Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

The association between social support provision, psychological capital, subjective well-being and sense of indebtedness among undergraduates with low socioeconomic status

Abstract

Background

Social support consists of receipt and provision in the interpersonal exchange process. Many studies have explored and verified the effect of received social support. This study focuses on whether and when social support provision can benefit the providers’ positive psychological capital and subjective well-being.

Methods

A sample of 732 Chinese undergraduates with low socioeconomic status completed questionnaires on social support provision, psychological capital, life satisfaction, positive affect, negative affect, and sense of indebtedness.

Results

The correlation and regression analyses showed that impoverished college students’ social support provision was positively associated with life satisfaction, positive affect, and psychological capital and negatively associated with negative affect. The interaction between the sense of indebtedness and social support provision was negatively associated with life satisfaction, positive affect, and psychological capital, not significantly associated with negative affect.

Conclusion

The results demonstrated that giving social support can be as beneficial as receiving social support, and the sense of indebtedness can limit the benefits. Individuals with a lower sense of indebtedness are more likely to benefit from social support provision. The findings have implications for marginalized groups’ subjective well-being and positive psychological capital and show the necessity of guiding individuals to provide social support while maintaining their autonomy.

Peer Review reports

Introduction

Social support consists of receipt and provision in the interpersonal exchange process [1]. Many studies have explored and verified the effect of social support that is received (or perceived), such as health [2], positive feelings [3], mental health [4], engagement [5], well-being [6], the positive effect of social support provision is not fully explored. Existing studies have mainly focused on the impact of the elderly’s social support provision on physical health [7] and longevity [8], and less attention has been paid to the positive impact of social support provision on youth groups, such as psychological capital and subjective well-being. As a key psychological resource, psychological capital significantly influences individuals’ development [9]. Subjective well-being is an essential developmental indicator of the individual, comprising long-term levels of positive affect, life satisfaction, and lack of negative affect [10].

The personal well-being model suggests that giving behaviors can enhance and improve self-evaluation and build self-psychological resources [11]. It is reasonable to infer that giving social support may increase the individual’s psychological capital. According to the need to belong theory, interpersonal interaction is an essential source of subjective well-being [12]. Social support provision contains many deep interpersonal interactions and may profoundly influence subjective well-being. More importantly, the buffer role for the psychological capital and subjective well-being of an individual with low socioeconomic status, recognized as more generous in supporting others [13], should be explored. However, insufficient economic resources may deteriorate subjective well-being [14] and psychological capital [15]. It is of theoretical and practical importance to explore the potential influence of social support provision on impoverished individuals' psychological capital and subjective well-being.

The sense of indebtedness is an emotion produced by an individual after receiving the favor of others, and different individuals have different levels of indebtedness after receiving the same favor. Compared to individuals with a low sense of indebtedness, individuals with a high sense of indebtedness are more likely to experience indebtedness and thus have a stronger sense of obligation and compulsion to repay [16]. Every year in China, many impoverished college students are guided to give back after receiving a national bursary by doing things beneficial to others and society. It is unknown whether this situation will induce a higher sense of indebtedness in individuals with high indebtedness and affect the autonomy of social support provision. Giving social support is a self-initiated and self-determined behavior. If giving is affected by a sense of obligation, will it reduce the positive impact of this autonomous behavior? To fill this gap, this study focused on the link between social support provision, psychological capital and subjective well-being in college students with different sense of indebtedness.

Social support provision and subjective well-being

Social support involves receiving and providing in any process of interpersonal exchange. The former refers to perceived or received care and respect from others [17], while the latter refers to providing care and respect to others [18]. The two influence each other in interpersonal interaction. People may actively provide social support to others or provide emotional or instrumental support to others after receiving support from them [19]. However, social support in psychology research is tacitly regarded as received or perceived care from others. Many scholars have researched the influence of social support on recipients, but the positive impact of social support on the provider has yet to be emphasized. A few studies have confirmed the positive effect of social support provision on the providers’ physical health and mortality [20, 21]. However, not enough studies have focused on the psychological benefit of social support provision on the provider, especially on their subjective well-being.

Providing social support to others is prosocial behavior. From the perspective of biological evolution, providing support to others is an instinctive behavior with adaptive significance. According to the self-motivation effect of altruism in the evolutionary process [22], giving emotional support can enable helpers to adjust their psychological resources to engender positive influence. Therefore, from an evolutionary point of view, providing emotional support, such as comfort to others, can also help individuals obtain psychological benefits. The increase in positive emotions, reduction in negative emotions, and improvement in life satisfaction are likely important results of social support provision. In addition, social exchange theory believes that human behavior is dominated by exchange activities that can bring rewards and follow the principle of reciprocity and equality; providing emotional support to others can improve their emotions and create positive emotions and life satisfaction [23].

From a psychological point of view, compared with receiving social support, the behavior of giving social support is an autonomous behavior, which itself has the effect of enhancing well-being. The model of longitudinal well-being posited that intentional engagement could trigger increasing and lasting well-being than life circumstances [24]. In other words, individuals can obtain more happiness through activities toward others as a “positive happiness maker” but not as a “negative owner.” Moreover, the interdependence theory suggests that individuals’ well-being is independent of the satisfaction of their own needs but dependent on the satisfaction of social needs, attributable to the establishment, maintenance, and enhancement of interpersonal relationships [25]. Providing social support to others refers to establishing deep interpersonal connections; thus, it is reasonable to infer that it could positively impact subjective well-being. Previous research has found that prosocial behaviors can significantly and positively predict life satisfaction and positive emotions [26] and negatively predict negative emotions [27]. Social support provision is one type of giving behavior. Therefore, this study hypothesized that social support provision could significantly predict individuals’ subjective well-being.

Research on the relationship between social support and the life satisfaction of the elderly found that social support can promote the elderly’s life satisfaction, especially for the elderly over 75 years of age, Because the elderly is more easily seen as weak and incompetent when the elderly provides social support to others, it can break stereotypes, enhance self-esteem, and thus have a positive effect on the individual [28]. Similarly, college students with low economic status are regarded as vulnerable groups and individuals who need help, and their positive giving behavior may also play a role in improving individual well-being. Based on this, this study assumes that the social support provision of college students with low socioeconomic status correlates significantly with subjective well-being.

Social support provision and psychological capital

The personal well-being model suggests that giving behaviors can enhance and improve self-evaluation and build self-psychological resources. However, the giving may be initiated by different goals and motivations [29]. Providing emotional support to others to overcome difficulties can help accumulate experience in dealing with stressful events and increase self-evaluations and perceived competence [30], mental resilience, and sense of value [31]. On the other hand, it can distract help-givers from focusing on their troubles and stress [32] and facilitates psychological adaption via the shift of internal standards, values, and the conceptualization of well-being [33]. After social support is given, individuals may be more resilient in the face of difficulties, have a more positive self-evaluation, have a more optimistic attitude towards success, and be full of hope for the future [34]. This state is consistent with the concept of psychological capital, defined as a positive psychological state composed of self-efficacy, hope, optimism, and resilience [35]. Individuals with high psychological capital rate success more positively and perceive themselves as possessing sense of control and resilience to setbacks [36]. In short, the provision of care to others increases psychological capital. Thus, it can be inferred that social support provision may promotes individuals’ psychological capital.

Sense of indebtedness as a moderator

Although studies have confirmed the positive effect of social support provision, some studies have found that giving does not positively impact the giver [37]; giving to others may sometimes burden the provider. A study on family caregivers finds that more caregiving was linked to lower life satisfaction [38]. It can be seen that there are certain conditions for giving behaviors to produce benefits. As mentioned earlier, the self-determination theory emphasizes that the autonomy of individual behavior is an essential condition for behaviors to have positive effects. This means that behaviors must be voluntary and freely chosen [39]. Additionally, according to the hypothesis proposed by Inagaki and Orehek [40], freedom of giving is necessary for social support provision to generate benefits. Existing studies have also found that being forced to participate in prosocial behaviors may be counterproductive and have adverse effects [41].

A sense of indebtedness is the obligation to repay another, which is often a relatively stable and lasting emotional state after receiving support from others and society [42, 43]. Different individuals differ significantly in their sense of indebtedness after receiving [44, 45]. The greater the state of indebtedness, the greater the pressure to compel individuals to provide feedback to others [46] to seek a balance in social interaction and achieve the goal of mutual benefit, equality, and fairness [47]. The level of the sense of indebtedness may vary after receiving a national bursary, so there must be significant differences in the sense of pressure to repay, and there are also great differences in the level of autonomy of social support given. For college students with a high sense of indebtedness, the autonomy of giving behaviors decreases and turns into stressful behaviors, so the benefits of giving behaviors may decrease or even disappear. However, college students with low indebtedness also receive the same national bursary. They have a lower pressure to repay, so they have a higher degree of autonomy in giving behavior, and their giving behavior can still produce benefits. From this point of view, although the individual initiates social support, the benefit of giving may be affected by the sense of indebtedness. Whether the degree of the sense of indebtedness, which is induced by receiving state financial aid, could limit the benefits generated by social support provision needs to be further explored.

The current study

This study aims to examine the association between social support provision, sense of indebtedness, psychological capital, and subjective well-being of undergraduates with low socioeconomic status. The regression model was employed to interpret the following questions: (a) Can social support provision be positively associated with psychological capital and subjective well-being? (b) can sense of indebtedness weaken the benefit of social support provision?

Materials and methods

Participants

The college students who received state financial aid were taken as the participants. College students with an annual family income of less than 3,000 yuan can apply for state financial aid. Students apply to their department in the first month of enrollment, and the student management office verifies the application. After the verification is passed, it will be included in the school’s poor student database. The database covers all impoverished college students receiving financial aid. The students selected for this study are from this database.

The researcher sent survey requests to the student management offices of 6 colleges and universities in the Midwest, and four responded to our request. The researcher informs student administrators of respondent eligibility criteria to ensure that the number of students in each grade and gender is relatively balanced. After finalizing the list of participants, the student management office sent survey requests to the assistant of each faculty and got approval from them. Then the researchers conducted investigation training through online meetings. The assistant investigated at the weekly class meeting. The teacher first explained the requirements of the investigation, provided informed consent, and emphasized the voluntary nature and anonymity of the investigation. After scanning the QR code, students fill out the questionnaire and get 2-yuan or 20-yuan coupon rewards. Due to the use of an online platform (Questionnaire Star), the platform required that all questions be answered before submitting or dropping out. No students dropped out of the survey. The researcher aggregates the data collected.

A total of 800 questionnaires were returned, and 68 were excluded because they did not meet the selection criteria, which provided the incorrect answer to the following question: “What is the capital of China” (Beijing). This question was included to determine whether the participants had taken the questionnaire randomly. The chi-square test results show that there is no significant difference in gender (χ2(1,800) = 0.29, p > 0.05) and age (χ2(5,800) = 0.41, p > 0.05) between the excluded participants and the final sample, indicating that there is no structural missing. Finally, 732 valid questionnaires were collected: 336 (45.9%) participants were male, and 396 (54.1%) were female; 203 first-year students, 196 sophomores, 176 juniors, and 157 seniors; and their ages ranged from 18 to 23.

Measures

Social support provision questionnaire

The emotional social support provision scale [48] was adopted in this study. The confirmatory factor analysis was performed for the Chinese revision, and the model fit well [49]. The scale has three items, each rated on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = never, 5 = always). A higher score indicates more emotional social support provided to others. Cronbach’s α of the emotional social support provision scale in the present study was 0.81.

Sense of indebtedness questionnaire

Sense of indebtedness was measured by the Indebtedness Questionnaire [16, 50]. The result of CFA for the Chinese version fits well: χ2/df = 4.932, GFI = 0.951, TLI = 0.934, CFI = 0.961, RMSEA = 0.076, SRMR = 0.035. The scale consisted of five items. The scale was rated on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree, 5 = strongly agree). The higher the total score, the greater the sense of indebtedness a person feels. The Cronbach’s α of the Indebtedness Questionnaire in the present study was 0.86.

Psychological capital questionnaire

Psychological capital was measured by the Psychological Capital Questionnaire Chinese revision [51]. The model fit index in this study meets the requirements: χ2/df = 3.899, GFI = 0.915, TLI = 0.956, CFI = 0.985, RMSEA = 0.043, and SRMR = 0.032. The scale consists of 26 items, including four dimensions: self-efficacy, resilience, optimism, and hope. Each item was rated on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = never, 5 = always). The total score of psychological capital reflects the individual’s psychological capital status. The higher the score, the higher the level of positive psychological capital. The Cronbach’s α of the Psychological Capital Questionnaire in the present study was 0.91.

Subjective well-being questionnaire

Based on the theory of subjective well-being, Individuals with more life satisfaction and positive affect and less negative affect are happier [52]. Life satisfaction was measured by the satisfaction with life scale [52]. The Chinese version of life satisfaction was revised and validated by Xiong & Xu [53]. The model fits well in this study: χ2/df = 4.389, GFI = 0.978, TLI = 0.928, CFI = 0.964, RMSEA = 0.054, SRMR = 0.037. The 5-item scale was rated on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree, 5 = strongly agree), and the total score indicates the level of life satisfaction. The Cronbach’s α of life satisfaction in the present study was 0.81.

The Chinese version of the affect balance scale [54] was used to measure positive and negative affect. Six items were used to assess positive affect (Cronbach’s α = 0.84), and six were used to assess negative affect (Cronbach’s α = 0.84). The model fit of negative affect is as follows: χ2/df = 3.469, GFI = 0.965, TLI = 0.974, CFI = 0.985, RMSEA = 0.061, SMRR = 0.0252. The model fit of positive affect is as follows: χ2/df = 5.94, GFI = 0.975, TLI = 0.947, CFI = 0.968, RMSEA = 0.086, SRMR = 0.041. Each item was rated on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = never, 5 = always), and positive and negative affect were computed. A higher score on each scale means more positive/negative affect.

Procedure

This study was conducted in 2021 at four universities in Xi’an, Taiyuan, and Wuhan. Ethical approval was obtained from the researcher’s university ethics committee. Self-report questionnaires were completed after obtaining informed consent. Participants filled out questionnaires regarding the social support provision, sense of indebtedness, psychological capital, and subjective well-being. It took 10–15 min to complete the questionnaires. The participants were free to withdraw from the study at any time. They did not place their names on the measures, and the confidentiality of their responses was assured.

Data analysis

First, the descriptive analysis of observed variables was performed by IBM SPSS Statistics 25. Second, a multiple regression model was conducted by Mplus 8.3 to determine the association between social support provision, psychological capital, and subjective well-being. In this analysis process, the research variables are converted into latent variables before incorporating them and control variables into the model. Third, the interaction between social support provision and the sense of indebtedness was entered into the regression model to test the moderated effect of the sense of indebtedness.

Results

Common method variance test

The pre-procedure and post-statistics were used to control and test the possible common method variance. The study found that the tendency to pursue consistent responses is one of the sources of common method variance [55]. Therefore, different response words were used in the questionnaires. Some questionnaires used 1 = strongly disagree, 5 = strongly agree, and some used 1 = never, 5 = always. To eliminate the concerns of the participants, all scales were conducted anonymously. The collection of demographic information was placed at the end of the questionnaire. The Harman single-factor test was performed to evaluate the common method variance. There were nine factors with eigenvalues greater than 1, and the variance explanation rate of the first factor was 29.906%, which was less than the recommended value of 40% [55]. Using Mplus 8.3 to perform confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) on the single-factor model, the results showed that the model fitting index did not meet the requirements (\(\chi\) 2/df = 5.449, CFI = 0.481, TLI = 0.473, RMSEA = 0.173, SRMR = 0.132). Moreover, the unmeasured latent method factor (ULMC) was used to further test for common method variance. First, construct a model loading the items on their corresponding constructs (model1:χ2/df = 3.345, CFI = 0.918, TLI = 0.901, RMSEA = 0.059, SRMR = 0.064). Second, added a method factor to the model1(model2: χ2/df = 3.273, CFI = 0.926, TLI = 0.919, RMSEA = 0.047, SRMR = 0.051). Third, compared Model 2 with Model 1. The fit of model2 did not significantly improve compared to model1(ΔCFI = 0.008, ΔTLI = 0.018, ΔRMSEA = 0.012, ΔSRMR = 0.013). The above method has reduced the common method variance to a certain extent.

Descriptive statistics and correlations

Table 1 shows the mean, standard deviation, and correlation of the variables. The correlation analysis revealed that social support provision was positively related to psychological capital (r = 0.344, p < 0.01), life satisfaction (r = 0.293, p < 0.01), and positive affect (r = 0.348, p < 0.01); psychological capital is positively related to life satisfaction (r = 0.536, p < 0.001) and positive affect (r = 0.607, p < 0.001); social support provision is negatively related to negative affect (r =  − 0.13, p < 0.001); psychological capital is negatively correlated with negative affect (r =  − 0.243, p < 0.01) and sense of indebtedness (r =  − 0.141, p < 0.01); and the sense of indebtedness is positively correlated with negative affect (r = 0.247, p < 0.01).

Table 1 Descriptive statistics and correlations

Testing for the effect of social support provision on psychological capital and subjective well-being

To examine the effect of social support provision on psychological capital and subjective well-being, the regression model was tested by using Mplus 8.3. First, take age and gender as the predictors, psychological capital, positive affect, negative affect, and life satisfaction as outcomes. The relationship between gender, age, and the outcome variables was insignificant. Second, take age and gender, social support provision, and sense of indebtedness as the predictors, psychological capital, positive affect, negative affect, and life satisfaction as outcomes. The model fitted the data well (χ2/df = 2.665, CFI = 0.928, TIL = 0.916, RMSEA = 0.05, SRMR = 0.044).The result showed that the social support provision was positively associated with psychological capital(b = 0.466, t = 11.418, p < 0.001, Cohen’s ƒ2 = 0.286), life satisfaction(b = 0.317, t = 6.607, p < 0.001, Cohen’s ƒ2 = 0.114) and positive affect(b = 0.388, t = 9.481, p < 0 0.001, Cohen’s ƒ2 = 0.191), negatively associated with negative affect(b = -0.173, t = 3.748, p < 0.01, Cohen’s ƒ2 = 0.173), The results are presented in Table 2.

Table 2 The effect of social support provision

Testing for the moderated effect of sense of indebtedness

The study assumes that sense of indebtedness weakens the association between social support provision, psychological capital and subjective well-being. To verify the moderating effect of sense of indebtedness, take the age, gender, social support provision, sense of indebtedness, and the interaction between social support provision and the sense of indebtedness as predictors, psychological capital, positive affect, negative affect, life satisfaction as outcomes. The regression result found that the interaction between social support provision and the sense of indebtedness was significantly associated with psychological capital(b = -0.091, t = -2.144, p < 0.05, Cohen’s ƒ2 = 0.03), life satisfaction(b = -0.103, t = -2.165, p < 0.05, Cohen’s ƒ2 = 0.08), positive affect(b = -0.142, t = -3.078, p < 0.01, Cohen’s ƒ2 = 0.119), but not significantly associated with the negative affect(b = -0.005, t = 0.115, p > 0.05, Cohen’s ƒ2 = 0.01). The details are presented in Table 3.

Table 3 The moderated effect of sense of indebtedness

To clarify the essence of the interaction between social support provision and the sense of indebtedness, following Aiken [56] and Hayes’s [57] recommendation, the average level of indebtedness + 1SD is defined as a high level of indebtedness and—1SD as a low level of indebtedness. As shown in Table 4, compared with individuals with high level of indebtedness, social support provision was associated with greater psychological capital, life satisfaction, and positive affect in individuals with low level of indebtedness. However, there is no significant difference in the association between social support provision and negative affect under different levels of sense of indebtedness.

Table 4 The moderated effect of indebtedness

The J-N technique is used to determine the interval of the moderated effect of sense of indebtedness. Figure 1 shows that when the value of sense of indebtedness is lower than 1.2 standard deviations above the mean, the relationship between social support provision and psychological capital is significant; Fig. 2 shows that when the value of sense of indebtedness is lower than 0.82 standard deviations above the mean, social support provision has a significant relationship with life satisfaction; Fig. 3 shows that when the value of sense of indebtedness is lower than 0.95 standard deviations above the mean, social support provision has a significant relationship with positive affect. Overall, with the increase in the sense of indebtedness, the relationships between social support provision and outcome variables are weakening.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Effect of SSP on PSYCAP conditional on INDEB

Fig. 2
figure 2

Effect of SSP on LS conditional on INDEB

Fig. 3
figure 3

Effect of SSP on PA conditional on INDEB

Discussion

The results confirmed a significant association between social support provision and psychological capital and subjective well-being, and the sense of indebtedness limits the relation.

Social support provision and subjective well-being

Numerous studies have confirmed the positive predictive effect of social support on subjective well-being. This study revealed that social support provision could be significantly associated with subjective well-being. The results verified the self-determination theory and positive activities model [30], consistent with the studies about the psychological benefits of prosocial behaviors such as volunteering and donating [58, 59]. College students with low socioeconomic status provide emotional support to others rather than monetary and instrumental support, which also benefits their subjective well-being. Additionally, the study confirmed the model of longitudinal well-being [24]. This study indicated that social support provision, as an intentional positive act on relationships, can engender the provider's well-being. This reminds us that providing resources for individuals is not the only choice to improve individuals’ subjective well-being. We can try to guide individuals to actively construct their well-being, such as providing emotional support to others.

The social support provision and psychological capital

The personal well-being model suggests that giving behavior has the function of enhancing psychological resources [60], even kindness meditation can build personal resources [61]. This study confirmed that giving emotional support to others to help them cope with their difficulties can provide experience in dealing with stressful events and improve self-competence and resilience. These acts can build a robust social support system to cope with potential difficulties so individuals can be more optimistic. In short, social support provision is significantly associated with individuals’ psychological capital.

The moderated effect of indebtedness

This study found that providing emotional support to others by college students with low socioeconomic status benefits the construction of individuals’ psychological resources and the acquisition of subjective well-being. However, there is a specific restriction on the positive effect of social support provision. Impoverished college students have a sense of indebtedness after receiving the state’s aid, which induces repayment-style giving, which is forced giving essentially, causing the benefits of social support provision to reduce, validating the self-determination theory [62]. Involuntary behavior does not positively impact the actor [63] and even negatively impacts the perpetrator due to detriment to autonomy [38, 64, 65]. The results are consistent with Inagaki’s hypothesis that social support provision becomes a burden when it lacks autonomy, and the benefits would be reduced. On the contrary, if the provision satisfies the individual’s autonomy needs, it can play a positive role.

Limitations and implications

First, the study adopted a cross-sectional design to explore the influence of social support provision on psychological capital and subjective well-being. However, the experimental design can lead to more convincing conclusions [66], and future research should adopt an experimental design. Second, the data collection for this research was done using a self-report method, which entails a certain degree of moral evaluation in providing social support to others. It may overestimate the social support given by oneself. Using a combination of self-reports and field experiments to measure the social support provision by individuals may more accurately decrease the influence of socially desirable responses. Third, the test and control for common method variance may not be sufficient. In terms of procedure remedy, the counterbalancing question order is only implemented at the school level and not at the participants' level, and anonymization is used to eliminate the evaluation apprehension of the participants, but the effect has not been thoroughly evaluated; In terms of statistical remedy, CFA Marker Technique needs to set a marker in the questionnaire, the study did not set markers in advance, so the technique has not been implemented. Fourth, take undergraduates who receive national financial aid as research subjects, and their sense of indebtedness is generated by receiving financial support. However, this study does not consider other types of support, such as emotional support, in which the sense of indebtedness is not as strong as receiving financial support. Therefore, future research should determine the cause of indebtedness to examine its influence on the relationship between social support provision and subjective well-being.

Despite the shortcomings mentioned above, the results of this study have specific implications. From a theoretical perspective, this study focuses on the benefit of social support provision, which expands the field of social support research and verifies the hypothesis that social support provision plays a positive role in individuals’ development, such as psychological capital and well-being; from a practical perspective, social support provision can improve psychological capital and subjective well-being. Therefore, it is not enough to provide social support to improve mental health and promote the subjective well-being of marginalized groups. One should also guide them to contribute to others and society. In addition, the positive effect of support provision is restricted by the individual’s sense of indebtedness, so when guiding individuals to provide social support, they should be given freedom, and it should satisfy their need for autonomy.

Conclusion

The study explores the relationship between social support provision, sense of indebtedness and psychological capital, and subjective well-being. The results prove that social support provision is significantly related to subjective well-being and psychological capital. The association is affected by the individual’s sense of indebtedness; college students with low sense of indebtedness can benefit more from social support provision.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

SSP:

Social Support Provision

PSYCAP:

Psychological Capital

LS:

Life Satisfaction

PA:

Positive Affect

NA:

Negative Affect

INDEB:

Sense of Indebtedness

References

  1. Eisenberger NI. An empirical review of the neural underpinnings of receiving and giving social support: implications for health. Psychosom Med. 2013;75(6):545.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. Lee CYS, Dik B. Associations among stress, gender, sources of social support, and health in emerging adults. Stress Health. 2017;33(4):378–88.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Kong F, Gong X, Sajjad S, Yang K, Zhao J. How is emotional intelligence linked to life satisfaction? The mediating role of social support, positive affect and negative affect. J Happiness Stud. 2019;20(8):2733–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Saltzman LY, Hansel TC, Bordnick PS. Loneliness, isolation, and social support factors in post-COVID-19 mental health. Psychol Trauma Theory Res Pract Policy. 2020;12(S1):S55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Xin Z. Perceived social support and college student engagement: moderating effects of a grateful disposition on the satisfaction of basic psychological needs as a mediator. BMC Psychol. 2022;10(1):1–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Poudel A, Gurung B, Khanal GP. Perceived social support and psychological well-being among Nepalese adolescents: the mediating role of self-esteem. BMC Psychol. 2020;8(1):1–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Inagaki TK, Meyer ML. Individual differences in resting-state connectivity and giving social support: implications for health. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2020;15(10):1076–85.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Chen E, Lam PH, Finegood ED, Turiano NA, Mroczek DK, Miller GE. The balance of giving versus receiving social support and all-cause mortality in a US national sample. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2021;118(24):e2024770118.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Preston A, Rew L, Young CC. A systematic scoping review of psychological capital related to mental health in youth. J Sch Nurs. 2023;39(1):72–86.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Diener E, editor. Assessing well-being: The collected works of Ed Diener. Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands; 2009.

  11. Proulx CM, Helms HM, Buehler C. Marital quality and personal well-being: a meta-analysis. J Marriage Fam. 2007;69(3):576–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Baumeister RF, Leary MR. The need to belong: desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychol Bull. 1995;117(3):497.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Kuang Y, Wang F, Wang ZJ. Social class and children’s prosociality: a study in the context of China’s dual urban–rural structure. Soc Psychol Pers Sci. 2021;12(1):63–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Willroth EC, Atherton OE, Robins RW. Life satisfaction trajectories during adolescence and the transition to young adulthood: findings from a longitudinal study of Mexican-origin youth. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2021;120(1):192.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Jia X, Zhu H, Sun G, Meng H, Zhao Y. Socioeconomic status and risk-taking behavior among Chinese adolescents: the mediating role of psychological capital and self-control. Front Psychol. 2021;12:760968.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Naito T, Sakata Y. Gratitude, indebtedness, and regret on receiving a friend’s favor in Japan. Psychologia. 2010;53(3):179–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Callaghan P, Morrissey J. Social support and health: a review. J Adv Nurs. 1993;18(2):203–10.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Brown SL, Nesse RM, Vinokur AD, Smith DM. Providing social support may be more beneficial than receiving it: Results from a prospective study of mortality. Psychol Sci. 2003;14(4):320–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Gottlieb BH, Bergen AE. Social support concepts and measures. J Psychosom Res. 2010;69(5):511–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Brown SL. An altruistic reanalysis of the social support hypothesis: the health benefits of giving. New Dir Philanthr Fundraising. 2003;42:49–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Konrath S, Brown S. The effects of giving on givers. In Health and social relationships: The good, the bad, and the complicated. WA: American Psychological Association; 2013.

  22. Dunn EW, Whillans AV, Norton MI, Aknin LB. Prosocial spending and buying time: money as a tool for increasing subjective well-being. Adv Exp Soc Psychol. 2020;61:67–126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Curry OS, Rowland LA, van Lissa CJ, Zlotowitz S, McAlaney J, Whitehouse H. Happy to help? A systematic review and meta-analysis of the effects of performing acts of kindness on the well-being of the actor. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2018;76:320–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Sheldon KM, Lyubomirsky S. Achieving sustainable gains in happiness: change your actions, not your circumstances. J Happiness Stud. 2006;7(1):55–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Van Lange PA, Columbus S. Vitamin S: Why is social contact, even with strangers, so important to well-being? Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2021;30(3):267–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Hutchison P, Cox S, Frings D. Helping you helps me: Giving and receiving social support in recovery groups for problem gamblers. Group Dyn Theory Res Pract. 2018;22(4):187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Inagaki TK, Haltom KEB, Suzuki S, Jevtic I, Hornstein E, Bower JE, Eisenberger NI. The neurobiology of giving versus receiving support: the role of stress-related and social reward-related neural activity. Psychosom Med. 2016;78(4):443.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Lestari SK, et al. A longitudinal study on social support, social participation, and older Europeans’ Quality of life. SSM-Population Health. 2021;13:100747.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Thoits PA, Hewitt LN. Volunteer work and well-being. J Health Soc Behav. 2001;42:115–31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Lyubomirsky S, Layous K. How do simple positive activities increase well-being? Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2013;22:57–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Krause N, Shaw BA. Giving social support to others, socioeconomic status, and changes in self-esteem in late life. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2000;55(6):S323–33.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Schwartz C, Meisenhelder JB, Ma Y, Reed G. Altruistic social interest behaviors are associated with better mental health. Psychosom Med. 2003;65(5):778–85.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Schwartz CE, Sendor RM. Helping others helps oneself: response shift effects in peer support. Soc Sci Med. 1999;48(11):1563–75.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Gruenewald TL, Liao DH, Seeman TE. Contributing to others, contributing to oneself: perceptions of generativity and health in later life. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2012;67:660–5.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Luthans F, Youssef CM, Avolio BJ. Psychological capital: Developing the human competitive edge. Oxford: Oxford Press; 2007.

  36. Youssef-Morgan CM, Luthans F. Psychological capital theory: Toward a positive holistic model. Bingley: Emerald Group Publishing Limited; 2013.

  37. Caprariello PA, Reis HT. “This one’s on me!”: Differential well-being effects of self-centered and recipient-centered motives for spending money on others. Motiv Emot. 2021;45:705–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Maguire R, Hanly P, Maguire P. Beyond care burden: associations between positive psychological appraisals and well-being among informal caregivers in Europe. Qual Life Res. 2019;28:2135–46.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Yu S, Levesque-Bristol C, Maeda Y. General need for autonomy and subjective well-being: a meta-analysis of studies in the US and East Asia. J Happiness Stud. 2018;19(6):1863–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Inagaki TK, Orehek E. On the benefits of giving social support: when, why, and how support providers gain by caring for others. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2017;26(2):109–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. van Schie S, Güntert ST, Oostlander J, Wehner T. How the organizational context impacts volunteers: a differentiated perspective on self-determined motivation. Int J Volunt Nonprofit Org. 2015;26(4):1570–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Greenberg MS. A theory of indebtedness. In: Social exchange. Boston: Springer; 1980. p. 3–26.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  43. Gergen K, ed. Social exchange: Advances in theory and research. NY: Springer Science & Business Media; 2012.

  44. Hitokoto H. Indebtedness in cultural context: The role of culture in the felt obligation to reciprocate. Asian J Soc Psychol. 2016;19(1):16–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Kim JH, Lim J. Gratitude and indebtedness: associations with personality traits and life satisfaction in South Korea. Asian J Soc Psychol. 2022;25(3):577–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Zheng W, Guo Q, Huang T, Lu J, Xie C. The prosocial outgrowth of filial beliefs in different cultures: a conditional mediation model analysis. Front Psychol. 2021;12:4824.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Peng C, Nelissen RMA, Zeelenberg M. Reconsidering the roles of gratitude and indebtedness in social exchange. Cogn Emot. 2018;32(4):760–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Shakespeare-Finch J, Obst PL. The development of the 2-way social support scale: a measure of giving and receiving emotional and Instrumental support. J Pers Assess. 2011;93(5):483–90.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Liu T. Social Support of Higher Vocational Students: Relationships and Interactions between Receiving Support and Providing Support. Nan Jing: Nan Jing normal university; 2012. In Chinese.

  50. Fu J. The Influencing factor of college students’ gratitude and indebtedness: research in the context of aiding impoverished students in higher education. High Educ Forum. 2016;2:9–28. [In Chinese].

    Google Scholar 

  51. Zhang K, Zhang S, Dong YH. Positive psychological capital: measurement and its association with mental health. Stud Psychol Behav. 2010;8(01):58–64. [In Chinese].

    Google Scholar 

  52. Diener E. Assessing subjective well-being: progress and opportunities. Soc Indic Res. 1994;31(2):103–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Xiong C, Xu Y. Reliability and validity of the Chinese version of the life satisfaction scale in the public. Chin J Health Psychol. 2009;17(08):948–9. [In Chinese].

    Google Scholar 

  54. Chen WF, Zhang JX. Factorial and construct validity of the Chinese positive and negative affect scale for student. Chin Ment Health J. 2004;18(11):763–5. [In Chinese].

    Google Scholar 

  55. Podsakoff PM, MacKenzie SB, Lee JY, Podsakoff NP. Common method biases in behavioral research: a critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. J Appl Psychol. 2003;88(5):879.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Aiken LS, West SG, Reno RR. Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. CA: Sage; 1991.

  57. Hayes AF, Matthes J. Computational procedures for probing interactions in OLS and logistic regression: SPSS and SAS implementations. Behav Res Methods. 2009;41(3):924–36.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Lee MA. Volunteering and happiness: Examining the differential effects of volunteering types according to household income. J Happiness Stud. 2019;20(3):795–814.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Kushlev K, Radosic N, Diener E. Subjective well-being and prosociality around the globe: Happy people give more of their time and money to others. Social Psychol Pers Sci. 2022;13(4):849–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Finch J, Farrell LJ, Waters AM. Searching for the HERO in youth: Does psychological capital predict mental health symptoms and subjective well-being in Australian school-aged children and adolescents? Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2020;51(6):1025–36.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  61. Fredrickson BL, Cohn MA, Coffey KA, Pek J, Finkel SM. Open hearts build lives: positive emotions, induced through loving-kindness meditation, build consequential personal resources. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2008;95(5):1045.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  62. Ryan RM, Deci EL. Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. Am Psychol. 2000;55(1):68.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Hui BP, Ng JC, Berzaghi E, Cunningham-Amos LA, Kogan A. Rewards of kindness? A meta-analysis of the link between prosociality and well-being. Psychol Bull. 2020;146(12):1084.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Rootenberg M. Doing good when you don’t want to: an experience sampling study examining autonomy and compassionate action. 2021.

    Google Scholar 

  65. Aknin LB, Wiwad D, Girme YU. Not all gifts are good: the potential practical costs of motivated gifts. J Appl Soc Psychol. 2019;49(2):75–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  66. Stone-Romero EF, Rosopa PJ. The relative validity of inferences about mediation as a function of research design characteristics. Organ Res Methods. 2008;11(2):326–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

I’m grateful to all the participants for giving their time and participating willing.

Funding

The research design, analysis, interpretation of data, and writing the manuscript was funding by Social Science Foundation of Shaanxi Province (Grant No: 2019Q026), and the Science Foundation of Shaanxi Xue Qian Normal University (Grant No:2021YBRS19), the data collection was funding by the Teaching Reform Project of Shaanxi Xue Qian Normal University (Grant No:19JG003Y;22JG007Y), and the Youth Innovation Team of Shaanxi Universities [Grant No:20JZ035; 22JP016]. The funding agencies did not interfere with the research design and collection, analysis, interpretation of data, or writing the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Zhongyi Xin: Conceptualization, Investigation, Formal analysis, Writing-original draft, Writing-review & editing.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Zhongyi Xin.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The authors confirm that all the methods comply with current guidelines and regulations that follow the Declaration of Helsinki. Ethical approval was obtained from the academic and moral committee of Shaanxi Xue Qian Normal University (approved no: 202010023). Informed consent was obtained from all participants.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The author declares he has no conflict of interest.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Xin, Z. The association between social support provision, psychological capital, subjective well-being and sense of indebtedness among undergraduates with low socioeconomic status. BMC Psychol 11, 291 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-023-01325-w

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-023-01325-w

Keywords