Skip to main content

Early maladaptive schema, attachment style, and parenting style in a clinical population with personality disorder and normal individuals: a discriminant analysis model

Abstract

Introduction

Researchers have shown various variables’ role in forming personality disorders (PD). This study aimed to assess the role of early maladaptive schema (EMS), attachment style (AS), and parenting style (PS) in discriminating between personality disorders and normal individuals.

Methods

In this study, 78 personality disorder patients and 360 healthy volunteers aged 18–84 were selected using convenience sampling. They completed the Schema Questionnaire-Short Form (SQ-SF), Revised Adult Attachment Scale (RAAS), and Baumrind’s Parenting Styles Questionnaire (PSI). Data were analyzed using discriminant analysis with IBM SPSS 25.

Results

The results showed higher mean scores in all early maladaptive schema domains, insecure attachment styles, and authoritarian parenting in the personality disorder group than in the normal group. Also, discriminant analyses revealed that the function was statistically significant and could distinguish between the two groups and a compound of essential variables, disconnection, impaired autonomy, and secure attachment, respectively, discriminating two groups. Given that all components were able to distinguish between the two groups.

Conclusion

Therefore, intervention based on these factors early in life may help reduce the characteristics of personality disorders. Also, considering the role of these factors, treatment protocols can be prepared.

Peer Review reports

Introduction

The latest version of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5), which was published in 2015 [1], defines personality disorder (PD) as a “persistent pattern of behaviors and inner experiences that deviate from expectations of the individuals’ culture standards, is inflexible, begins in adolescence or youth, and is fixed over time, lead to dysfunction or functional impairment and is shown in at least 2 of the four domains of cognition, emotion, interpersonal relations or impulse control” [2]. Since the third published edition of DSM, PD has separated from axis one disorder and put onto a separate axis, bringing this area of psychopathology into the researchers’ center of attention [3].

Studies on the prevalence rates of PDs indicate these disorders are common between 9 and 13% in the nonclinical community [4] and 50% in the clinical sample [5]. Prevalence estimates indicate that personality pathology is widespread and has caused public health concerns [3]. Clinical history shows PDs are associated with significant functional impairment [6], considerable risk factors for suicidal behavior [7,8,9], and are comorbid with a wide range of other psychiatric disorders [7, 10, 11], and social deviation (e.g., crime, addiction) [12, 13].

Accordingly, understanding “How” and “Why” PDs are developed is essential. So far, several studies have been conducted on the description, explanation, and etiology of PDs. There is broad disagreement among scientists about the causes of PDs, but generally, the risk factors for these disorders are multifaceted and influenced by biological, psychological, and social factors [14,15,16].

Twin and adoption studies have revealed that PDs have a hereditary component; Fontaine and Viding (2008) estimated heritability to be between 30 and 80%, while Buffie (2017) suggested it to be 58% [17, 18]. The remaining unexplained percentage is attributed to environmental influences [19] and adverse childhood experiences [20, 21].

Personality generally arises from the interplay of genetic and environmental factors. Differences in individual traits, characterized under the umbrella term “temperament,” are closely tied to biological processes. These traits, some of which are hereditary, can be observed from early childhood [22, 23]. However, the development of these traits is influenced by both the environment and heredity: children’s experiences shape their character traits, while their traits, in turn, affect their experiences. This ongoing interaction mutually affects psychological processes such as emotional expression, empathy, social behavior, impulse control, and risk-taking [24].

On the other hand, the development of these personality traits is a two-way process: children’s experiences affect the development of their traits, while their traits, in turn, affect the type of experiences they have, and this interaction exists over time [25]. These interactions apply to various psychological processes, such as emotional expression, empathy, attributional style, sociability, impulse control, and risk-taking [24].

Studies investigating the causes of PDs have affirmed the role of genetics [15, 26], early maladaptive schemas (EMS) [27,28,29,30,31,32], attachment style (AS) [33,34,35,36], and parenting style (PS) [37,38,39,40,41].

Early maladaptive schema (EMS)

Young (1990, 1999) and others hypothesized that these self-defeating emotional and cognitive information-processing patterns might be at the core of many PDs [42]. According to Young, primary maladaptive schemas are “an overarching theme or pattern consisting of memories, emotions, cognitions, bodily sensations, and strong and inflexible beliefs about oneself, the world, and one’s relationships with others that are seen as symptoms in personality disorders.” which the American Psychiatric Association has described [28, 42].

This theory suggests negative/traumatic childhood or adolescence experiences are the primary source of these dysfunctional patterns, perpetuated throughout the lifespan, and generate self-defeating behavioral patterns [43]. The activation of primary maladaptive schemas causes intense emotions and frustrations, which show that their basic emotional needs are not satisfied, and the symptoms observed in personality disorders are the methods that people use to cope with this issue [28]. EMSs are assumed to be highly stable and resistant to change, acting like templates to assign repetitive meanings to the individual’s experiences and guide their thoughts, perceptions, emotions, and behavior [31]. Findings indicate EMSs are dysfunctional to a significant degree [42] and lead to emotional, interpersonal, and professional relationship confusion and dissatisfaction, psychological distress, and PDs [42, 44].

EMSs are believed to underlie, perpetuate, and maintain characteristics commonly seen in PDs. So far, clinical and nonclinical studies have investigated the relationship between EMSs and PDs. These research studies have generally indicated that EMSs contributed significantly to the prediction of PDs [28, 45], there are significantly higher levels of EMSs in PDs in comparison to the control group and other patients [27, 30, 46], and some EMSs predict certain PDs [28,29,30,31, 47, 48]. Also, some of these research studies found a relationship between the PD and EMS domain [27, 31, 47, 49]. For example [47] in their research indicated some domains of EMSs relate to a broader vulnerability factor for PDs (i.e., disconnection and rejection for both borderline and avoidant PDs), and some domains differentially relate to the specific PDs (i.e., other-directedness domain for dependent personality disorder and over vigilance for obsessive-compulsive personality disorder). Narcissistic personality disorder was positively associated with impaired limits [31, 49], disconnection/rejection, vigilance, and inhibition [31] and negatively associated with the other-directedness domain. Paranoid personality disorder was associated with the disconnection and rejection and the impaired autonomy and performance domain, and borderline and antisocial personality disorder were associated with the disconnection and rejection and the impaired limits domain [49]. Antisocial personality disorder was related to three EMS domains: impaired limits, impaired autonomy/performance and disconnection/rejection, and borderline personality disorder was related to all domains except other-directed [27].

Attachment

Another theory that has been described as a conceptual framework for understanding PDs is attachment theory (AS) [34]. This theory was formulated by British psychoanalyst John Bowlby (1969, 1973, 1980) and American-Canadian developmental psychologist Mary Ainsworth (1991). This theory explains how the emotional bonds one forms with primary caregivers during infancy have lasting effects and extend into adulthood, serving as enduring templates to determine their characteristic ways of relating to others [35, 50, 51]. In other words, the attachment theory suggests that how people relate to others and respond to intimacy throughout life are learned during infancy through interactions with primary caregivers or “attachment figures.” [34, 35].

Bowlby (1969–1999) claimed infants internalize their interactions with primary caregivers as a cognitive framework that comprises mental representations of the world, self, and others. Such representations are called “internal working models.” They are hypothesized to shape the development of ASs [33] and to establish a base for personality development, identity formation, subsequent intimate relationships [35, 52], expectations about interpersonal relations, social acceptance, as well as attitudes to rejection, strategies for coping with distress, and mental health throughout life [34, 35, 52].

Attachment theory has proven to be a fruitful framework for predicting vulnerability to psychopathology. Bowlby (1973, 1980) postulated that the development of the negative representation of self or others during the early years of life can be related to the subsequent development of psychopathology [53]. Many personality theorists have considered attachment theory as a framework for understanding the development, maintenance, and treatment of the interpersonal difficulties and adaptations that characterize personality pathology [34, 35, 54].

PDs are characterized by distorted representations of self and others, as well as dysfunctional interpersonal relationships [36]. According to attachment theory, PDs reflect insecure internal working models that are assumed to operate in such a way as to confirm or fulfill themselves. These representational working models, in this way, make themselves rigid and inflexible and cause difficulties in social, occupational, and relational functioning [54].

Further, a large body of empirical research has examined the relationship between PDs and ASs and supported the general role of insecure attachment in forming PDs [33, 55,56,57]. Along the same lines, research studies have shown that secure attachment negatively predicts PD [58, 59].

Some research has studied the relationship between attachment and PDs across three different clusters of PDs and provided evidence of the role of attachment in Cluster B PDs [56, 60]. Cascio and Alaimo indicated that preoccupied attachment is a specific feature of patients’ Cluster C PDs, and Cluster A and B disorders were associated with fearful avoidant attachment [61].

Parenting style (PS)

The third variable hypothesized in this study to act as a factor influencing the development of PDs is parenting style (PS). Many researchers consider the interactions between the individual and their environment as the most critical factor in the development of PDs [62, 63]. Prominent research and theory have considered family processes as significant sources of socialization for children and the development of personality [64, 65]. Along the same lines, research indicates children brought up by warm parents tend to show better social adjustment [66].

As dysfunctional interpersonal relationships comprise a core feature of PDs [39], it’s probably safe to argue that socialization deficits observed among individuals with PD might partly emerge from problematic parenting [64]. One theory to conceptualize the impact of early socialization on the development of PDs has been Diana Baumrind’s PSs (1967, 1978, 1991). She conceptualized PSs along two dimensions: parental demand (e.g., control) and parental response (e.g., warmth). Accordingly, Baumrind defined three PSs: authoritative (high demand and high responsiveness), authoritarian (high demand and low responsiveness), and permissive (low demand and high responsiveness) [67].

Research on PSs suggested lack of parental care, overprotection, or both in childhood resulted in a change in normal personality, such as higher neuroticism, self-criticism, and perfectionism [68].

Many studies have found associations between parenting and PDs [37, 41, 64, 69,70,71,72]. For example, Cheng et al. [41] showed parental rejection and over-protection were linked to the higher occurrence of PD, while emotional warmth was negatively related to PD. In addition, it has been concluded that lower levels of care and higher levels of overprotection characterize patients with PDs. Similarly, personality-disordered patients perceived less parental care and more paternal freedom control and autonomy denial than normal adolescents and adults [40, 41].

Nordahl et al. [30] declared patients with antisocial, schizoid, and schizotypal PDs did not show any significant association with any of the EMSs. Also, in this study, the scores of enmeshment/underdeveloped self-schemas, emotional deprivation, and entitlement did not differentiate between patients with PD and without a diagnosis of PD.

Research also indicates a significant relationship between AS, EMS, and perceived PS. The interaction between a child’s unique nature, temperament, and negative experiences like maladaptive PSs contributes to the development and persistence of maladaptive schemas [42, 71, 73]. Studies have shown that negative parenting practices from both parents are linked to stronger levels of schema domains [74, 75]. ASs are also shaped by genetic factors, a child’s temperament, and attachment experiences [76, 77]. Some studies have found a positive relationship between authoritative parenting and secure attachment and negligent and authoritarian parenting, which predicted avoidant attachment [78]. The findings of a meta-analysis indicated a strong positive association between insecure attachment styles (anxious and avoidant) and primary maladaptive schemas. In contrast, secure attachment negatively correlates with primary maladaptive schemas [79].

In this study investigating the origins of PDs, the focus was on three key factors: schema, attachment, and parenting. Based on previous research, these variables have shown significant relevance to understanding PDs. However, despite this background, it remains unclear which factors significantly influence the development of these disorders. Therefore, the current study aimed to examine all three variables simultaneously and determine their respective contributions in distinguishing individuals with PDs from those without.

One of the innovative aspects of this study is looking into how the linear relationships between these constructs could be used to discriminate the normal (NR) group from the disordered group. In light of the data declared in the current study, this research aims to (1) Determine whether PDs can be predicted based on EMSs, AS, and perceived PS; (2) Can variables differentiate between the NR and PD groups? and (3) Another purpose of this study was to determine which component or components could demonstrate such distinction and estimate each variable’s share in the difference between the two groups.

Materials and methods

Participants and procedure

The study included two groups: normal and clinical. The clinical group included outpatients seeking treatment at an Iranian Specialized Psychiatry clinic in Tehran and Alborz provinces. Researchers visited three psychiatric clinics in person to collect samples from individuals with PDs. They talked to individuals diagnosed with PD by a psychiatrist, and after giving informed consent to all patients, they filled out a paper and online questionnaire. Inclusion criteria were (a) a primary diagnosis of PDs by a clinical psychologist and psychiatrist, (b) older than 18 and younger than 84 years old, and (c) a level of education of at least middle school. The final sample comprises 78 outpatients with PDs (Female N = 30, 61.5%; with mean age = 32; Male N = 38, 38.5%; with mean age = 32). Patients were diagnosed with PD (clusters A (N = 19), B (N = 17), and C (N = 42)) according to the (DSM-5, American Psychiatric Association, 2013).

In discriminant analysis, the smallest group should have more individuals than the number of predictor variables [80]. This study’s smallest group consists of individuals with PDs, totaling 78 participants, while we had 11 predictor variables. Hence, the number of individuals in the personality-disordered group was five times greater than the total number of variables, confirming the hypothesis.

In this study normal population, including 422 subjects, was selected by convenience sampling. The examiner evaluated them after screening with SCID, ensuring that all entry criteria were met and conducting thorough interviews with the participants. As a result, 62 subjects were excluded due to not meeting the entry criteria. Ultimately, data from 360 normal subjects were analyzed. Participants who were willing to participate received and filled out the questionnaire online. Inclusion criteria were (a) no PD and other psychiatric disorders, (b) older than 18 and younger than 84, and (c) level of education of at least middle school. The final sample comprised 360 (Female N = 210, 58.3%; with mean age = 29; Male N = 150, 41.7%; with mean age = 30). The gender frequency difference between the two groups was evaluated, and no significant difference was observed in terms of this variable between the two groups (X2 = 0.272, df = 1, P = 0.602).

Instrument and questionnaire

The Schema Questionnaire-Short Form (YSQ-SF)

It is a 75-item self-report assessing 15 different EMSs that are clustered in five domains, including (a) disconnection and rejection, (b) impaired autonomy and performance, (c) impaired limits, (d) other-directedness, and (e) over-vigilance and inhibition. Each item is rated on a 6-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (Absolutely wrong) to 6 (Absolutely true). The average score of each schema is calculated by summing the outcomes of all related items and then dividing this by the total number of questions. Evidence supports this instrument’s reliability, validity, and factor structure of the SQ-SF [81, 82]. The Persian version of the YSQ-SF has demonstrated adequate validity and reliability in Iranian samples [83, 84]. For all subscales, Cronbach’s αs for the YSQ‐SF subscales were estimated between 0.69 and 0.90 [83].

Revised Adult Attachment Scale (RAAS)

It is an 18-item self-report instrument that includes three subscales (each with six items): (a) comfort with closeness, (b) comfort with depending on others, and (c) anxious concern about abandonment. According to these subscales, three AS of secure, anxious, and avoidant are identified. Each item is rated on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (I completely agree) to 5 (I completely disagree). Internal consistency reliability, α coefficient, and retest reliability after a 2-month interval were 0.68, 0.71, and 0.52 for close, dependency, and anxiety subscales, respectively [85, 86]. Collins and Read (1996) reported adequate internal consistency. Cronbach’s alphas for the close, depend, and anxiety subscales were 0.77, 0.78, and 0.85, respectively. Research on the Iranian population indicates a high validity of 0.95 for this variable. Moreover, Cronbach’s alpha for this scale is reported to be 0.8, further underlining its high-reliability coefficient [87].

PS Inventory (PSI)

This questionnaire was designed by Diana Baumrind in 1972, and it includes 30 items that evaluate three PSs: authoritative, authoritarian, and permissive styles. The question’s response pattern follows a 5-point Likert scale from “Absolutely opposed” to “Absolutely agree.” The reliability of this questionnaire, by test-retest method, is 0.81 for permissive, 0.92 for authoritarian, and 0.92 for authoritative PS [88]. In evaluating the validity of this instrument, the relationship between permissive and authoritarian has been reported as -0.50, and between authoritative and authoritarian, -0.52 [89]. In Iran, Esfandiari (1995) has declared the reliability of subscales by test-retest, 0.69 for permissive style, 0.77 for authoritarian, and 0.73 for authoritative [90].

Statistical analyses

Participants were assessed using YSQ-SF, RAAS, and PSI. According to the nature of the variables, discriminant analysis was used to analyze the data to predict group membership in dependent variable classes from a set of independent variables.

First, the hypotheses of diagnostic analysis were examined. One of the assumptions for discriminant analysis is the lack of a multi-collinearity relationship between independent variables. The correlation between independent variables was first obtained in this study to conduct the discriminant analysis. Calculating the matrix of average correlations within groups indicated the lack of a multi-collinearity relationship. Also, log determinants and Box’s M test (F = 1.121, P = 0.235) showed that the variance/covariance matrix of the two considered groups is equal. Since all the discriminant analysis test assumptions have been confirmed, this study used the discriminant analysis test to examine the research assumptions. All statistical analyses were performed using SPSS (version 25.0).

Results

From among 438 participants (258 females and 180 males) in this research, 78 were included in the PD group (17.8%) and 360 in the NR group (82.2%). Table 1 shows the demographic characteristics of the participants in the two groups. The average age was 32 (SD = 6.9) and 29 (SD = 8) for the PD and NR groups, respectively. The PD group comprised 19 people with Cluster A (24.4%), 17 people with Cluster B (21.8%), and 42 people with Cluster C (53.8%).

Pearson correlation matrix for the relationship between schema domain, AS, and PS in PD and NR individuals has been provided in Table 2. Additionally, Table 3 presents the descriptive statistics of the subjects. According to this table, compared to the NR group, people with PDs scored higher on average in all five EMS domains: anxious AS, avoidant AS, and authoritarian PS. The average score for the NR group in secure AS, authoritative PS, and permissive PS was higher than the PD group.

The conducted discriminant analysis was a two-group analysis, so one discriminant function was performed (Table 4). The discriminant function was significant as Wilks` lambda was 0.860 (P < 0.001). This value indicated the existence of differences between groups (Table 5).

Table 6 shows standardized coefficients and structure matrix. Canonical discriminant function coefficients were used to evaluate the independent variable’s unique contribution to the discriminant function (Fig. 1). The canonical structure matrix revealed the correlations between each independent variable and the discriminant functions. It allowed us to compare correlations and see how closely a variable was related to each function [91].

Structure matrix coefficients value showed PD was mainly determined by positive relation discriminant functions with disconnection/rejection domain (0.844), impaired autonomy/performance (0.795) schema domain, and negative relation discriminant functions with secure AS (-0.651), respectively (Table 6). The classification result clearly showed how constituting the sample was distributed across groups. According to the results, 261 individuals (72.5%) of the NR group were classified correctly. Also, 53 individuals (67.9%) in the PD group were correctly classified. A total of 71.7% of individuals were distributed in line with the expected classification (Table 7).

Table 1 Demographic characteristics of participants
Table 2 Pearson correlation matrix for the relationship between schema domain, AS, and PS in PD and NR groups
Table 3 Descriptive statistics (Mean and Standard Deviation) for NR and PD groups
Table 4 Tests of equality of group mean
Table 5 Wilks’ lambda summary of canonical discriminant functions
Table 6 Structure matrix and canonical discriminant function coefficients
Table 7 Classification Resultsa,c
Fig. 1
figure 1

The plot of the discriminant function for NR and PD groups

Discussion

In response to the questions raised in this study, the results showed that the EMS, AS, and PS variables could predict personality disorders. These variables can be used to identify individuals at risk for developing PDs. Furthermore, the variables were also found to differentiate between the NR group and those with PDs, so they can be used to distinguish between individuals with a normal personality and those with a PD. Additionally, the study found that specific components contributed more to differentiating between the two groups. The components of schema domains involving disconnection/rejection, impaired autonomy/performance, and secure attachment style were found to have the highest contribution in creating this distinction.

The results of this study indicated ASs were capable of discriminating between NR and PD groups. Secure attachment, insecure-anxious attachment, and insecure-avoidant ASs made the highest distinction between the two groups, respectively. In this research, consistent with the findings of some other studies, secure attachment negatively predicted PD [58, 59]. Moreover, the PD group differed from the NR group concerning anxious and avoidant attachments. In the current study, as the other studies declared, they played a more significant role in discriminating between the two groups [58, 92].

Another finding revealed ASs could predict PDs, with secure attachment being a negative predictor and anxious and avoidant insecure attachments being a positive predictor for PDs, in which some studies obtained the same results [33, 34, 57].

PDs are characterized by enduring maladaptive patterns of behavior, which include interpersonal (disturbances in arousal regulation or impulse and emotion control), intrapersonal (incompetent relationship patterns), and social components (which create conflict with others and the social institutions) [33]. The attachment theory could explain such characteristics of PDs. Each PD has a distinctive and incompetent intrapersonal style that is usually the characteristic feature of the disorder. There is also a series of experimental supports for the hypothesis that PD is a disorder in intrapersonal relationships [34, 54]. A study by Meyer, et al. has found that dysfunctional intrapersonal behaviors make up 45% of diagnostic criteria for the PDs in DSM-IV, outstripping the remaining criteria [dysfunctional cognition (23%), emotional disorders (20%), and the other behaviors (12%)] [59]. The formation of secure attachment, a main growth stage, is influenced by how the children interact with their caregivers. Since the mental representations of the type of primary relationships extend to an individual’s future and affect their relationships, it may explain how an insecure attachment damages the intrapersonal relationships of an individual with PD.

The findings indicate that PSs can discriminate between the NR and PD groups. In this study, the authoritarian PS positively correlated with the PDs, whereas the authoritative PS (characterized by high responsiveness, high care, and high demands) negatively correlated with the PDs. The results are consistent with the studies reporting that patients with PD receive lower parental care but higher freedom, control, and autonomy denial (the characteristics of authoritarian parenting) than normal adolescents and adults [55,56,57]. As mentioned before, one of the main features of PDs is a significant problem in intrapersonal relationships. How a child behaves generally depends on the PSs that the parents adopted. A good relationship with parents would help the children achieve social and emotional adjustment during growth [64, 65].

Furthermore, the parents affect the socialization process and impact various components of personality, such as children’s cognition, emotion, and behavior [93]. People with PD lack at least two of the four areas of intrapersonal relationship: cognition, emotion, and impulse control [5]. Our hypothesis was confirmed by the studies that have indicated that problematic parenting increases the risk of developing PD [41, 64, 69, 71, 94].

Apart from authoritative parenting, permissive parenting also negatively correlated with the PD group in this study. The permissive PS, characterized by a high level of care, may serve as a protective factor against such PD traits. Limited research has explored the association between the perceived parenting style within the PD group and how it differs from the NR group. However, the findings have shown that A review of the conclusions of research such as Henschel, Claudio, and Kılıçkaya on the effect of permissive PS on the development of PDs revealed that narcissistic PD is related to permissive parenting [95,96,97]. Moreover, Morrison’s study showed that PSs that lack boundaries are over-indulgent or inconsistent and may predispose children to develop histrionic PD [98], which resembles permissive PS. The results of research on permissive PS require further analysis.

Among the ten specific PDs, the schizoid, schizotypal, paranoid, borderline, avoidant, and obsessive-compulsive disorders are characterized by features that indicate a record of parental neglect and lack of adequate care for children [20, 99,100,101].

The findings indicate distinctions between the PD and the NR groups in the disconnection and rejection, impaired autonomy and performance, impaired limits, over-vigilance and inhibition, and other-directedness domains, respectively.

In this study, the scores of EMSs in PD individuals were higher than in the NR group, and the finding of this study was supported by the studies that show some early maladaptive schemas were predictors of PDs [27,28,29,30,31, 47, 102, 103]. People with EMSs cannot perceive and analyze the information, happenings, relationships, and their surroundings as they exist. They perceive the situation distortedly and react to it based on the pre-existing beliefs in their schemas. They also have low flexibility and cannot accept that this is, in fact, their schemas that create problems, not the people around them or their experiences. Schemas also cause problems in an individual’s cognitive and emotional aspects, feelings, identity, intrapersonal relationships, as well as occupational and social performance, which closely resemble the qualities of people with PD. The problems of people with PD could be attributed to the maladaptive schemas that they have developed.

Findings indicated all components could significantly separate the NR and PD groups. The following variables accounted for the most significant share in making a distinction among the two groups respectively: (1) Disconnection and rejection, (2) Impaired autonomy and performance, (3) Secure attachment, (4) Impaired limits, (5) Over vigilance and inhibition, (6) Anxious attachment, (7) Authoritarian parenting, (8) Other-directedness, 9. Authoritative parenting, 10. Permissive parenting, and 11) Avoidance attachment.

Disconnection and rejection include abandonment/instability, mistrust/abuse, emotional deprivation, defectiveness/shame, and social isolation/alienation schemas. Patients with these schemas cannot experience secure attachment to others and have grown up in a family characterized as rejectionist, unstable, blameful and humiliating, abusive, or isolated. These people’s basic needs for security, tranquility, acceptance, support, and sympathy have not been satisfied. Meanwhile, among the EMS, patients with schemas in this domain had traumatic childhoods and were often the most damaged. In adults, they had unhealthy relationships [42]. According to the explanations, the domain of disconnection and rejection includes schemas with features that cover some of the concepts related to insecure attachments (anxiety and avoidance) that, in adults, had unhealthy relationships.

Impaired autonomy and performance domains include dependence/incompetence, vulnerability to harm or illness, enmeshment/underdeveloped self, and failure schema. Patients with these schemas have grown up in families that have been entangled, overprotective, rigid, and lacking self-confidence, hindering their independence and readiness to live outside the family and perform well [42]. Moreover, impaired autonomy and performance include characteristics that could explain problematic parenting, especially authoritarian parenting.

Limitations

Limitations of the study include different size numbers of the normal and disordered groups. Due to the challenges posed by individuals with personality disorders regarding cooperation and healthy social interaction, it becomes challenging to expand the size of the personality disorder group. This expansion requires a lengthy collection period for obtaining samples and the cooperation and support of additional treatment centers such as hospitals and psychiatric clinics. Meanwhile, a personality disorder is comorbid with other psychiatric disorders. The relation between these variables should be studied through a modeling research method.

Conclusions

The current study declared that, compared to normal individuals, PD patients had a higher level of EMS domain (e.g., disconnection and rejection and impaired autonomy), insecure attachment, and authoritarian perceived PS. The highest level of distinction was caused by disconnection and rejection domain schema, impaired autonomy and performance, secure attachment, impaired limits domain, over vigilance and inhibition, anxious attachment, authoritative parenting, other-directedness domain schema, authoritarian parenting, permissive parenting, and avoidant attachment, respectively. So, the result suggests that special attention should be paid to these factors in the treatment and psychopathology of PD.

Data availability

The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding authors upon reasonable request.

References

  1. Zachar P, Krueger R, Kendler K. Personality disorder in DSM-5: an oral history. Psychol Med. 2016;46(1):1–10.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Association AP. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5®). American Psychiatric Pub; 2013.

  3. Lenzenweger MF, Clarkin JF. The Personality Disorders: History, Classification, and Research Issues. 2005.

  4. Lenzenweger MF. Epidemiology of personality disorders. Psychiatr Clin North Am. 2008;31(3):395–403.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Kaplan BJ. Kaplan and Sadock’s synopsis of psychiatry. Behavioral sciences/clinical psychiatry. Tijdschrift Voor Psychiatrie. 2016;58(1):78–9.

    Google Scholar 

  6. Skodol AE, Johnson JG, Cohen P, Sneed JR, Crawford TN. Personality disorder and impaired functioning from adolescence to adulthood. Br J Psychiatry. 2007;190(5):415–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Johnson JG, Cohen P, Skodol AE, Oldham JM, Kasen S, Brook JS. Personality disorders in adolescence and risk of major mental disorders and suicidality during adulthood. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1999;56(9):805–11.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Pompili M, Ruberto A, Girardi P, Tatarelli R. Suicidality in DSM IV Cluster B personality disorders. An overview. Annali dell’Istituto Superiore Di sanità. 2004;40(4):475–83.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Reuben J, Kannapiran DR, Ambujam DG, Gill D, Sakthi D. Personality disorders among suicide attempters. Eur J Mol Clin Med. 2020;7(6):140–69.

    Google Scholar 

  10. Hawton K, Houston K, Haw C, Townsend E, Harriss L. Comorbidity of axis I and axis II disorders in patients who attempted suicide. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160(8):1494–500.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Links PS, Eynan R. The relationship between personality disorders and Axis I psychopathology: deconstructing comorbidity. Ann Rev Clin Psychol. 2013;9:529–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Tyrer P, Reed GM, Crawford MJ. Classification, assessment, prevalence, and effect of personality disorder. Lancet. 2015;385(9969):717–26.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Walter M. Personality disorder and addiction. Co-occurring addictive and psychiatric disorders: Springer; 2015. p. 137– 48.

  14. Paris J. Personality disorders: a biopsychosocial model. J Personal Disord. 1993;7(3):255–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Torgersen S, Lygren S, Øien PA, Skre I, Onstad S, Edvardsen J, et al. A twin study of personality disorders. Compr Psychiatr. 2000;41(6):416–25.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  16. Coolidge FL, Thede LL, Jang KL. Heritability of personality disorders in childhood: a preliminary investigation. J Personal Disord. 2001;15(1):33–40.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  17. Fontaine N, Viding E. Genetics of personality disorders. Psychiatry. 2008;7(3):137–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Buffie ML. Heritability of personality traits across the Lifespan: a twin study. University of Colorado Colorado Springs; 2017.

  19. Braungart JM, Plomin R, DeFries JC, Fulker DW. Genetic influence on tester-rated infant temperament as assessed by Bayley’s infant Behavior Record: nonadoptive and adoptive siblings and twins. Dev Psychol. 1992;28(1):40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Teicher MH, Tomoda A, Andersen SL. Neurobiological consequences of early stress and childhood maltreatment: are results from human and animal studies comparable? Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2006;1071(1):313–23.

    Article  PubMed  ADS  Google Scholar 

  21. Johnson JG, Bromley E, Bornstein RF, Sneed JR. Personality disorders. Behavioral and emotional disorders in adolescents: Nature, assessment, and treatment. 2006:463– 84.

  22. Rothbart MK. Advances in temperament. Handbook of temperament. 2012:3–20.

  23. Shiner RL, Buss KA, McClowry SG, Putnam SP, Saudino KJ, Zentner M. What is temperament now? Assessing progress in temperament research on the twenty-fifth anniversary of Goldsmith et al(). Child Dev Perspect. 2012;6(4):436–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Nathan R, Wood H. Forensic Psychiatry and Forensic Psychology: Personality Disorder. 2016.

  25. Shiner RL, Caspi A. Temperament and the development of personality traits, adaptations, and narratives. 2012.

  26. Reichborn-Kjennerud T. Genetics of personality disorders. Psychiatr Clin North Am. 2008;31(3):421–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Gilbert F, Daffern M. The association between early maladaptive schema and personality disorder traits in an offender population. Psychol Crime Law. 2013;19(10):933–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Carr SN, Francis AJ. Early maladaptive schemas and personality disorder symptoms: an examination in a nonclinical sample. Psychol Psychotherapy: Theory Res Pract. 2010;83(4):333–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Reeves M, Taylor J. Specific relationships between core beliefs and personality disorder symptoms in a nonclinical sample. Clin Psychol Psychotherapy: Int J Theory Pract. 2007;14(2):96–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Nordahl HM, Holthe H, Haugum JA. Early maladaptive schemas in patients with or without personality disorders: does schema modification predict symptomatic relief? Clin Psychol Psychother. 2005;12(2):142–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Jovev M, Jackson HJ. Early maladaptive schemas in personality disordered individuals. J Personal Disord. 2004;18(5):467–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Petrocelli JV, Glaser BA, Calhoun GB, Campbell LF. Early maladaptive schemas of personality disorder subtypes. J Personal Disord. 2001;15(6):546–59.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  33. Garofalo C, Bogaerts S. Attachment and personality disorders among child molesters: the role of trust. Sex Abuse. 2019;31(1):97–124.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Levy KN, Johnson BN, Clouthier TL, Scala J, Temes CM. An attachment theoretical framework for personality disorders. Can Psychol. 2015;56(2):197.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Lorenzini N, Fonagy P. Attachment and personality disorders: a short review. Focus. 2013;11(2):155–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Sinha P, Sharan P. Attachment and personality disorders. J Indian Association Child Adolesc Mental Health. 2007;3(4):105–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Xu Y, Lin L, Yang L, Zhou L, Tao Y, Chen W, et al. Personality disorder and perceived parenting in Chinese students of divorced and intact families. Family J. 2016;24(1):70–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Steele KR, Townsend ML, Grenyer BF. Parenting and personality disorder: an overview and meta-synthesis of systematic reviews. PLoS ONE. 2019;14(10):e0223038.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Laulik S, Chou S, Browne KD, Allam J. The link between personality disorder and parenting behaviors: a systematic review. Aggress Violent Beh. 2013;18(6):644–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Yu R, Wang Z, Qian F, Jang KL, Livesley WJ, Paris J, et al. Perceived parenting styles and disordered personality traits in adolescent and adult students and in personality disorder patients. Social Behav Personality: Int J. 2007;35(5):587–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Cheng HG, Huang Y, Liu Z, Liu B. Associations linking parenting styles and offspring personality disorder are moderated by parental personality disorder, evidence from China. Psychiatry Res. 2011;189(1):105–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Young JE, Klosko JS, Weishaar ME. Schema therapy. New York: Guilford; 2003. p. 254.

    Google Scholar 

  43. Rafaeli E, Bernstein DP, Young J. Schema therapy: distinctive features. Routledge; 2010.

  44. Young JE. Cognitive therapy for personality disorders: a schema-focused approach. Professional Resource Press/Professional Resource Exchange; 1999.

  45. Thimm JC. Incremental validity of maladaptive schemas over five-factor model facets in the prediction of personality disorder symptoms. Pers Indiv Differ. 2011;50(6):777–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Khodarahimi S. Early maladaptive schemas in individuals with and without B cluster personality disorders. Curr Issues Personality Psychol. 2017;5(4):260–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Kunst H, Lobbestael J, Candel I, Batink T. Early maladaptive schemas and their relation to personality disorders: a correlational examination in a clinical population. Clin Psychol Psychother. 2020;27(6):837–46.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. Ball SA, Cecero JJ. Addicted patients with personality disorders: traits, schemas, and presenting problems. J Personal Disord. 2001;15(1):72–83.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  49. Corral C, Calvete E. Early maladaptive schemas and personality disorder traits in perpetrators of intimate partner violence. Span J Psychol. 2014;17.

  50. Levy KN, Ellison WD, Scott LN, Bernecker SL. Attachment style. J Clin Psychol. 2011;67(2):193–203.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Ainsworth MS, Bowlby J. An ethological approach to personality development. Am Psychol. 1991;46(4):333.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Voestermans D, Eikelenboom M, Rullmann J, Wolters-Geerdink M, Draijer N, Smit JH et al. The Association between Childhood Trauma and attachment functioning in patients with personality disorders. J Personal Disord. 2020:1–19.

  53. Dozier M, Stovall-McClough KC, Albus KE. Attachment and psychopathology in adulthood. 2008.

  54. Lyddon WJ, Sherry A. Developmental personality styles: an attachment theory conceptualization of personality disorders. J Couns Dev. 2001;79(4):405–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Timmerman IG, Emmelkamp PM. The relationship between attachment styles and cluster B personality disorders in prisoners and forensic inpatients. Int J Law Psychiatry. 2006;29(1):48–56.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Li X, Du T, editors. The relation between cluster B personality disorders and attachment dimensions. 2020 4th international seminar on Education, Management and Social Sciences (ISEMSS 2020). Atlantis Press; 2020.

  57. Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van IJzendoorn MH. The first 10,000 adult attachment interviews: distributions of adult attachment representations in clinical and nonclinical groups. Attach Hum Dev. 2009;11(3):223–63.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. MacDonald K, Berlow R, Thomas ML. Attachment, affective temperament, and personality disorders: a study of their relationships in psychiatric outpatients. J Affect Disord. 2013;151(3):932–41.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Meyer B, Pilkonis PA, Proietti JM, Heape CL, Egan M. Attachment styles and personality disorders as predictors of symptom course. J Personal Disord. 2001;15(5):371–89.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  60. Bender DS, Farber BA, Geller JD. Cluster B personality traits and attachment. J Am Acad Psychoanal Dynamic Psychiatry. 2001;29(4):551–63.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  61. Cascio M, Alaimo S. The relationship between attachment styles, emotional dysregulation, and Metacognitive functions in patients with Personality disorders. J Psychol Psychother. 2016;6(265):2161–048710002.

    Google Scholar 

  62. Paris J. Antisocial personality disorder: a biopsychosocial model. Can J Psychiatry. 1996;41(2):75–80.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Paris J. Social factors in the personality disorders. Transcult Psychiatry. 1997;34(4):421–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  64. Johnson JG, Cohen P, Chen H, Kasen S, Brook JS. Parenting behaviors associated with risk for offspring personality disorder during adulthood. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006;63(5):579–87.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Kabbur RU. The analysis of parenting style and personality disorder of the college students. Dharwad: UAS; 2006.

    Google Scholar 

  66. Beres D, Obers SJ. The effects of extreme deprivation in infancy on psychic structure in adolescence: a study in ego development. Psychoanal Study Child. 1950;5(1):212–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  67. Doinita NE, Maria ND. Attachment and parenting styles. Procedia-Social Behav Sci. 2015;203:199–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  68. Enns MW, Cox BJ, Larsen DK. Perceptions of parental bonding and symptom severity in adults with depression: mediation by personality dimensions. Can J Psychiatry. 2000;45(3):263–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Stravynski A, Elie R, Franche RL. Perception of early parenting by patients diagnosed avoidant personality disorder: a test of the overprotection hypothesis. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 1989;80(5):415–20.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. De Panfilis C, Salvatore P, Marchesi C, Cazzolla R, Tonna M, Maggini C. Parental bonding and personality disorder: the mediating role of alexithymia. J Personal Disord. 2008;22(5):496–508.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  71. Thimm JC. Mediation of early maladaptive schemas between perceptions of parental rearing style and personality disorder symptoms. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 2010;41(1):52–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Russ E, Heim A, Westen D. Parental bonding and personality pathology assessed by clinician report. J Personal Disord. 2003;17(6):522–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  73. Mairet K, Boag S, Warburton W. How important is temperament? The relationship between coping styles, early maladaptive schemas and social anxiety. Int J Psychol Psychol Therapy. 2014;14(2):171–90.

    Google Scholar 

  74. Gök AC. Associated factors of psychological well-being: early maladaptive schemas, schema coping processes, and parenting styles. Middle East Technical University; 2012.

  75. Kooraneh AE, Amirsardari L. Predicting early maladaptive schemas using Baumrind’s parenting styles. Iran J Psychiatry Behav Sci. 2015;9(2).

  76. Otani K, Suzuki A, Matsumoto Y, Enokido M, Kuwahata F, Takahashi N. Associations of working models of the self and other with Cloninger’s personality dimensions. Compr Psychiatr. 2015;56:175–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  77. Picardi A, Fagnani C, Nisticò L, Stazi MA. A twin study of attachment style in young adults. J Pers. 2011;79(5):965–92.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. Doyle AB, Markiewicz D, Karavasilis L. Associations between parenting style and attachment to mother in middle childhood and adolescence. Int J Behav Dev. 2003;27(2):153–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  79. Karantzas GC, Younan R, Pilkington PD. The associations between early maladaptive schemas and adult attachment styles: a meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Sci Pract. 2023;30(1):1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  80. Tabachnick BG, Fidell LS, Ullman JB. Using multivariate statistics: Pearson Boston. MA; 2013.

  81. Lee CW, Taylor G, Dunn J. Factor structure of the schema questionnaire in a large clinical sample. Cogn Therapy Res. 1999;23(4):441–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  82. Schmidt NB, Joiner TE, Young JE, Telch MJ. The schema questionnaire: investigation of psychometric properties and the hierarchical structure of a measure of maladaptive schemas. Cogn Therapy Res. 1995;19(3):295–321.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  83. Ahi G, Mohammadifar M, BESHARAT MA. Reliability and validity of young’s schema questionnaire-short form. 2007.

  84. Mojallal M, Hakim Javadi M, Mousavi SVA, Hosseinkhanzadeh AA. Gholam Ali Lavasani M. Early maladaptive schemas and interpersonal problems in Iranian university students. 2015.

  85. Collins NL. Revised adult attachment scale. Behavior Therapy. 1996.

  86. Collins NL, Read SJ. Adult attachment, working models, and relationship quality in dating couples. J Personal Soc Psychol. 1990;58(4):644.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  87. Boroujerdi FG, Kimiaee SA, Yazdi SAA, Safa M. Attachment style and history of childhood abuse in suicide attempters. Psychiatry Res. 2019;271:1–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  88. Buri JR. Parental authority questionnaire. J Pers Assess. 1991;57(1):110–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  89. Mahdavi N, Khalil E, Vajiheh K. Parenting styles and dimensions of children’s maladaptive behaviors. 2013.

  90. Yousefia S, Far AS, Abdolahian E. Parenting stress and parenting styles in mothers of ADHD with mothers of normal children. Procedia-Social Behav Sci. 2011;30:1666–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  91. Poulsen J, French A. Discriminant function analysis. San Francisco, CA: San Francisco State University; 2008.

    Google Scholar 

  92. Nakash-Eisikovits O, Dutra L, Westen D. Relationship between attachment patterns and personality pathology in adolescents. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2002;41(9):1111–23.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  93. Reitman D, Rhode PC, Hupp SD, Altobello C. Development and validation of the parental authority questionnaire–revised. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. 2002;24(2):119–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  94. Reti IM, Samuels JF, Eaton WW, Bienvenu Iii OJ, Costa PT Jr, Nestadt G. Influences of parenting on normal personality traits. Psychiatry Res. 2002;111(1):55–64.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  95. Henschel C. The effects of parenting style on the development of narcissism. Behav Health. 2014;1(1):1–8.

    Google Scholar 

  96. Claudio L. Can parenting styles affect the children’s development of narcissism? A systematic review. Open Psychol J. 2016;9(1).

  97. Kılıçkaya S, Ucar N, Denizci Nazlıgül M. A systematic review of the association between parenting styles and narcissism in young adults: from Baumrind’s perspective. Psychol Rep. 2023;126(2):620–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  98. Morrison J. Histrionic personality disorder in women with somatization disorder. Psychosomatics: Journal of Consultation and Liaison Psychiatry; 1989.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  99. Beck AT, Davis DD, Freeman A. Cognitive therapy of personality disorders. Guilford Publications; 2015.

  100. Johnson JG, Cohen P, Smailes EM, Skodol AE, Brown J, Oldham JM. Childhood verbal abuse and risk for personality disorders during adolescence and early adulthood. Compr Psychiatr. 2001;42(1):16–23.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  101. Armour J-A, Joussemet M, Mageau GA, Varin R. Perceived parenting and borderline personality features during adolescence. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2022:1–12.

  102. Steylaerts B, Dierckx E, Schotte C. Relationships between DSM-5 personality disorders and early maladaptive schemas from the perspective of dimensional and categorical comorbidity. Cogn Therapy Res. 2023;47(3):454–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  103. Bilge Y, Balaban G. The relationships between personality disorders and early maladaptive schemas and the moderating role of gender. Alpha Psychiatry. 2021;22(1):12.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This study is part of the Master’s thesis of Maryam Emmi and is supervised by Dr. Maryam Moghadasin. This research received no specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors. Also, the authors would like to thank all those who participated in the study and supported the authors in completing this research.

Funding

This research received no financial support or specific grant.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Study concept and design: M.M. and M.E.; Data acquisition: M.E., M.M., and A.T.; Analysis and interpretation of data: M.M. and M.E.; Drafting of the manuscript: M.E., M.M. and H.M.; Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: M.M.; Study supervision: M.M. All authors reviewed the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Maryam Moghadasin.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

All procedures performed in the study involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. The ethics committee of Kharazmi University, Iran, approved all experimental protocols. Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Conflict of interest

There is no conflict of interest.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Emami, M., Moghadasin, M., Mastour, H. et al. Early maladaptive schema, attachment style, and parenting style in a clinical population with personality disorder and normal individuals: a discriminant analysis model. BMC Psychol 12, 78 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-024-01564-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-024-01564-5

Keywords