxiety}}\, * \,{\rm{stress}}} \right) \cr & {\rm{ + }}\,{{\rm{\pi }}_{7i}}\,\left( {{\rm{Avoidance}}\, * \,{\rm{stress}}} \right)\,{\rm{ + }}\,{{\rm{\pi }}_{{\rm{8}}i}}\,\left( {{\rm{Anxiety}}\, * \,{\rm{emotio}}{{\rm{n}}_t}{{\rm{ - }}_{1i}}} \right) \cr & {\rm{ + }}{{\rm{\pi }}_{9i}}\,\left( {{\rm{Avoidance}}\, * \,{\rm{emotio}}{{\rm{n}}_t}{{\rm{ - }}_{1i}}} \right){\rm{ + }}{{\rm{e}}_{ti}} \cr} $$
(1)

Between-person predictors and interaction terms: The coefficients π4i and π5i capture the associations between anxiety or avoidance and individual differences in average PA and NA reports. The coefficients for π6i and π7i are interaction effects between attachment orientations and stress, capturing the extent to which anxious or avoidant attachment orientations are associated with the stress effects on PA or NA. The coefficients for π8i and π9i represent interaction effects between lagged PA or NA and attachment anxiety and avoidance. These coefficients represent moderator effects of the first order autoregressive effects of positive or negative affect across repeated measurements. Positive coefficients would indicate that prior affect reports of more anxious or avoidant individuals would more strongly and positively predict subsequent affect reports, which can be interpreted as PA or NA inertia being stronger in more anxious or avoidant individuals. The error term eti denotes the residual variance.

First, we ran models with separate coefficients for female and male participants. Next, we compared the fit of these models with that of models in which we imposed equality constraints for female and male participants’ coefficients for those coefficients relevant to test the hypotheses. These models comparisons yielded superior AIC and BIC indices for all models with constraints and suggested that estimating separate parameter estimates for men and women did not improve the model fit significantly (χ2 (2)[] > 3.001, p >.223). As a result, we report models with pooled estimates.

Results

Descriptive statistics

Table 1 presents the means (M) and the standard deviation (SD) for all our variables for women and men. Correlation coefficients among study variables are presented in Table 2. Across and within individuals M and SDs were calculated. Specifically, between all individuals, the average report of positive affect was M = 7.59 (SD = 1.73), the average report of negative affect was M = 1.06 (SD = 1.41) and the average report of stress was M = 1.74 (SD = 2.19). The within person variability was SD = 1.38 for positive affect, SD = 1.08 for negative affect and SD = 1.68 for stress. The intraclass correlation (ICC) for positive affect was 0.824 (CI 0.817 − 0.832) and for negative affect it was 0.462 (CI 0.447 − 0.476).

Table 1 Participants mean ratings of attachment orientation, stress and emotional states
Table 2 Correlation Matrix for Anxiety, Avoidance, Stress, PA, NA, PA-lagged, NA lagged

Attachment predicting daily affect

We predicted that both attachment anxiety (H1a) and avoidance (H2b) were associated with less PA and more NA reports. Overall, we found significant associations between anxiety, and PA and NA respectively, supporting H1a and partially supporting H1b (see Table 3). Specifically, anxiety was significantly and negatively associated with PA (b = − 0.335, p =.000), and positively with NA (b = 0.217, p =.000). Attachment avoidance was significantly associated with higher PA (b = 0.159, p =.043) but not with NA (b = − 0.064, p =.323; see Table 3).

Table 3 The effects of attachment anxiety, avoidance and stress on emotional states and lagged affect

Analyses for our exploratory hypotheses on stress suggested that momentary stress significantly predicted more NA but not less PA (cf. Table 3). Furthermore, regarding differences of stress effects on emotional states, the results were in line with our expectations for NA only. Attachment anxiety did not significantly interact with stress to predict PA (b = − 0.023, p =.162) but it significantly predicted the experience of more negative emotion (b = 0.078, p =.000). The same pattern emerged for avoidance. Avoidance did not significantly interact with stress to predict positive emotion (b = − 0.019, p =.614), but it did so predicting more negative emotion (b = 0.088, p =.006). Estimating simple slopes (using a tool proposed by Preacher, Curren & Bauer, 2006) [77] suggest that for individuals with low anxiety (1 SD below average), stress effects on NA were lower, but still significant (b = 0.191, p >.001), and they were substantial for individuals with 1 SD above average anxiety (b = 0.383, p >.001). Likewise, for individuals with moderate attachment avoidance (1 SD below average), stress effects on NA were somewhat lower (b = 0.239, p >.001), whereas they were stronger (b = 0.336, p >.001) in participants with elevated levels of attachment avoidance (1 SD above average). In summary, the results suggest that stress was more strongly associated with more negative emotional states for individuals with an anxious or avoidant attachment orientation, as compared to relatively secure individuals, but no equivalent differences emerged for stress associations with positive affect. To ensure that simple effects of attachment and lagged emotion are not affected by stress, we ran separate analyses where stress was not included as a predictor (cf. Table 4).

Table 4 The effects of attachment anxiety and avoidance on emotional states and lagged affect

Were affect reports of insecure participants more inert across time?

We predicted that individuals high in attachment anxiety and avoidance orientation would show more inert positive and negative affect reports (Hypotheses 2a and 2b, ). Only one significant association resulted (cf. Table 3), which was contrary to our hypothesis. The data suggested that attachment anxiety significantly moderated autoregressive effects of PA, reflecting inertia, such that in more anxious individuals, prior PA reports predicted current PA reports more negatively or less positively (b = − 0.055, p =.003). Specifically, estimating simple slopes suggest that an individual who scores 1 SD above average in attachment anxiety would have an autoregressive effect of PA of b = − 0.051, whereas an individual who scores 1 SD below average in attachment anxiety would have an autoregressive effect of b = 0.059, with none of these estimates reaching significance (p >.495). We found no significant association between attachment anxiety and NA inertia (b = − 0.003, p =.842). Likewise, attachment avoidance did not predict higher PA (b = − 0.069, p =.092) or NA (b = − 0.016, p =.721) inertia.

Discussion

The goal of this research was to examine whether individuals with insecure attachment orientations differed in their daily affective experience from individuals with more secure attachment orientations and explored associations with stress. We expected that individuals high in attachment anxiety orientation (H1a) and individuals high in attachment avoidance orientation (H2b) experienced less positive and more negative emotional states, and that people higher in anxiety (H2a) and avoidance (H2b) would show elevated inertia in their positive and negative affect reports.

Overall, the data provided support for generally lower positive and higher negative affect reports for individuals with an anxious attachment orientation, supporting H1a. In contrast, attachment avoidance was not significantly associated with negative affect. Rather, a small positive association emerged for positive affect, suggesting that individuals high in attachment avoidance may actually experience more positive affect, contradicting our hypothesis H1b. Exploratory analyses testing whether less positive and more negative emotional states were experienced by anxious and avoidant individuals particularly when facing stress might offer some additional insight. While attachment orientations were not predictive of emotional experiences as associated with stress, both anxious and avoidant orientations predicted stronger negative emotional responses in times of stress, which could indicate that attachment effects manifest themselves specifically in stressful situations [78] and in the form of more negative emotional responses to stress [79,80,81]. Future studies may want to test and confirm this possibility.

The hypothesized associations between attachment orientations and emotional inertia did not receive support (H2a, H2b). The results for anxious individuals contrast with this prediction, suggesting that these individuals experienced less stable affect, as reflected by lower positive emotional inertia. Taken together, the data yield partial support for H1a and H1b, and no support for H2a and H2b.

The results for hypothesis H1a are largely consistent with the notion that anxiously attached individuals tend to perceive negative emotions as congruent with their attachment goals to get attention, and they may seek to sustain them [82], because signs of weakness and neediness can sometimes elicit attachment figures’ attention and care [31]. Hyperactivating strategies like rumination, used by anxiously attached individuals, may intensify negative emotions and overt displays of distress [83], hostility, and sadness [1]. Furthermore, a possible explanation for the experience of less positive and more negative affect might be a higher reactivity and less adequate recovery from negative affect [2]. The finding that the differences in affect reports associated with attachment anxiety were also enhanced in times of momentary stress, is consistent with such a possibility.

The hypothesis H1b that individuals with an avoidant orientation were expected to experience lower levels of positive and higher levels of negative affect was only partially supported. According to previous research, avoidant individuals adopt deactivating strategies that allow distancing from distress-eliciting events [1]. This may cause them to avoid noticing– or even reporting– their own negative emotional reactions. Avoidant individuals tend to divert attention from emotion related material and defend against the conscious experience of unpleasant emotions [11] by denying or suppressing them [84], which may explain why avoidant individuals in this study experienced more positive affect. Therefore, considering the possibility that avoidants suppress their negative emotions, it would seem possible that they would only report higher positive experiences but not negative ones. Moreover, prior studies have also shown that suppression resulted in more positive behaviors (e.g. smiling, laughing) [63], which is in line with the findings of the current study. Individual and situational differences may also help explain these results. Avoidant individuals experience activation of attachment-related worries only under conditions of high cognitive load [84]. Specifically, in severe and persistent stressful conditions imposing increasing demands on their cognitive system, avoidant individuals exhibit high levels of distress [85], which was confirmed by the current data showing that avoidant individuals experienced more negative affect at times when they reported stress. It could be that if individuals did not experience a prolonged stressful situation, the avoidant attachment system was not activated and therefore, attachment avoidance was not significantly associated with more negative emotions in general. Contextual factors such as current interactions with a partner, the attachment dynamics of the partner and a person’s current life situation (e.g. physical and psychological traumas) also modify the tendency that an avoidant individual has for deactivation of the attachment system and the experience of negative emotions [1]. Moreover, there is some evidence that, rejecting parents often discourage emotional display in their children and they do not teach them how to label and represent their emotions. As such, avoidant individuals are found to experience more negative emotions but fail to acknowledge them [86] and have greater difficulty in describing these emotions [87] relative to anxious individuals. Consequently, these conditions may lead to later difficulties in affect regulation [88].

In contrast to our hypothesis H2a, proposing that anxious individuals would show more inert positive and negative emotional states, we observed that anxious individuals showed less inert positive dynamics. In line with these findings, Koval & Kuppens (2012b) found that more vulnerable individuals had a greater drop in inertia levels when they anticipated a stressor suggesting that it is stress vulnerability underlying changes in emotional inertia. Considering that anxious individuals seek proximity to gain support and love [89] aiming at establishing intimacy and closeness [1] maybe this leads to more frequent emotional fluctuations (i.e., lower emotional inertia). Yet, another possibility is that in anticipation of, or during a stressor– like the transition to parenthood– individuals might change their daily activities, which might influence changes in moment-to-moment feelings [21]. Therefore, assuming that anxious individuals would exhibit higher inert emotional experiences might not be true in the first place because of their hyperactivating strategies during stress. It is important to emphasize, however, that although the interaction effects were highly significant, the simple slopes did not suggest significant autoregressive associations of positive affect for individuals with a comparatively anxious attachment style (relative to the sample mean in anxious attachment, which reflects intermediate levels of attachment anxiety). Individuals high in avoidance did not report more inert positive or negative emotions as was predicted by hypothesis 2b. Thus, they may report less negative emotion in daily life and show a faster recovery– lower levels of inertia– when they experience them, in an effort to emotionally distance themselves from distress-eliciting events, which could cause proximity-seeking and lead to their partner’s rejection [1]. These findings are in line with previous findings, which have shown that suppression– an emotion regulation strategy preferentially used by avoidant individuals [33, 78] is linked to restricted emotional flexibility [57, 63]. Suppressing feelings that imply vulnerability, results in ignoring important information about stressful situations and as such they did not show inert negative emotional experiences. In addition, another consideration is that the difficulty in regulating their emotions makes avoidant individuals to keep anger and resentment alive internally while attempting not to express them externally [78], which we suggest it needs further exploration. We also suggest that the fact that insecure individuals appear to respond stronger with negative emotions to stress might point to more instable affect [78,79,80,81]; maybe more negative affect inertia would emerge after major stress experiences, which should be tested in future studies.

One consideration is that priming thoughts of a supportive attachment figure and cognitive models of self and others lead people high on anxiety or avoidance to behave like more secure individuals [90]. Individuals with avoidant workings models when they are in stressful situations and their romantic partners support them with unsolicited support, are rated as more calmed [91]. This indicates that such persons can benefit from support, which can in turn lead them to experience less negative affect and less inert dynamics because their partner’s support buffered their attachment-related concerns. In our sample, couples were relatively happy expecting their first child, which means that they might have encountered more supportive and positive emotional experiences. In this line of reasoning, anxiously or avoidantly oriented individuals are less inclined to feel and behave according to their insecure working models when they are involved in more committed relationships [92] or are more dependent on their partner [93]. For instance, when they encounter stressful situations these individuals are less likely to react in insecure ways when their partner buffers their attachment related concerns, helping to actually experience less negative affect [94].

One question that deserves attention is how affect reactivity and inertia go together and whether they can co-exist, especially when people experience and report stress. Our results show that stress predicts less positive and more negative affect generally. However, in both anxious and avoidant individuals reported feeling stressed predicts more negative but not less positive affect, raising the question of whether some people are more stress-reactive and what mechanisms are activated after a stressful experience that they might have failed to cope and regulate their emotions.

Limitations

The results reported in this study should be appreciated with caution due to several limitations. First, due the to the correlational nature of the analyses, causal inferences among attachment insecurity and emotion or emotional inertia cannot be drawn with certainty. Second, participants were healthy and involved in stable, relatively happy romantic relationships, expecting their first child. It is therefore unclear whether these results can be generalized to other relationships in the broader population. Third, the current study focused on attachment insecurity and its effect on the experience of affect and its over time dynamics. Several other factors that potentially influence affect experience and dynamics have not been considered in this study (e.g. felt intimacy, current partner interactions). Fourth, we assessed momentary affect four times a day and reported analyses using three time points (we did not include overnight estimates), and this might be too coarse a sampling grid to capture everyday emotional fluctuations. Considering this, it is reasonable to anticipate relatively modest cross-level interaction effects. Therefore, a more fine-grained assessment of momentary affect fluctuations may be necessary to capture fluctuations in affect inertia [57] or momentary emotional experiences.

Conclusion and future directions

Despite the limitations, our study advances the current understanding of how emotions and emotional inertia are associated with attachment anxiety and avoidance, especially when individuals feel stressed. Furthermore, studying momentary fluctuating affect offers complementary insight into the emotional functioning of individuals with different attachment orientations. Further research is needed to better understand the role of attachment in the experience of emotions, their sustainability through time and their impact on subsequent functioning and social interactions. It still needs to be clarified why affective experiences are more or less inert in some people than in other and how attachment orientation and felt stress potentially affect these experiences. The current study examined only the subjective feeling component of emotions. However, emotions are considered to also involve changes in behavior, and physiology [95]. Therefore, future studies should focus on temporal dynamics of emotions as the outcome of multiple factors and processes. Additional investigation on the role of gender and attachment in relation to emotions is warranted. The study of individual differences in attachment orientations, the experience of emotion and emotion dynamics, is likely to contribute to our understanding of why close relationships vary in both their quality and their interpersonal nature.

Data availability

A final dataset used for the analyses of this manuscript can be found here: https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/Y3BX8.

Abbreviations

a:

Cronbach’s coefficient of internal consistency

b:

Beta coefficient

p :

Probability value

r:

Pearson correlation coefficient

CI:

Confidence interval

ECR:

Experiences in close relationships

ICC:

Intraclass correlation coefficient

LA:

Lagged affect

MLM:

Multilevel modeling

NA:

Negative affect

PA:

Positive affect

SD:

Standard deviation

References

  1. Mikulincer M, Shaver PR. Attachment theory and emotions in close relationships: Exploring the attachment-related dynamics of emotional reactions to relational events. Pers Relatsh [Internet]. 2005;12(2):149–68. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1350-4126.2005.00108.x.

  2. Schoebi D, Randall AK. Emotional dynamics in intimate relationships. Emot Rev. 2015;7(4):342–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Butler EA. Emotions are temporal interpersonal systems. Vol. 17, current opinion in psychology. Elsevier B.V.; 2017. pp. 129–34.

  4. Kuppens P, Oravecz Z, Tuerlinckx F. Feelings change: accounting for individual differences in the Temporal Dynamics of Affect. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2010;99(6):1042–60.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Feeney BC, Collins NL. A New look at Social Support: a theoretical perspective on Thriving through relationships. Personal Soc Psychol Rev. 2015;19(2):113–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Feeney JA. Adult attchment and emotional control. Pers Relatsh. 1995;2(2):143–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Bowlby J. Attachment and loss. Vol. 2: separation: anxiety and anger. New York: Basic Books.; 1973.

    Google Scholar 

  8. Scharfe E. Attachment Theory. In: Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science [Internet]. Springer International Publishing; 2017. pp. 1–10. http://link.springer.com/https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-16999-6_3823-1.

  9. Collins NL. Working Models of Attachment: Implications for Explanation, Emotion, and Behavior. J Pers Soc Psychol [Internet]. 1996;71(4):810–32. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.71.4.810.

  10. Nielsen SKK, Lønfeldt N, Wolitzky-Taylor KB, Hageman I, Vangkilde S, Daniel SIF. Adult attachment style and anxiety– the mediating role of emotion regulation. J Affect Disord. 2017;218:253–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Mikulincer M, Shaver PR. Attachment orientations and emotion regulation. Curr Opin Psychol. 2019;25:6–10.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Koval P, Sütterlin S, Kuppens P. Emotional inertia is associated with lower well-being when controlling for differences in emotional context. Front Psychol. 2016;6(JAN):1–11.

    Google Scholar 

  13. Kuppens P, Verduyn P. Emotion dynamics. Curr Opin Psychol [Internet]. 2017;17:22–6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.06.004.

  14. Lane AM, Beedie C, Devonport TJ. Emotion and emotion regulation. 2011.

  15. Kuppens P, Allen NB, Sheeber LB. Emotional inertia and psychological maladjustment. Psychol Sci. 2010;21(7):984–91.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Kuppens P, Verduyn P. Looking at emotion regulation through the window of emotion Dynamics. Psychol Inq. 2015;26(1):72–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Kashdan TB, Rottenberg J. Psychological flexibility as a fundamental aspect of health. Clinical Psychology Review. Volume 30. Elsevier Inc.; 2010. pp. 865–78.

  18. Koval P, Pe ML, Meers K, Kuppens P. Affect dynamics in relation to depressive symptoms: variable, unstable or inert? Emotion. 2013;13(6):1132–41.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Houben M, Van Den Noortgate W, Kuppens P. The relation between short-term emotion dynamics and psychological well-being: a meta-analysis. Psychol Bull. 2015;141(4):901–30.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Kuppens P, Sheeber LB, Yap MBH, Whittle S, Simmons JG, Allen NB. Emotional inertia prospectively predicts the onset of depressive disorder in adolescence. Emotion. 2012;12(2):283–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Koval P, Kuppens P. Changing emotion dynamics: individual differences in the effect of anticipatory social stress on emotional inertia. Emotion. 2012;12(2):256–67.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Fairbairn CE, Sayette MA. The effect of alcohol on emotional inertia: a test of alcohol myopia. J Abnorm Psychol. 2013;122(3):770–81.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Luginbuehl T, Schoebi D. Emotion Dynamics and responsiveness in intimate relationships. Emotion. 2020;20(2):133–48.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Mcneil S. The impact of attachment insecurity on emotion regulation. J New Zeal Coll Clin Psychol. 2012;22(2):17–9.

    Google Scholar 

  25. Hazan C, Shaver P. Romantic Love Conceptualized as an Attachment Process. J Pers Soc Psychol [Internet]. 1987;52(3):511–24. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.52.3.511.

  26. Bowlby J. Attachment and loss, Vol. 1: attachment. Attachment and loss. Basic Books.; 1969.

  27. Florian V, Mikulincer M, Bucholtz I. Effects of adult attachment style on the perception and search for Social Support. J Psychol. 1995;129(6):665–76.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Mikulincer M, Florian V. Are emotional and instrumental supportive interactions beneficial in times of stress? The impact of attachment style. Anxiety Stress Coping. 1997;10(2):109–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Mikulincer M. Adult attachment style and individual differences in functional Versus dysfunctional experiences of anger. Volume 74. Psychological Association, Inc; 1998.

  30. Simpson JA. Influence of Attachment Styles on Romantic Relationships. J Pers Soc Psychol [Internet]. 1990;59(5):971–80. https://api.semanticscholar.org/CorpusID:144694374.

  31. Cassidy J, Berlin LJ. The Insecure/Ambivalent Pattern of Attachment: Theory and Research. Child Dev [Internet]. 1994;65(4):971–81. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131298.

  32. Campbell L, Simpson JA, Boldry J, Kashy DA. Perceptions of conflict and support in romantic relationships: the role of attachment anxiety. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2005;88(3):510–31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Brodie ZP, Goodall K, Darling S, McVittie C. Attachment insecurity and dispositional aggression: the mediating role of maladaptive anger regulation. J Soc Pers Relat. 2019;36(6):1831–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Diamond LM, Hicks AM. Attachment style, current relationship security, and negative emotions: the mediating role of physiological regulation. 22, Journal of Social and Personal Relationships. 2005. p. 499–518.

  35. Gillespie SM, Beech AR. Theories of emotion regulation. The Wiley Handbook on the theories, Assessment and treatment of sexual Offending. John Wiley & Sons Singapore Pte. Ltd.; 2016. pp. 245–63.

  36. Feeney JA, Karantzas GC. Couple conflict: insights from an attachment perspective. Curr Opin Psychol. 2017;13:60–4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Barrett LF, Robin L, Pietromonaco PR, Eyssell KM. Are women the`More emotional’’ sex? Evidence from emotional experiences in Social Context. Cogn Emot. 1998;12(4):555–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Tidwell MO, Reis HT, Shaver PR. Attached SpringerReference. 2011;71(4):729–45.

    Google Scholar 

  39. Gentzler AL, Kerns KA, Keener E. Emotional reactions and regulatory responses to negative and positive events: associations with attachment and gender. Motiv Emot. 2010;34(1):78–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Powers SI, Pietromonaco PR, Gunlicks M, Sayer A. Dating couples’ attachment styles and patterns of cortisol reactivity and recovery in response to a relationship conflict. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2006;90(4):613–28.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Belsky J, Rovine M. Patterns of Marital Change across the transition to parenthood: pregnancy to three years Postpartum. J Marriage Fam. 1990;52(1):5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Goodman JH. Paternal postpartum depression, its relationship to maternal postpartum depression, and implications for family health. J Adv Nurs. 2004;45(1):26–35.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Kluwer ES, Johnson MD. Conflict frequency and relationship quality across the transition to parenthood. J Marriage Fam. 2007;69(5):1089–106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Doss BD, Rhoades GK, Stanley SM, Markman HJ. The Effect of the transition to parenthood on Relationship Quality: an 8-Year prospective study. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2009;96(3):601–19.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  45. Cowan CP, Cowan PA. When partners become parents: the big life change for couples. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.; 2000.

  46. Reid KM, Taylor MG. Social support, stress, and maternal postpartum depression: A comparison of supportive relationships. Soc Sci Res [Internet]. 2015;54:246–62. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ssresearch.2015.08.009.

  47. Mitnick DM, Heyman RE, Smith Slep AM. Changes in relationship satisfaction across the transition to parenthood: a meta-analysis. J Fam Psychol. 2009;23(6):848.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. Bowlby J. A secure base. Basic Books.; 1988.

  49. Feeney JA, Alexander R, Noller P, Hohaus L. Attachment insecurity, depression, and the transition to parenthood. Pers Relatsh. 2003;10(4):475–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Simpson JA, Rholes WS, Campbell L, Tran S, Wilson CL. Adult attachment, the transition to Parenthood, and depressive symptoms. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2003;84(6):1172–87.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Kazmierczak M. Couple empathy– the mediator of attachment styles for partners adjusting to parenthood. J Reprod Infant Psychol. 2015;33(1):15–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Trillingsgaard T, Elklit A, Shevlin M, Maimburg RD. Adult attachment at the transition to motherhood: Predicting worry, health care utility and relationship functioning. J Reprod Infant Psychol. 2011;29(4):354–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Cassidy J. Emotion Regulation: Influences of Attachment Relationships. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev [Internet]. 1994;59(2–3):228–83. https://doi.org/10.2307/1166148.

  54. Watson D, Tellegen A. Toward a consensual structure of Mood. Psychol Bull. 1985;98(2):219–35.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Reynolds S, Searight HR, Ratwik S. Adult attachment styles and rumination in the context of intimate relationships. N Am J Psychol. 2014;16(3):495–506.

    Google Scholar 

  56. Meuwly N, Davila J. Feeling bad when your partner is away: the role of dysfunctional cognition and affect regulation strategies in insecurely attached individuals. J Soc Pers Relat. 2019;36(1):22–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Brose A, Schmiedek F, Koval P, Kuppens P. Emotional inertia contributes to depressive symptoms beyond perseverative thinking. Cogn Emot. 2015;29(3):527–38.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Pascuzzo K, Moss E, Cyr C. Attachment and emotion regulation strategies in Predicting Adult Psychopathology. SAGE Open. 2015;5(3).

  59. King DB, DeLongis A. When couples disconnect: rumination and withdrawal as maladaptive responses to everyday stress. J Fam Psychol. 2014;28(4):460–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Caldwell W, da Estrela C, MacNeil S, Gouin JP. Association between romantic partners’ rumination and couples’ conflict is moderated by respiratory sinus arrhythmia. J Fam Psychol. 2019;33(6):640–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Szasz PL, Szentagotai A, Hofmann SG. The effect of emotion regulation strategies on anger. Behav Res Ther [Internet]. 2011;49(2):114–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2010.11.011.

  62. Butler EA, Gross JJ, Philippot P, Feldman RS. Hiding feelings in social contexts: Out of sight is not out of mind. In: The regulation of emotion. 2004. pp. 101–26.

  63. Koval P, Butler EA, Hollenstein T, Lanteigne D, Kuppens P. Emotion regulation and the temporal dynamics of emotions: Effects of cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression on emotional inertia. Cogn Emot [Internet]. 2015;29(5):831–51. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2014.948388.

  64. Mikulincer M, Florian V. The relationship between adult attachment styles and emotional and cognitive reactions to stressful events. In: Simpson JA, Rholes WS, editors. Attachment theory and close relationships. The Guilford.; 1998. pp. 143–65.

  65. Bowlby J. A secure base: parent-child attachment and healthy human development. Basic Books.; 1988.

  66. Simpson JA, Rholes WS. Adult attachment orientations and well-being during the transition to parenthood. Curr Opin Psychol [Internet]. 2019;25:47–52. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.02.019.

  67. Lawrence E, Nylen K, Cobb RJ. Prenatal expectations and marital satisfaction over the transition to parenthood. J Fam Psychol. 2007;21(2):155–64.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Kluwer ES. From Partnership to Parenthood: a review of Marital Change across the transition to parenthood. J Fam Theory Rev. 2010;2(2):105–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  69. Bolger N, Davis A, Rafaeli E. Diary methods: capturing life as it is lived. Annu Rev Psychol. 2003;54(1):579–616.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Fraley RC, Heffernan ME, Vicary AM, Brumbaugh CC. The experiences in Close relationships-Relationship structures Questionnaire: a method for assessing attachment orientations across relationships. Psychol Assess. 2011;23(3):615–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Favez N, Tissot H, Ghisletta P, Golay P, Notari SC. Validation of the French version of the experiences in close relationships- revised (ECR-R) adult romantic attachment questionnaire. Swiss J Psychol. 2016;75(3):113–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  72. Neumann E, Rohmann E, Bierhoff HW. Entwicklung Und Validierung Von Skalen Zur Erfassung Von Vermeidung Und Angst in Partnerschaften Der Bochumer Bindungsfragebogen (BoBi). Diagnostica. 2007;53(1):33–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  73. Ebner-Priemer UW, Kuo J, Kleindienst N, Welch SS, Reisch T, Reinhard I, et al. State affective instability in borderline personality disorder assessed by ambulatory monitoring. Psychol Med. 2007;37(7):961–70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Kenny DA, Cook W. Partner Effects in Relationship Research: Conceptual Issues, Analytic Difficulties, and Illustrations. Pers Relatsh [Internet]. 1999;6:433–48. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-6811.1999.tb00202.x.

  75. Pinheiro J, Bates D, DebRoy S, Sarkar D, Team RC. nlme: Linear and Nonlinear Mixed Eeffects Models [Internet]. 2018. https://cran.r-project.org/package=nlme.

  76. RStudio Team. R Studio: Integrated Development for R. [Internet]. Boston, MA: RStudio, PBC. 2020. http://www.rstudio.com/.

  77. Preacher KJ, Curran PJ, Bauer DJ. Computational tools for probing interactions in multiple Linear regression, Multilevel modeling, and latent curve analysis. J Educ Behav Stat. 2006;31(4):437–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  78. Mikulincer M, Shaver PR. Attachment in adulthood: structure, dynamics, and change. The Guilford; 2010.

  79. Bowlby J. Attachment and loss. Vol. 3: loss, sadness and depression. Basic Books.; 1980.

  80. Consedine NS, Tuck NL, Fiori KL. Attachment and Health Care utilization among Middle-aged and older African-Descent men: Dismissiveness predicts less frequent digital rectal examination and prostate-specific Antigen Screening. Am J Mens Health. 2013;7(5):382–93.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  81. Mikulincer M, Dolev T, Shaver PR. Attachment-related strategies during thought suppression: ironic rebounds and vulnerable self-representations. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2004;87(6):940–56.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  82. Kobak RR, Sceery A. Attachment in Late Adolescence: Working Models, Affect Regulation, and Representations of Self and Others. Child Dev [Internet]. 1988;59(1):135–46. https://doi.org/10.2307/1130395.

  83. Mikulincer M, Shaver PR. The attachment behavioral system in Adulthood: activation, psychodynamics, and interpersonal processes. Adv Exp Soc Psychol. 2003;35:53–152.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  84. Shaver PR, Mikulincer M. Attachment-related psychodynamics. Attach Hum Dev. 2002;4(2):133–61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  85. Mikulincer M. Attachment Working models and the sense of Trust: An Exploration of Interaction goals and affect regulation. 74, J Personal Soc Psychol. 1998.

  86. Fraley RC, Shaver PR. Adult attachment and the suppression of unwanted thoughts. J Pers Soc Psychol [Internet]. 1997;73(5):1080–91. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.73.5.1080.

  87. Wearden A, Cook L, Vaughan-Jones J. Adult attachment, alexithymia, symptom reporting, and health-related coping. J Psychosom Res. 2003;55(4):341–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  88. Shields AM, Cicchetti D, Ryan RM. The development of emotional and behavioral self-regulation and social competence among maltreated school-age children. Dev Psychopathol. 1994;6(1):57–75.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  89. Cassidy J, Kobak RR. Avoidance and its relation to other defensive processes. In: Belsky J, Nezworski T, editors. Clinical implications of attachment. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.; 1988. pp. 300–23.

  90. Mikulincer M, Shaver PR. Attachment theory and intergroup bias: evidence that priming the secure base schema attenuates negative reactions to out-groups. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2001;81(1):97–115.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  91. Simpson JA, Rholes WS, Nelligan JS. Support seeking and support giving within couples in an anxiety-provoking Situation: the role of attachment styles. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1992;62(3):434–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  92. Tran SS, Simpson JA. Prorelationship maintenance behaviors: the Joint roles of attachment and commitment. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2009;97(4):685–98.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  93. Campbell L, Simpson JA, Kashy DA, Rholes WS. Attachment orientations, dependence, and behavior in a stressful situation: An application of the Actor-Partner-Interdependence Model. J Soc Pers Relat [Internet]. 2001;18(6):841–3. https://doi.org/10.1177/02654075011860.

  94. Simpson JA, Overall NC. Partner Buffering of attachment insecurity. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2014;23(1):54–9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  95. Scherer KR. The dynamic architecture of emotion: evidence for the component process model. Cogn Emot. 2009;23(7):1307–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

The data is based on a project which was funded 100% by the Swiss National Science Foundation (SNSF) to Professor Dominik Schöbi.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

DS is the head of the bigger project, where from data was extracted and used for analyses of this manuscript. DS is the supervisor of this paper which is part of a doctoral thesis. DS also contributed with capturing the idea and the main focus of this paper, improving it, and offering insights about the statistical analyses. NM contributed with a substantial review of the manuscript and provided ideas regarding the structure and the emphasis of important elements and ideas throughout the whole manuscript. MR is the collaborator of the bigger project where from data was collected and used for this manuscript. MR contributed in data collection and statistical analyses. MR also provided a critical review of the paper. GK is the first author of this manuscript. GK captured the idea, worked on the collection of data, did the analyses, and wrote the manuscript. All authors have read, reviewed and approved the manuscript.

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Georgia Kouri or Dominik Schoebi.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The project was approved from the swiss ethics review board of the regional government (Canton Vaud). The project started in 2018 with the ID: 2018 − 00372, under the name: ‘Affect regulation, stress response and responsiveness– the impact of challenging conditions on intimate relationships and well-being’. This research project was carried out in accordance to the research plan and with principles enunciated in the current version of the Declaration of Helsinki (DoH), the Essentials of Good Epidemiological Practice issued by Public Health Schweiz (EGEP), the Swiss Law and Swiss regulatory authority’s requirements as applicable. All participants read, completed and signed the informed consent form before participating in the study.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Kouri, G., Meuwly, N., Richter, M. et al. Attachment insecurities, emotion dynamics and stress in intimate relationships during the transition to parenthood. BMC Psychol 12, 200 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-024-01686-w

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-024-01686-w

Keywords