- Research
- Open access
- Published:
Pragmatic competence in people with dual diagnosis: down syndrome and autism spectrum disorder
BMC Psychology volume 12, Article number: 74 (2024)
Abstract
Background
Pragmatics is an area that can be affected in a wide variety of disorders. In this sense, Syndromic Autism is defined as a disorder in which a causal link is established between an associated syndrome and Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD). Likewise, Down Syndrome (DS) is one of the main genetically based syndromes in which ASD is described as one of its possible manifestations. In this direction, people with DS are described as social beings whereas in ASD there seems to be a specific alteration of this domain.
Methods
In this study, pragmatic performance was analysed in a sample of 72 participants, where comparisons were made between the scores obtained by children with ASD (nâ=â24), with DS (nâ=â24) and with DSâ+âASD (nâ=â24).
Results
The Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ), the Block Objective and Criterial Language Battery (BLOC-SR) and the Neuropsychology subtest (NEPSY-II) aimed at Theory of Mind (ToM) identified significant differences between the groups. However, two-to-two comparisons reported no significant differences between DS and DSâ+âASD.
Conclusions
Although several studies report differences between the three proposed groups, our data seem to suggest that ASD symptomatology in DS is associated with Intellectual Developmental Disorder (IDD). However, the lack of solid scientific evidence regarding comorbid diagnosis makes further research along these lines indispensable.
Trial registration
This study was approved by the Ethics Committee for Social Research at UCLM with reference CEIS-704,511-L8M4.
Background
Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) is a Neurodevelopmental Disorder characterized by persistent deficits in communication and social interaction [1], as well as restricted and repetitive patterns of behaviors and interests [2], without these deficits being better explained by the presence of an Intellectual Development Disorder (IDD) [3]. In Spain, it is estimated that 15 out of every 1000 school-age children exhibit symptoms compatible with ASD [4]. In this regard, according to data from the National Institute of Statistics (2020), it could be said that the exponential growth in ASD diagnoses over the last three decades, going from an estimated prevalence of 1.1 case per 10,000 inhabitants in 1999 to 3.3 cases per 10,000 inhabitants currently [5], is due to changes in diagnostic criteria [6].
Given all of this, it is not surprising that the diagnosis of ASD is considered a challenging process. However, if we add an underlying genetic syndrome to the mix, the detection becomes even more complex [7]. These cases not classified in diagnostic manuals are known as syndromic autism or âdouble syndromes,â with an estimated prevalence of 20% [8]. Specifically, authors like Artigas-Pallares, Gabau-Vila & Guitart-FeliubadalĂł (2005b) reported that Down Syndrome (DS) is one of the main genetically based syndromes in which ASD is described as one of its possible manifestations [9]. In this context, scientific literature has found high prevalence rates for the presence of this comorbidity, ranging from 18 to 38% [10].
In this regard, DS is one of the most common conditions within IDD, commonly known as Intellectual Disability (ID) [11,12,13]. DS is understood as a congenital syndrome resulting from the presence of an additional chromosome in pair 21 [14, 15], and it is characterized by atypical individual development [16, 17], generalized delays in developmental milestones, personal/social domain [18], adaptive functioning [19, 20] and linguistic development [21].
Based on the aforementioned background and considering various studies confirming the connection between ASD and ID [22], some research has reported increased severity of prototypical ASD symptoms in individuals with genetic syndromes as their Intellectual Quotient (IQ) decreases [23]. However, some studies have described reduced sensitivity and specificity of ASD diagnostic tools, such as the Autism Diagnostic Observation Scheduleâ 2 (ADOS-2) [24] or the Autism Diagnostic Interviewâ Revised (ADI-R) [25], when used in individuals with genetic syndromes [26]. From this perspective, analyses by Marlborough et al. (2021) and Thurm et al. (2019) examined the performance of ID in the characteristic behavioral patterns of ASD in genetic disorders [27, 28] such as DS and Fragile X Syndrome (FXS) [29, 30]. As a result, it was concluded that the level of ID did not justify an increased prevalence of clinical ASD characteristics in DS and FXS. It is notable that most studies focused on analyzing the clinical phenotype of the DSâ+âASD population establish comparisons predominantly with the DS population [10, 26, 29,30,31,32,33,34]. These comparisons seem to have been based on the assumption that individuals with a comorbid diagnosis, in addition to more severe cognitive, communicative, and behavioral deficits, would also have a specific and distinctive phenotype in relation to ASD. For instance, concerning the cognitive aspect, studies emphasizing the impact on the population with a DS diagnosis and those with a comorbid diagnosis confirm that the latter have lower IQ [10, 34] and limited comprehensive and expressive language skills [35], among others. Therefore, several studies have determined that those with the comorbid diagnosis of DSâ+âASD exhibit greater impairment in the language component compared to ASD or DS alone [36].
Hence, while research on DSâ+âASD is ongoing, the early identification of this dual diagnosis is complex due to the overlap of behavioral signs in these disorders [37,38,39]. Nonetheless, studies have identified two patterns of ASD onset in DS [40, 41]. In the first scenario, research such as that by Hahn, Hamrick, Kelleher & Roberts (2020) or Hepburn, Philofsky, Fidler, et Rogers (2008) showed that ASD symptoms in children with DS diagnosed with ASD emerge early in development, with subjects from both studies demonstrating deficiencies in communication and social skills [42, 43]. Various analyses have corroborated that children with ID and ASD are characterized by inadequate performance in the domains of socialization and communication [44,45,46]. Regarding the second scenario, Castillo et al. (2008) found a plateau in developmental milestones related to language acquisition and use, as well as social skills, where the regression pattern in those with DSâ+âASD is similar to those with ASD only [47].
For these reasons, we can assert that these investigations primarily emphasize the importance of pragmatic skills and Theory of Mind (ToM) in the DSâ+âASD population, identifying significant deficits in areas of social and emotional reciprocity [48], clear limitations in communication skills compared to individuals with DS only, and limited or absent meaningful symbolic communication [49].
Methods
Aim
The aim of this present study is to analyze the pragmatic skills of the population with the comorbid diagnosis of DSâ+âASD to determine the characteristics in this area compared to the population with DS or ASD as a sole diagnosis. The main hypothesis of the study is based on the fact that the pragmatic profile of people with dual diagnosis will be more similar to the population with ASD compared to the population with DS.
Participants
The selection of participants was established based on the following inclusion criteria: (1) Having a diagnosis of DS, ASD, or DSâ+âASD conducted by a medical specialist or clinical psychologist, (2) Having a chronological age between 8 and 16 years, and (3) Having Spanish as their native language. On the contrary, the exclusion criteria were: (1) Individuals with a medical diagnosis other than DS, ASD, or DSâ+âASD, (2) Subjects under 8 years of age, or conversely, over 16 years of age, and (3) Participants whose native language is not Spanish. This range wase selected because it is worth noting that the minimum age set is 8 years, as even though the presence of ASD symptoms in DS manifests in early stages, ensuring a certain level of pragmatic development is necessary and we consider that at the age of 16 adolescence has already ended. Additionally, due to the frequency of studies focusing on the DSâ+âASD population without a truly diagnosed ASD group, we decided to ensure that all participants in that group had a diagnosis of ASD beyond a screening tool. All participants in the DSâ+âASD group had a clinical diagnosis conducted by a clinical psychologist based on the results from the ADI-R and ADOS-2 assessments.
Therefore, the study comprises minor users from various Spanish entities and associations. Thus, the research involves 72 participants divided into 3 groups. The first group consists of 24 subjects with DS (13 males and 11 females) with a mean age of 11.6 (SDâ=â2.9). The second group is composed of 24 individuals with ASD (16 males and 8 females), with a mean age of 11.5 (SDâ=â2.11). The third and final group is formed by 24 children and adolescents with DSâ+âASD (17 males and 7 females), with a mean age of 11.1 (SDâ=â2.21). Furthermore, due to the potential influence of participantsâ intellectual capacity on pragmatic components, the level of intelligence was assessed using the NEPSY-II neuropsychological assessment battery [50], revealing no significant differences among the groups. The DS participants had an average IQ of 51.8 (SDâ=â2.61); the group of children and adolescents with ASD had an average IQ of 52.5 (SDâ=â3.16), and the group of participants with comorbid diagnosis had an average IQ of 49.3 (SDâ=â3.09).
Instruments
The choice of the instruments was based on the fact that they are currently the questionnaires that are most adaptable for people with special needs. For data collection, Form B of the Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ) [51] was employed. This screening method assesses communicative and social capacities in children aged 4 and above. The questionnaire comprises 40 questions to be answered affirmatively or negatively by the childâs parents or caregivers. The total score is calculated by summing the number of described behaviors marked as âyes.â It is essential to note that the SCQ should never be considered a diagnostic tool but rather as a filter that guides us on whether a more comprehensive evaluation is needed. If a cut-off score exceeding 15 is obtained, the possibility of ASD is considered, warranting further evaluation. To quantify the obtained criteria, the pragmatic module of the Objective and Criterial Language Battery (BLOC-SR) [52] was administered. This standardized test is intended for users aged 5 to 14, evaluating language use in communicative acts through 23 items related to 4 sub-scenes, originating from a specific scene involving a visit to the veterinarian. The interpretation of scores in this section is more intricate than in others, as if the response is literal, a score of 1 point is immediately assigned; otherwise, the examiner must indicate approval or disapproval of the implied content based on whether the response employs the intended pragmatic category. Finally, to assess the prevalence of mentalistic resources, a subtest from the NEPSY-II [50] called âTheory of Mindâ (ToM) was employed. This test comprises two specific tasks: verbal and contextual. In the former, based on a specific description or scenario, questions are posed to determine the ability to attribute beliefs or behaviors to a third party. In the latter, there are 21 items that assess 1st and 2nd order task resolution, comprehension of mentalistic verbs, idiomatic phrases, and inferential stories, imitation, integration of information into a coherent whole, and interpretation of facial expressions based on defined conditions. This subtest evaluates recognition of othersâ emotions based on a series of images depicting specific social situations.
Procedure
Initially, contact was established with the Down Syndrome Federation of Castilla - La Mancha and various centers and private clinics in Madrid, Valladolid, Palencia, and Toledo. Information for participation in the project was provided, explaining the potential scope of the study and addressing any queries that may arise. In this regard, an initial meeting was crucial, during which a gathering with patients, caregivers (parents or guardians), and professionals involved in therapeutic intervention took place to ensure understanding of the study and explain the roles of participants and the significance of their involvement. This step empowered them to make decisions freely and knowingly, thereby guaranteeing their informed and voluntary participation. Once the groups agreed, completed, signed, and submitted the informed consent form, a date was scheduled for the administration of assessment tests. The consent had to be expressed by the parent/guardian as the legal representative of the minor or dependent individual. Subsequently, individual sessions of 30Â min were arranged for each family, during which the speech therapist conducted the subjectâs assessment while parents completed the SCQ. This study was approved by the Ethics Committee for Social Research at University of Castilla-La Mancha with reference CEIS-704,511-L8M4.
Data analysis
Firstly, an exploratory analysis was conducted by examining measures of central tendency (mean and median) and their measures of dispersion. Subsequently, the normality of distribution was assessed through tests of normality, shape statistics (skewness and kurtosis), and normal Q-Q plots with and without trend. The normality of the sample was assessed using the KolmogorovâSmirnov test, which indicated that the data were non-parametric. Specifically, the Shapiro-Wilk test was employed given that the sample size for each group did not exceed 50 participants. All variables were found to be non-parametric (pâ<â0.5). Additionally, in order to determine significant differences among the variables, the Kruskal-Wallis analysis of variance test was used, as the variables are quantitative and do not follow a normal distribution.
Results
Firstly, the data reveal that the group comprising children and adolescents with ASD achieves a higher mean score than the groups consisting of individuals with DS and DSâ+âASD in the SCQ questionnaire. Conversely, they attain a lower mean score compared to the other two groups in the BLOC assessment (Table 1).
Continuing, the analysis of results concerning the SCQ was undertaken. The results have revealed significant differences (X2(2)â=â20.021; pâ<â0.001) in the total communicative competence level among children with ASD, children with DSâ+âASD, and children with DS. Specifically, it was observed that all three groups exhibited symptomatology consistent with ASD by surpassing the questionnaireâs cutoff point of 15 points. However, the group consisting of individuals with ASD obtained a higher mean score compared to the other two groups. Pairwise comparisons demonstrated that while significant differences in pragmatic competence level were observed between the DS and ASD groups (W(2)â=â5.34; pâ<â0.001) and between the ASD and DSâ+âASD groups (W(2) = -5.46; pâ<â0.01), such differences were absent when comparing the DS and DSâ+âASD groups (W(2) = -0.04; pâ>â0.05).
However, when considering the grouping of SCQ items into the three core areas of ASD diagnosis, no significant differences were found between groups: social interaction (H(2)â=â3.52; pâ>â0.05), communication difficulties (H(2)â=â0.20; pâ>â0.05), and restricted, repetitive, and stereotyped behavior (H(2)â=â039; pâ>â0.05).
Moving on to the BLOC results, they once again demonstrated statistically significant differences (H(2)â=â20.40; pâ<â0.001) in pragmatic competence level among children with ASD, children with DS, and children with DSâ+âASD. In this regard, pairwise comparisons indicated significant differences in pragmatic competence between DS and ASD (W = -5.06; pâ<â0.001) and between ASD and DSâ+âASD (Wâ=â5.68; pâ<â0.001). Conversely, no significant differences were found when comparing groups of children with DS and DSâ+âASD (Wâ=â1.79; pâ>â0.05).
Finally, the results regarding the Theory of Mind (ToM) subtest indicated statistically significant differences among groups (H (2)â=â21.1; pâ<â0.001), with the ASD group obtaining the lowest scores. Pairwise comparisons revealed significant differences between the groups comprising individuals with DS and ASD (W = -5.65; pâ<â0.001) and between those with ASD and DSâ+âASD (Wâ=â5.47; pâ<â0.001). Conversely, no differences were found between groups composed of individuals with DS and DSâ+âASD (W = -1.41; pâ>â0.05).
Discussion
The objective of the present investigation focused on analyzing whether there were differences in pragmatic skills between the population presenting isolated DS or ASD compared to the comorbid presentation of DSâ+âASD. This inquiry was prompted by the contentious debate within the scientific community regarding whether the pragmatic alterations commonly associated with ASD observed in the DS population are secondary to the DS or instead linked to comorbidity between both disorders. The results have revealed that the group of children and adolescents with a comorbid diagnosis of DSâ+âASD exhibited similar characteristics to the DS group and differed in skills from the group with ASD, contrasting with other studies [53, 54]. For example, Channell et al. (2015) reported that individuals with DS showing ASD symptomatology displayed a more pronounced developmental delay, characterized by frequent disruptions in social communication and socioemotional reciprocity compared to those with isolated DS [55]. Similarly, Dressler et al. (2011) evaluated peer relationships [56] using the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scale (VABS) [57] and found greater impairment in DSâ+âASD, in contrast to results obtained using the Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS) [58], which suggested favorable scores in DSâ+âASD as opposed to those with ASD.
This disparity in the profile of pragmatic skills reflected in the scientific literature could stem from the fact that the majority of scientific publications employ surveys or interviews with parents aimed at diagnosing ASD, attempting to define symptomatology consistent with the disorder and its severity [35]. From this perspective, the study by Warner et al. (2017), based on the application of the SCQ [51], compared 183 children with ASD to 189 children with DS who scored above 15 on this screening test. The results demonstrated significant differences between both groups in SCQ scores [59], in line with our findings. However, the authors discovered that distinctions primarily occurred in the domain of reciprocal social interaction, unlike our study. This can be attributed to differing perspectives on DS, distinguishing between those who perceive these individuals as sociable beings [60] and those who demonstrate pragmatic difficulties [61].
It appears, therefore, that the findings regarding pragmatic development in these populations suggest that while pragmatic aspects pose challenges in ASD [62], DS [61], and DSâ+âASD [63], their pragmatic competence differs when compared to each other. Thus, the DSâ+âASD group [36] seems to exhibit relative patterns of proficiency in this competence when compared to DS. This aligns with prior research by Dressler et al. (2011), which found that, based on the CARS [58], individuals with DSâ+âASD exhibit greater similarity to those with DS, significantly differing from those with ASD, especially in the imitation skills and interactive pragmatics present in personal relationships [56]. For this reason, it is expected that the DS population tends to be affable and sociable [64], thus not exhibiting deficits in their social and pragmatic skills [60].
On the other hand, focusing on the comparison between DS and ASD, we find that the pragmatic level of DS is conceived as a tool for communication and social interaction, as previously shown in Ferrarioâs research [65]. Conversely, ASD presents a pronounced deficit in this component. In this regard, Adamson et al. (2009), comparing 23 children with ASD and 29 children with DS to 56 typically developing children, found that the ASD group exhibited lower intersubjectivity due to more pronounced alterations compared to the DS group [66]. This corroborates our findings, with the ASD group scoring lower than the DS and DSâ+âASD groups in the pragmatics section of the BLOC.
Furthermore, Godfrey et al. (2019), using the ADI-R [25], assessed individuals with ASD, DS, and DSâ+âASD, observing that, based on parental criteria, the ASD group, in contrast to the DSâ+âASD group, displayed a higher index of prototypical ASD-associated behaviors. Likewise, the DSâ+âASD group exhibited symptoms more similar to ASD than DS, contrary to our results [63].
The obtained data also indicated differences in Theory of Mind (ToM) skills performance among groups of children and adolescents with DS and ASD, as well as between groups of children and adolescents with ASD and DSâ+âASD. Along these lines, the findings of this study can be interpreted in line with previous research that demonstrated better performance by the DS population compared to the ASD population [67]. Although no literature has been found that directly compares ToM task performance in the DSâ+âASD population, it would have been expected that they would perform worse than the other two groups, given the existing literature suggesting a more severe deficit profile. However, our data did not reveal significant differences between groups consisting of individuals with DS and DSâ+âASD, which implies that the ASD-compatible symptomatology detected in the DS population could arise due to the DS itself [68]. Regarding the relationship between Theory of Mind and pragmatic abilities, some authors such as [69], found that in people with ASD, group with âLower ToM abilitiesâ was characterized by more severe ASD symptoms and poorer pragmatic skills, in terms of inappropriate communicative beginnings and deficits in coherence and interpretation of language depending on the context, among other indicators. This group also showed significantly less mastery of daily living skills and poorer adaptive skills than the âHigher ToM abilitiesâ profile, which showed less widespread impairment. This relationship has also been documented in the case of Down Syndrome [70]. Similarly, the absence of differences in ToM skills between the DS and DSâ+âASD groups may be influencing the results obtained in the BLOC. This test requires the use of these skills to successfully complete it, as participants need to put themselves in the position of another to respond as they should in a given social situation.
Finally, it is important to highlight that early diagnosis is necessary to implement an effective therapy tailored to the childâs needs at a critical time in development [71]. Appropriate early treatment can reduce childrenâs symptoms and improve their overall development and quality of life, allowing them to gain social skills and the ability to act better in social situations, in order to achieve more autonomy in later life [72]. It is for this reason that it must be taken into account that a precise and correct diagnosis in these populations will serve as a starting point to propose the most appropriate intervention possible.
Conclusions
Therefore, this research confirms significant differences between the clinical DS and ASD groups, as well as between the ASD and DSâ+âASD groups, in both the scores obtained in the SCQ and the BLOC, as well as in the ToM task. However, no differences were found between the DS and DSâ+âASD groups in any of the tests. Thus, our data suggest that there are no differences in the pragmatic performance of the DSâ+âASD population compared to the isolated presentation of ASD. Nevertheless, the lack of robust scientific evidence regarding comorbid diagnosis, coupled with the fact that the available studies suggest a clustering of more severe ASD symptomatology in the DSâ+âASD population, makes further investigation in this direction essential.
Data availability
All data from this study are available upon request to the corresponding author.
Abbreviations
- ADOS-2:
-
Autism Diagnostic Observation Scheduleâ 2
- ADI-R:
-
Autism Diagnostic Interviewâ Revised
- ASD:
-
Autism Spectrum Disorder
- BLOC S-R:
-
Block Objective and Criterial Language Battery
- DS:
-
Down Syndrome
- FXS:
-
Fragile X Syndrome
- ID:
-
Intellectual Disability
- IDD:
-
Intellectual Developmental Disorder
- IQ:
-
Intellectual Quotient
- SCQ:
-
Social Communication Questionnaire
- ToM:
-
Theory of Mind
References
Celis AlcalĂĄa G, Ochoa Madrigal MG. Trastorno Del espectro autista (TEA). Revista De La Facultad De Medicina De La UNAM. 1 de enero de. 2022;65(1):7â7.
Daswani Daswan RP, SantamarĂa Ramiro M, Lago GarcĂa B, RodrĂguez HernĂĄndez PJ. Consideraciones actuales sobre el trastorno del espectro autista. Canarias PediĂĄtrica, ISSN 1131â6128, Vol 43, No 1, 2019, pags 25â30 [Internet]. 2019; Disponible en: https://dialnet.unirioja.es/servlet/oaiart?codigo=7186931.
Psychiatric Association A. author. DSM-5. Manual DiagnĂłstico y EstadĂstico de los Trastornos Mentales: DSM-5Âź [Internet]. 2014 [citado 3 de marzo de 2023]. Disponible en: http://www.medicapanamericana.com/VisorEbookV2/Ebook/9788491101727.
Morales-Hidalgo P, Roige-Castellvi J, Hernandez-Martinez C, Voltas N, Canals J. Prevalence and Characteristics of Autism Spectrum Disorder Among Spanish School-Age Children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 1 de septiembre de. 2018;48(9):3176-3176-90.
INEbase, Sociedad. /Salud /Encuestas de discapacidades / Resultados [Internet]. 2020 [citado el 16 de marzo de 2023]; Disponible en: https://www.ine.es/dyngs/INEbase/es/operacion.htm?c=Estadistica_C&cid=1254736176782&menu=resultados&idp=1254735573175
Stefan N, Hansen ET, Parner. Diana Schendel. Explaining the increase in the prevalence of autism spectrum disorders:the proportion attributable to changes in reporting practices. 1 de enero de 2015 [citado 10 de marzo de 2023]; Disponible en: https://explore.openaire.eu/search/publication?articleId=doi_dedup___::8d5870cad3e8a3d60fada75a0d88d332.
Artigas-PallarĂ©s J, Gabau-Vila E, Guitart-FeliubadalĂł M. [Syndromic autism: I. General aspects]. Revista De neurologĂa 15 de enero de. 2005;40(Suppl 1):143âS143.
Balbuena F. Revista Chilena de Neuro-PsiquiatrĂa. EtiologĂa del autismo: el continuo idiopĂĄtico-sindrĂłmico como tentatitva explicativa. 1 de enero de 2015 [citado 5 de marzo de 2023]; Disponible en: https://explore.openaire.eu/search/publication?articleId=od______2736::a931cf6eef40dea0bc78a53e1aabdfc5.
Artigas-PallarĂ©s J, Gabau-Vila E, Guitart-FeliubadalĂł M. [Syndromic autism: II. Genetic syndromes associated with autism]. Revista De neurologĂa 15 de enero de. 2005;40(Suppl 1):151âS151.
Carter JC, Capone GT, Gray RM, Cox CS, Kaufmann WE. Autistic-spectrum disorders in down syndrome: further delineation and distinction from other behavioral abnormalities. Am J Med Genet Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genet. 2007;144B(1):87â94.
Stylianos E, Antonarakis BG, Skotko MS, Rafii A, Strydom SE, Pape DW, Bianchi et al. Down syndrome. Nature Reviews Disease Primers [Internet]. 1 de enero de 2020 [citado 5 de marzo de 2023];6. Disponible en: https://explore.openaire.eu/search/publication?articleId=doi_dedup___::e639f6d37952d0e80b73821ec9cb8be6.
Blythe G, Crissman G, Worley N, Roizen PS. Kishnani. Current perspectives on Down syndrome: Selected medical and social issues. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part C: Seminars in Medical Genetics. 1 de enero de 2006;127-127-30.
HĂŒls A, Costa ACS, Dierssen M, Baksh RA, Bargagna S, Baumer NT et al. Medical vulnerability of individuals with Down syndrome to severe COVID-19âdata from the Trisomy 21 Research Society and the UK ISARIC4C survey. EClinicalMedicine [Internet]. 1 de marzo de 2021 [citado 7 de marzo de 2023];33. Disponible en: https://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=edselp&AN=S2589537021000493&site=eds-live.
Leonard H, Wen X. The epidemiology of mental retardation: challenges and opportunities in the new millennium. Mental Retard Dev Disabil Res Reviews 1 de agosto de. 2002;8(3):117â7.
Akhtar F, Bokhari SRA. StatPearls. 1 de enero de 2022 [citado 5 de marzo de 2023]; Disponible en: https://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=mdc&AN=30252272&site=eds-live
Patterson D. Molecular genetic analysis of Down syndrome. Hum Genet 1 de Julio De. 2009;126(1):195â5.
Sommer CA, Henrique-Silva F. Trisomy 21 and Down syndrome-a short review/Trissomia do 21 e Sindrome De Down: uma breve revisao. Brazilian J Biology 1 de mayo de. 2008;68(2):447â7.
Larrosa CM. GonzĂĄlez RodrĂguez N. AceptaciĂłn social de niños con SĂndrome de Down integrados en escuelas comunes. 2019.
Luckasson R, Borthwick-Duffy S, Buntinx WHE, Coulter DL, Craig EM, Reeve A et al. Mental Retardation: Definition, Classification, and Systems of Supports. 10th Edition. [Internet]. 2002 [citado 5 de marzo de 2023]. Disponible en: https://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=eric&AN=ED468327&site=eds-live.
Daunhauer L, International Review of Research in Developmental Disabilities. Chapter Five - The Early Development of Adaptive Behavior and Functional Performance in Young Children with Down Syndrome: Current Knowledge and Future Directions [Internet]. Vol. 40,. 2011 [citado 13 de marzo de 2023]. 109 p. Disponible en: https://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=edselp&AN=B9780123744784000058&site=eds-live.
Moraleda-SepĂșlveda E, LĂłpez-Resa P. Morphological Difficulties in People with Developmental Language Disorder. Children. 1 de enero de 2022;9(125):125-125-125.
Siklos S, Kerns KA. Assessing need for Social Support in parents of children with Autism and Down Syndrome. J Autism Dev Disorders 1 de diciembre de. 2006;36(7):921â1.
Wester Oxelgren U, Aberg M, Myrelid A, Anneren G, Westerlund J, Gustafsson J, et al. Autism needs to be considered in children with Down Syndrome. Acta Paediatr 1 de noviembre de. 2019;108(11):2019â9.
Lord C, Rutter M, DiLavore P, Risi S, Gotham K, Bishop S. Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule, Second Edition (ADOS-2) Manual (Part I). Modules. Torrance, CA: West. Psychol. Serv. 2012, 32, 88â92.
Rutter M, Le Couteur A, Lord C. Autism diagnostic interview revised (ADI-R). Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services; 2003.
Oliver C, Richards C, Moss J, Nelson L. Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder symptomatology and related behavioural characteristics in individuals with Down syndrome. Autism 15 de mayo de. 2012;17:390â0.
Marlborough M, Welham A, Jones C, Reckless S, Moss J. Autism spectrum disorder in females with fragile X syndrome: a systematic review and meta-analysis of prevalence. J Neurodevelopmental Disorders 1 De Julio De. 2021;13(1):1â1.
Thurm A, Farmer C, Salzman E, Lord C, Bishop S. State of the Field: Differentiating Intellectual Disability From Autism Spectrum Disorder. Frontiers in Psychiatry [Internet]. 1 de julio de 2019 [citado 21 de abril de 2023];10. Disponible en: https://doaj.org/article/1a971e4b4fe743d49fb3464abc66d532.
Bradbury KR, Anderberg EI, Huang-Storms L, Vasile I, Greene RK, Duvall SW. Co-occurring Down Syndrome and Autism Spectrum Disorder: cognitive, adaptive, and behavioral characteristics. J Autism Dev Disorders 1 De marzo de. 2022;52(3):1235â5.
Channell MM, The Down Syndrome Cognition Project, Hahn LJ, Rosser TC, Hamilton D, Frank-Crawford MA, et al. Characteristics Associated with Autism Spectrum Disorder Risk in individuals with Down Syndrome. J Autism Dev Disord. 2019;49(9):3543â56.
Capone GT, Grados MA, Kaufmann WE, Bernad-Ripoll S, Jewell A. Down syndrome and comorbid autism-spectrum disorder: characterization using the aberrant behavior checklist. Am J Med Genet Part A. 2005;134A(4):373â80.
Hepburn S, Eric J. Moody. Diagnosing Autism in Individuals with Known Genetic Syndromes: Clinical Considerations and Implications for Intervention. 1 de enero de 2011 [citado 4 de abril de 2023]; Disponible en: https://explore.openaire.eu/search/publication?articleId=doi_dedup___::5b4898507bd77fc438fe331c69db0c7b.
Ji NY, Capone GT, Kaufmann WE. Autism spectrum disorder in Down syndrome: cluster analysis of aberrant Behaviour Checklist data supports diagnosis. J Intellect Disabil Res 1 de noviembre de. 2011;55(11):1064â4.
Moss J, Howlin P. Autism spectrum disorders in genetic syndromes: implications for diagnosis, intervention and understanding the wider autism spectrum disorder population. J Intellect Disabil Res 1 de octubre de. 2009;53(10):852â2.
Hamner T, Hepburn S, Zhang F, Fidler D, Robinson Rosenberg C, Robins DL, et al. Cognitive profiles and autism symptoms in Comorbid Down Syndrome and Autism Spectrum Disorder. J Dev Behav Pediatrics: JDBP 1 de Abril De. 2020;41(3):172â2.
Magyar CI, Pandolfi V, Dill CA. An initial evaluation of the social communication questionnaire for the assessment of autism spectrum disorders in children with Down syndrome [Internet]. Vol. 33. 2012 [citado 23 de abril de 2023]. 134 p. Disponible en: https://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=edsgao&AN=edsgcl.289973911&site=eds-live.
Capone GT. Down syndrome and autistic spectrum disorder: a look at what we know. Disabil Solut. 3(5â6), 8â15.
Reilly C. Autism spectrum disorders in Down syndrome: a review. Res Autism Spectr Disorders 1 de octubre de. 2009;3:829â9.
Stone WL, Lee EB. Can Autism be diagnosed accurately in children under 3 years? J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Disciplines 1 de febrero de. 1999;40(2):219â9.
Kalb LG, Law JK, Landa R, Law PA. Onset patterns prior to 36 months in Autism Spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disorders 1 de noviembre de. 2010;40(11):1389â9.
Ozonoff S, Iosif AM. Changing conceptualizations of regression: what prospective studies reveal about the onset of autism spectrum disorder. Neurosci Biobehavioral Reviews 1 de mayo de. 2019;100:296â6.
Hahn LJ, Hamrick LM, Kelleher BL, Roberts JE. Autism Spectrum Disorder-Associated Behaviour in infants with Down Syndrome. J Health Sci Educ. 2020;4(2):180.
Hepburn S, Philofsky A, Fidler DJ, Rogers S. Autism symptoms in toddlers with Down syndrome: a descriptive study. J Appl Res Intellect Disabil 1 de enero de. 2008;21(1):48â8.
Donald P, Oswald, Thomas H, Ollendick. Role taking and social competence in autism and mental retardation. J Autism Dev Disorders 1 De marzo de. 1989;19:119â9.
Rodrigue JR, Others. A comparative evaluation of adaptive behavior in children and adolescents with Autism, Down Syndrome, and Normal Development. J Autism Dev Disorders 1 de junio de. 1991;21(2):187â7.
Kraijer D. Review of adaptive behavior studies in mentally retarded persons with Autism/Pervasive developmental disorder. J Autism Dev Disorders 1 de febrero de. 2000;30(1):39â9.
Castillo H, Patterson B, Hickey F, Kinsman A, Howard JM, Mitchell T et al. Difference in age at regression in children with autism with and without Down syndrome [Internet]. Vol. 29. 2008 [citado 18 de marzo de 2023]. 89 p. Disponible en: https://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=edsgao&AN=edsgcl.178731200&site=eds-live.
Molloy CA, Murray DS, Kinsman A, Castillo H, Mitchell T, Hickey FJ, et al. Differences in the clinical presentation of Trisomy 21 with and without autism. J Intellect Disabil Res 1 de febrero de. 2009;53(2):143â3.
Versaci TM, Mattie LJ, Imming LJ. Down Syndrome and Autism Spectrum Disorder Dual diagnosis: important considerations for Speech-Language pathologists. Am J Speech-Language Pathol 1 de febrero de. 2021;30(1):34â4.
Korkman M, Kirk U, Kemp SL. NEPSY-II. 2014;1maletân.
Rutter M, Anthony B, Catherine L. Bailey Anthony, Lord Catherine. Cuestionario De comunicaciĂłn social (SCQ). Madrid: TEA; 2005.
Puyuelo Sanclemente M. Bloc screening-R: evaluaciĂłn del lenguaje. Nueva ed. rev. y amp. 2007;120 p.; 24Â cmâ+âcuaderno de imĂĄgenes, cuaderno de registro, Cd-rom con el programa BLOC-Info, manual de usuario, libro para aplicaciĂłn a poblaciones con necesidades educativas especĂficas.
Cook A, Quinn ED, Rowland C. Exploring expressive communication skills in a cross-sectional sample of individuals with a dual diagnosis of Autism Spectrum Disorder and Down Syndrome. Am J Intellect Dev Disabil 1 de marzo de. 2021;126(2):97â7.
Moss J, Richards C, Nelson L, Oliver C. Prevalence of Autism Spectrum Disorder Symptomatology and Related Behavioural Characteristics in Individuals with Down Syndrome [Internet]. 2013 jul [citado 17 de mayo de 2023] p. 390â0. Disponible en: https://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=eric&AN=EJ1015039&site=eds-live
Channell MM, Phillips BA, Loveall SJ, Conners FA, Bussanich PM, Klinger LG. Patterns of autism spectrum symptomatology in individuals with Down syndrome without comorbid autism spectrum disorder. J Neurodevelopmental Disorders. 2015;7(1).
Dressler A, Perelli V, Bozza M, Bargagna S. The autistic phenotype in Down syndrome: differences in adaptive behaviour versus down syndrome alone and autistic disorder alone. Funct Neurol 1 de Julio De. 2011;26(3):151â1.
Sparrow SS, Balla DA, Cicchetti DV. Vineland Adaptive Behavior scales VABS: expanded form Manual. American Guidance Service; 1984.
The childhood autism rating scale (CARS). Los Angeles: WPS; 2010.
Warner G, Howlin P, Salomone E, Moss J, Charman T. Profiles of children with Down syndrome who meet screening criteria for autism spectrum disorder (ASD): a comparison with children diagnosed with ASD attending specialist schools. J Intellect Disabil Res 1 de enero de. 2017;61(1):75â5.
Mason-Apps E, Stojanovik V, Houston-Price C, Seager E, Buckley S. Do infants with Down Syndrome show an early receptive Language advantage? J Speech Lang Hear Res 1 de febrero de. 2020;63(2):585â5.
Martin GE, Bush L, Klusek J, Patel S, Losh M. A Multimethod Analysis of Pragmatic Skills in Children and Adolescents With Fragile X Syndrome, Autism Spectrum Disorder, and Down Syndrome. Journal of Speech, Language & Hearing Research. 1 de diciembre de. 2018;61(12):3023-3023-37.
Binns AV, Cardy JO. Developmental social pragmatic interventions for preschoolers with autism spectrum disorder: A systematic review. Autism & Developmental Language Impairments [Internet]. 1 de enero de 2019 [citado 12 de mayo de 2023];4. Disponible en: https://explore.openaire.eu/search/publication?articleId=doi_dedup___::881ca582e8c9c9fc55bb1fdc07fa5bdf.
Godfrey M, Hepburn S, Fidler DJ, Tapera T, Zhang F, Rosenberg CR, et al. Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) symptom profiles of children with comorbid Down syndrome (DS) and ASD: a comparison with children with DS-only and ASD-only. Res Dev Disabil 1 de junio de. 2019;89:83â3.
Gibbs MV, Thorpe JG. Personality stereotype of noninstitutionalized Down syndrome children. Am J Mental Defic 1 de mayo de. 1983;87:601â1.
Ferrario I. Do parent-mediated interventions improve communication and language development in children with Down syndrome? - a Cochrane Review summary with commentary. Dev Neurorehabilitation 1 de octubre de. 2020;23(7):482â2.
Adamson LB, Bakeman R, Deckner DF, Romski M. Joint engagement and the emergence of language in children with autism and Down syndrome. J Autism Dev Disorders 1 de enero de. 2009;39(1):84â4.
Yirmiya N, Solomonica-Levi D, Shulman C, Pilowsky T. Theory of mind abilities in individuals with autism, Down syndrome, and mental retardation of unknown etiology: the role of age and intelligence. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Disciplines 1 de noviembre de. 1996;37(8):1003â3.
Orsmond GI, Seltzer MM. Siblings of individuals with autism or down syndrome: effects on adult lives. J Intellect Disabil Res 1 de Septiembre De. 2007;51(9):682â2.
Rosello B, Berenguer C, Baixauli I, GarcĂa R, Miranda A. Theory of mind profiles in children with autism spectrum disorder: Adaptive/social skills and pragmatic competence. Front Psychol 17 de Septiembre De. 2020;11:567401.
Lee M, Bush L, Martin GE, Barstein J, Maltman N, Klusek J, Losh M. A multi-method investigation of pragmatic development in individuals with Down syndrome. Am J Intellect Dev Disabil. 2017;122(4):289â309.
Boccaccio FM, Platania GA, Guerrera CS, Varrasi S, Privitera CR, Caponnetto P, Pirrone C, Castellano S. Autism spectrum disorder: recommended psychodiagnostic tools for early diagnosis. Health Psychol Res. 2023;11:77357.
Elder JH, Kreider CM, Brasher SN, Ansell M. Clinical impact of early diagnosis of autism on the prognosis and parent-child relationships. Psychol Res Behav Manage. 2017;10:283â92. https://doi.org/10.2147/prbm.s117499.
Funding
This research received no external funding.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
Conceptualization, E.M.-S. and P.L.-R.; methodology, S.C.E. and P.L.-R.; software, E.M.-S. and P.L.-R.; validation, E.M.-S. and P.L.-R.; formal analysis, E.M.-S. and S.C.E.; investigation, S.C.E and P.L.-R.; writingâoriginal draft preparation, S.C.E and P.L.-R.; writingâreview and editing, E.M.-S. and P.L.-R.; supervision, E.M.-S. and P.L.-R. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Institutional review board statement
The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki. This study was approved by the Ethics Committee for Social Research at University of Castilla-La Mancha with reference CEIS-704511-L8M4.
Informed consent
Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study. If participants are under 16, from a parent and/or legal guardian.
Consent for publication
Not applicable.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisherâs Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the articleâs Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the articleâs Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.
About this article
Cite this article
Escudero, S.C., SepĂșlveda, E. Pragmatic competence in people with dual diagnosis: down syndrome and autism spectrum disorder. BMC Psychol 12, 74 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-023-01508-5
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-023-01508-5