Skip to main content

Exploring the indirect effect of loneliness in the association between problematic use of social networks and cognitive function in Lebanese adolescents

Abstract

Background

Problematic use of social networks is a widespread problem that may exert deleterious impacts on cognitive functions. Moreover, studies have added an important link between loneliness and its harmful effect on cognitive functions. Other studies have also revealed that problematic use of social networks among teenagers has a pejorative influence on their social interactions, leading to increased social isolation. Therefore, the goal of our research was to investigate the link between problematic use of social networks and cognitive function in a group of Lebanese adolescents while also taking into consideration the indirect role of loneliness in this relationship.

Methods

This cross-sectional study, which was carried out between January and April 2022, included 379 teenagers (aged between 13 and 17 years), from all Lebanese governorates. The PROCESS SPSS Macro version 3.4, model four was used to compute three pathways. Pathway A determined the regression coefficient for the effect of problematic use of social networks on loneliness; Pathway B examined the association between loneliness and cognitive function, and Pathway C’ estimated the direct effect of problematic use of social networks on cognitive function.

Results

Higher negative social comparison, addictive consequences of problematic use of social networks, and loneliness were significantly associated with worse cognitive function. Loneliness mediated the association between negative social comparison and worse cognitive function, as well as between addictive consequences of problematic use of social networks and worse cognitive function. In addition, higher financial burden was significantly correlated with worse cognitive function, whereas higher physical activity was related to better cognitive function.

Conclusion

In sum, the current study supports that problematic use of social networks is negatively associated with adolescents’ cognitive function, where loneliness seems to play a pivotal role in this equation. The results thus endorse the importance of helping Lebanese adolescents to overcome problematic use of social networks and recover from their loneliness, to achieve a better cognitive/academic performance.

Peer Review reports

Introduction

By definition, cognitive functions are the capacities of our brain that allow us in particular to interact with our environment [1]. As such, cognitive function is a broad term that refers to mental processes involved in acquisition of knowledge, manipulation of information, and reasoning [2]. Precisely, the DSM-5 (Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-Fifth Edition) characterizes six critical domains of cognitive functions, such as perceptual-motor function, language, executive function, complex attention, social cognition, and learning/memory [3].

Numerous extrinsic factors may however interfere with individuals’ cognitive abilities, causing deteriorations of these functions. For instance, one of these factors is the problematic use of social networks, which has been conceptually defined as a disorder that is not related to a psychoactive substance ingestion while it shares qualities related to a behavioral addiction [4]. Problematic use of social networks encompasses both addiction-related repercussions of social networks use and how these networks may be negatively used in a comparative fashion, without focus on a specific social network [5]. It is considered a widespread problem that may exert deleterious impacts on cognitive functions, such as memory, executive control, visual and auditory attention, and self-control [6]. Indeed, smartphones are a new generation of phones combining communication and entertainment capabilities; they have become so common that the number of mobile cellular subscriptions reached almost 6 billion worldwide in 2020 [7]. Furthermore, 35% of teenagers are major users of electronic media, having online interaction with virtual individuals constantly and throughout the day [8]. With the rapid increase in their use, a new type of health condition known as social media abuse/misuse has recently emerged as a serious public health issue among teenagers [9].

In addition, the relationship between negative social comparisons on social networks and cognitive functions needs to be accounted for [10, 11]. Namely, the temptation to utilize other people as data sources to evaluate one’s own talents and ways of behaving, thinking, or feeling has been termed as “social comparison” [12]. Depending on who the person compares himself/herself to, this comparison might have favorable or bad implications. If the person compares himself/herself to someone regarded as inferior (i.e., downward comparison [13]), his/her self-esteem is promoted; however, when comparing himself/herself to someone considered better (i.e., upward comparison), he/she feels down [14, 15]. In fact, the latter scenario is the classic situation on social networks, which is acknowledged to have aversive effects on mental health [16,17,18,19]. Since negative social comparisons on social networks has been shown to be detrimental to mental health and well-being [14, 15], it is conceivable that this condition may pejoratively affect adolescents’ cognitive functioning by provoking anger, envy, and depression [14, 15], and thus, diverting their focus from properly executing cognitive tasks. However, to date, no empirical analysis has examined this hypothesis.

Moreover, according to several studies [6, 20, 21], addictive consequences of problematic use of social networks (PUSN) are linked to impaired cognitive functions [22,23,24], which can be explained by decreased productivity, communication, and compromise of interactions with others/or new relationships with peers. Research has indeed found that excessive use of social networks in teenagers is likely to be accompanied by significant psychopathological issues, such as cognitive-emotional dysregulation, cognitive impairments, behavioral issues, somatic symptoms, attention deficiencies, depression, and impulsivity [20, 21]. Furthermore, some research has found a clear link between anxiety and ADHD symptoms and pejorative use of social networks [25, 26]; this relationship was explained by the fact that the continuous stimulation provided by social networking sites drastically diminished attention.

On the other hand, solitude and cognitive impairment are two of the factors linked to smartphone addiction [25]. Loneliness is a negative emotion associated with a perceived absence of social connections. A spectrum of serious repercussions on mental and physical health can result from loneliness, which incorporates subjective rather than objective social isolation [27]. Studies have added an important link between loneliness and its harmful effect on cognitive functions [28], explained by the heavy impact of depression, deterioration of acquired skills, and involution of brain development. Other research [29] has revealed that problematic use of social networks among teenagers has a pejorative influence on their social interactions, leading to increased social isolation [30]. In reality, technology has been successfully proved to push people into a condition of false loneliness, according to the literature [28]. As such, there was a link between problematic use of social networks and social isolation, which was explained by the amount of time spent on social networks and the detachment from parents/peers (e.g., lack of communication with parents, constant solitude in their rooms, etc.). Despite having access to technology and virtual connections, the individual is driven into an acute condition of loneliness [31]. In general, the negative repercussions of excessive use of social networks include symptoms such as withdrawal and decreased user productivity, social interactions, physical health, or emotional well-being in everyday life [6]. Thus, there is a possibility that loneliness could be part of the association between problematic use of social networks and cognitive function among adolescents. Moreover, during their quarantine, a big group of adolescents used their cellphones for online classes, also complaining about poor communication/contact with their teachers, which might have influenced their learning/cognitive abilities [32].

Adolescence is certainly an interesting and essential period to study; since cognitive development corresponds to the physiological changes that enable people to think and learn, there is a considerable change during this phase in terms of mentality, decision-making, the construction of connections with parents/siblings, and engagement with the environment [33]. Adolescents are also considered to be more susceptible to developing social media misuse than other age groups, as they seem to be more exposed to technology, including smartphones, online classrooms, and social networks [34]. Consequently, they are more likely to experience problematic use of social networks, social isolation, and cognitive dysfunction.

When it comes to Lebanon, the prevalence of problematic social media use was shown to be 20.2% within the adult population, specifically in young adults aged between 18 and 34 years [35]. Studies have successfully added a better understanding of the high smartphone addiction rate among Lebanese young adults, as well as the impact of smartphone addiction on their self-esteem and temperaments (i.e., mood disorders) [36]. However, there are actually no clear studies in Lebanon that illustrate the association of problematic use of social networks with Lebanese adolescents’ cognitive function. Furthermore, now is the most crucial time to look into this matter because of the COVID-19 pandemic [32]. As a result, there is a pressing need to shed light on the link between problematic use of social networks, cognitive function, and loneliness among adolescents. Consequently, the goal of our research was to investigate the link between problematic use of social networks and cognitive function in a group of Lebanese adolescents, while also taking into consideration the indirect role of loneliness in this relationship. We hypothesized that problematic use of social networks and loneliness would be associated with worse cognitive function among these adolescents; additionally, we expected that loneliness would mediate the relationship between the two components of problematic use of social networks (i.e., negative social comparisons and addictive consequences) and worse cognitive function. In addition, research has successfully shown the influence of sociodemographic variables and personal factors such as the body mass index and physical activity on the relationship between the use of social networks and adolescents’ cognitive functions [37, 38]. To this end, we also sought to control for these confounding variables within this study.

Methods

Study design and participants

This cross-sectional study, which was carried out between January and April 2022, included 379 teenagers (aged between 13 and 17 years) who were currently residing in Lebanon and came from every governorate in Lebanon (Beirut, Mount Lebanon, North, South, and Bekaa). The snowball method was used to recruit our sample, Google Forms was used to make a soft copy of the questionnaire, and an online approach was developed to continue with the data collection process. Prior to their participation, participants received online instructions on how to complete the questionnaire as well as the primary aims and objectives of the study. Afterwards, the original participants were requested to find new participants from their networks who were within the same age range and as diverse as feasible in terms of place of residence throughout the Lebanese governorates. There were no remunerative rewards for taking part.

Minimal sample size calculation

Using the formula proposed by Fritz and MacKinnon [39], to calculate the sample size, n = L/f2 + k + 1, where f = 0.26 for a small to moderate effect size, L = 7.85 for an error of 5%, power = 80%, and taking 10 variables to be included in the model, a minimum sample of 127 was considered required.

Questionnaire

A questionnaire in Arabic was sent as a Google form link via social media networks. It needed 7–10 min to be filled. It contained sociodemographic information about the participants (age, sex, and governorate of residence). Following the World Health Organization rules of calculation, current self-report weight and height were collected to calculate the Body Mass Index (BMI) [40]. The ratio of the number of people living in the house to the number of rooms in it, known as the household crowding index, was calculated. It reflects the socioeconomic standing of the family [41]. The multiplication of daily exercise frequency, intensity, and duration yielded the physical activity index [42]. On a scale of 1 to 10, with 10 denoting extreme pressure, respondents were asked to rate their feelings of financial strain in relation to their general personal financial condition, answering the following question: “How much pressure do you feel with regard to your personal financial situation in general?”. The second part included the scales used in the current research:

Problematic use of Social Networks (PUS) questionnaire

The PUS is a self-report instrument, examining the potential addiction-related repercussions of Social Networks (SNs) use and concentrating on how SNs are used in a comparative way with no restraint to a particular social network, which allows generalization to multiple SNs [5]. The tool is made of 18 Likert-type questions with five response options ranging from 1 = “completely disagree” to 5 = “completely agree”. Statements are categorized into two subscales: 8 questions for addictive consequences of SNs (e.g., “Using social networks, I lose track of time and ignore important tasks I have outstanding”) and 10 questions for negative social comparisons (e.g., “When I see content from influencers or celebrities, I feel inferior”). Greater scores indicate potentiated problematic use of SNs in both domains. In the current study, αCronbach = 0.95 for negative social comparison and αCronbach = 0.90 for addictive consequences.

Jong-Gierveld loneliness scale

We assessed subjective loneliness by the modified version of the Jong-Gierveld Loneliness Scale, made of 5 questions (e.g., “I experience a general sense of emptiness”; “I miss having people around”) [43, 44]. A score of 1 is given for a “yes” response and a score of 0 for a “no” answer. Higher ratings reflect increased loneliness. In the current study, αCronbach = 0.76. This scale is validated in Lebanon [45].

Cognitive Functioning Self-Assessment Scale (CFSS)

Participants were asked to estimate the frequency of each of the 18 statements on a five-point scale with the “never” and “always” anchors during the course of the previous year. Examples of the 18 statements including “Difficulty in performing two tasks simultaneously” and “Difficulty in performing mental calculation” [46]. Higher scores define lower cognitive function. In this study, αCronbach = 0.95.

Translation Procedure

On all scales, the forward and backward translation approach was used. A Lebanese translator who had no connection to the study translated the English text from English into Arabic. The Arabic version was then translated back into English by a Lebanese psychologist who is fully functional in English. To identify and then get rid of any inconsistencies, the first and second English versions were compared.

Statistical analysis

SPSS software version 23 was used to conduct data analysis. Cronbach’s alpha values were computed for each scale and subscale. We had no missing data since all questions were required in the Google form. Cronbach’s alpha values were recorded for reliability analysis of all scales and subscales. The cognitive function score was normally distributed, with its skewness and kurtosis varying between − 1 and + 1 [47].The Student t and ANOVA tests were used to compare two and three or more means respectively, whereas the Pearson correlation test was used to compare two continuous variables. The PROCESS SPSS Macro version 3.4, model four [47] was used to calculate three pathways. Pathway A determined the regression coefficient for the effect of problematic use of social networks on loneliness; Pathway B examined the association between loneliness and cognitive function, and Pathway C’ estimated the direct effect of problematic use of social networks on cognitive function. An indirect effect was deemed significant if the bootstrapped 95% confidence intervals of the indirect pathway AB did not pass by zero. Variables that showed a p < 0.25 in the bivariate analysis were entered in the mediation model. Significance was set at a p < 0.05.

Results

Sociodemographic and other characteristics of the participants

A total of 379 adolescents participated in this study; their mean age was 16.07 ± 1.19 years, with 64.9% females. Table 1 summarizes other characteristics of the participants (Table 1).

Table 1 Sociodemographic and other characteristics of the participants (N = 379)

Bivariate analysis

Higher negative social comparison (r = 0.55), addictive consequences of problematic use of social networks (r = 0.49), loneliness (r = 0.45), and financial burden (r = 0.27) were significantly associated with worse cognitive function (i.e., higher cognitive function scores), whereas more physical activity (r=-0.24) was significantly associated with better cognitive function (i.e., lower cognitive function scores). No significant difference was found between males and females in terms of cognitive function (24.56 ± 15.70 vs. 25.66 ± 13.38; p = 0.493) (Table 2).

Table 2 Bivariate analysis of the continuous variables associated with cognitive function

Indirect effect analysis

Loneliness mediated the association between negative social comparison and worse cognitive function, and between addictive consequences of PUSN and worse cognitive function (Table 3; Figs. 1 and 2). Higher PUSN was significantly associated with more loneliness, which in turn was significantly associated with worse cognitive function (i.e., higher cognitive function scores). Finally, higher PUSN was directly significantly associated with worse cognitive function (i.e., higher cognitive function scores).

Table 3 Indirect effect analyses results, taking the problematic use of social networks subscales as independent variables, loneliness as the mediator and the cognitive function score as the dependent variable
Fig. 1
figure 1

(a) Relation between negative social comparison and loneliness (R2 = 29.80%); (b) Relation between loneliness and cognitive function (R2 = 37.77%); (c) Total effect of the relation between negative social comparison and cognitive function (R2 = 34.90%); (c’) Direct effect of the relation between negative social comparison and cognitive function. Numbers are displayed as regression coefficients (standard error). ***p < 0.001

Fig. 2
figure 2

(a) Relation between addictive consequences of problematic use of social networks and loneliness (R2 = 29.80%); (b) Relation between loneliness and cognitive function (R2 = 37.77%); (c) Total effect of the relation between addictive consequences of problematic use of social networks and cognitive function (R2 = 34.90%); (c’) Direct effect of the relation between addictive consequences of problematic use of social networks and cognitive function. Numbers are displayed as regression coefficients (standard error). ***p < 0.001

Discussion

Our study’s objective was to explore both direct and indirect relationships between problematic use of social networks and cognitive function among Lebanese adolescents, taking into account loneliness as a potential actor through the indirect pathway. Our analysis revealed that problematic use of social networks, reflected by addictive consequences and negative social comparison, was strictly associated with worse cognitive function in those adolescents. Moreover, loneliness was identified as a potential mediator of these relationships.

First, our results have indeed demonstrated the positive link between the addictive consequences of problematic use of social networks and inferior self-reported cognitive functioning in Lebanese adolescents. Certainly, by the second decade of life, the brain’s structure is considerably modulated, with a reduction of gray matter, increase in while matter, and thus long-term effects on human cognitive function [48,49,50]. Such structural modulations have actually shown remarkable dependency to external/environmental factors, such as traumatic childhood experiences or the child’s raising medium [51, 52]. Moreover, social media use disorders (specifically addictive consequences) have also been found to be critical in determining cognitive function among adolescents. To exemplify, Wang et al. revealed that adolescents who endorsed internet addiction had lower levels of gray matter across the bilateral cerebellum and a variety of brain regions, as well as alterations in cognitive control [50]. Additionally, through a functional neuroimaging study, Li et al. have evidenced that adolescents with addictive behaviors to internet suffered from impairments in frontal-basal ganglia connectivity, with those dysfunctions severely interfering with their cognitive ability to inhibit undesirable behaviors [53]. Consistently, Horowitz-Kraus and Hutton have discovered the presence of lower levels of functional brain connectivity in basically healthy children who had an increased time spent on screen-based media; specifically, increased exposure to screens was correlated with a diminished connectivity between the left visual word form area and brain areas responsible for language and cognitive regulation [54].

In addition, our study highlighted that negative social comparisons in relation to the content seen on social networks was significantly associated with worse cognitive function in Lebanese adolescents. In line with this perspective, it has previously been documented that social comparisons and depressive moods may exert a significant impact on adolescents’ social decision-making, which represent an important negative repercussion on their cognitive functioning [55]. As stated in the introduction, when comparing themselves to other people or celebrities on social networks, adolescents tend to feel down and unworthy [14, 15]; this negative comparison on social networks then yields deleterious effects on their mental health and abilities to concentrate on other important tasks.

Further, numerous studies have shown that problematic use of social networks among adolescents engenders a wide array of psychopathological symptoms, prominently loneliness [21, 56, 57]. On the other hand, loneliness and depressive symptoms appeared to be incriminated in lowering cognitive function among older adults [58,59,60], with a scarcity of data covering this relationship among younger age groups. However, some research has in fact highlighted the deleterious effects of social deprivation on adolescents’ development and mental health [61, 62], as adolescence is a life period outlined by an elevated need for interaction with peers and an enhanced susceptibility to social stimuli and to the nocive repercussions of social exclusion [63]. For instance, both peer social acceptance and peer influence are crucial aspects of development in adolescence [63], where social deprivation exerts unique adverse effects on adolescents’ brain and behaviors in comparison with other age groups [61, 64,65,66]. The abovementioned facts thus sustain the potential indirect effect of loneliness, demonstrated by the present study, when studying the relationship between problematic use of social networks and cognitive function in adolescents, and call for more studies in this research area among this vulnerable age group.

Lastly, our analysis also showed that physical activity was related to higher levels of cognitive functioning, whereas adolescents having a higher financial burden had worse cognitive function. Those findings are consistent with previous literature showing that moderate physical activity bolsters cognitive function as well as brain’s structure and function among adolescents [67, 68], primarily by enhancing general health and subjective well-being [67]. On the other hand, studies have shown that the socio-economic status is linked to adolescents’ cognitive competencies, namely language development and executive functions, with socially disadvantaged adolescents having deteriorated cognitive processes [69]. Indeed, disparities within adolescents’ education, which are highly dependent on their socio-economic level in Lebanon (a developing country), could support and explain our finding.

Clinical implications

The results of this study may benefit school counselors, teachers, and parents, who should remain vigilant towards adolescents who lack true friendships or seem lonely. They should also be mindful of the possibility of problematic use of social networks underlying adolescents’ social isolation and thus implement effective awareness programs and supportive measures for those adolescents (e.g., social media guidance, social media time management, engagement in outdoor activities, promotion of physical activity, etc.), in order to help them limit their social media exposure, open up, and establish healthy and solid relationships with their peers. Detecting and fixing such problematic behaviors at a precocious level is very important to prevent deterioration of cognitive function among adolescents and protect their mental health and academic performance. As such, clinical psychologists and school psychologists are prompted to look into these matters in a dependent way and consider the possibility of their interrelations, in order to better identify the origins of cognitive function decline among adolescents and establish proficient and integral treatment and management plans. Specifically, this study might advance several recommendations, such as implementing leisure services other than smartphones for adolescents/students, facilitating adolescents’ access to sports and other free activities in schools, designing awareness lectures and programs about the deleterious impacts of problematic use of social networks among students, and enacting policies to restrain and supervise their use of smartphones and social networks. Future research might also build on our results to identify other factors that might play a role in the relationship between problematic use of social networks and cognitive function in adolescents.

Limitations

Caution is essential when interpreting the findings and conclusions generated by this study, taking into consideration several limitations. First, its cross-sectional design is unable to infer causality and temporality of the studied correlations. A selection bias might also have occurred, owing to the fact that the data was derived from a non-probability sample using the snowball sampling method via online networks. However, this sampling technique was the only available option during the COVID-19 pandemic and its related security measures. In this context, an information bias is possible since the study relied on self-report measures rather than a physician’s assessment. A residual confounding bias is possible as well, as some additional predictors of the studied relationships might have been overlooked. Finally, it is important to mention that we could not vigorously assess the construct of loneliness, which could be emotional/intimate, social, or collective. Some adolescents might have more online interactions with their friends on social networks, but still feel lonely. The subjectivity regarding the definition and experience of loneliness should be taken into account and further investigated in the relationship between problematic use of social networks and cognitive function in upcoming research.

Conclusion

In sum, the current study supports that problematic use of social networks is negatively associated with adolescents’ cognitive function, where loneliness seems to play a pivotal role in this equation. The results thus endorse the importance of helping Lebanese adolescents to overcome problematic use of social networks and recover from their loneliness, to achieve a better cognitive/academic performance. Future investigations into additional comprehensive factors contributing to the relationship between problematic use of social networks and cognitive function in adolescents may help to further understand its complexity for implementing better interventions.

Data availability

All data generated or analysed during this study are not publicly available to maintain the privacy of the individuals’ identities. The dataset supporting the conclusions is available upon request to the corresponding author.

References

  1. Kiely D. Cognitive Function in Encyclopaedia of Quality of Life and Well-Being Research (ed. Michalos, AC) 974–978. In.: Springer; 2014.

  2. Baltes PB, Staudinger UM, Lindenberger U. Lifespan psychology: theory and application to intellectual functioning. Ann Rev Psychol. 1999;50:471–507. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.50.1.471.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Edition F. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. Am Psychiatric Assoc. 2013;21(21):591–643. https://doi.org/10.1590/s2317-17822013000200017.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Griffiths M. A ‘components’ model of addiction within a biopsychosocial framework. J Subst use. 2005;10(4):191–7. https://doi.org/10.1080/14659890500114359.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. González-Nuevo C, Cuesta M, Postigo Á, Menéndez-Aller Á, Muñiz J. Problematic Social Network Use: Structure and Assessment. Int J mental health Addict 2021:1–16. doi: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-021-00711-y.

  6. Fabio RA, Stracuzzi A, Lo Faro R. Problematic smartphone Use leads to behavioral and cognitive self-control deficits. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2022;19(12):7445. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19127445.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Statista. Number of smartphone users worldwide from 2014 to 2020. 2017.

  8. Paakkari L, Tynjälä J, Lahti H, Ojala K, Lyyra N. Problematic social media use and health among adolescents. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2021;18(4):1885. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18041885.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Erdem E, Efe YS. The smartphone addiction, peer relationships and loneliness in adolescents. L’encephale. 2021. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.encep.2021.06.009.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. GUO X, ZHENG L, CHENG X, LIU Y, LI L. The cognitive and neural mechanisms of perception of unfairness and related decision-making process. Adv Psychol Sci 2017, 25(6):903. doi: https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-015-0362-7.

  11. Lee H-R, Lee HE, Choi J, Kim JH, Han HL. Social media use, body image, and psychological well-being: a cross-cultural comparison of Korea and the United States. J health communication. 2014;19(12):1343–58. https://doi.org/10.1080/10810730.2014.904022.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Festinger L. A theory of social comparison processes. Hum Relat. 1954;7(2):117–40. doi;10.1177/001872675400700202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Wang J-L, Wang H-Z, Gaskin J, Hawk S. The mediating roles of upward social comparison and self-esteem and the moderating role of social comparison orientation in the association between social networking site usage and subjective well-being. Front Psychol. 2017;8:771. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00771.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Li Y. Upward social comparison and depression in social network settings: the roles of envy and self-efficacy. Internet Res. 2018. https://doi.org/10.1108/IntR-09-2017-0358.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Schmuck D, Karsay K, Matthes J, Stevic A. Looking up and feeling Down”. The influence of mobile social networking site use on upward social comparison, self-esteem, and well-being of adult smartphone users. Telematics Inform. 2019;42:101240. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tele.2019.101240.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. de Lenne O, Vandenbosch L, Eggermont S, Karsay K, Trekels J. Picture-perfect lives on social media: a cross-national study on the role of media ideals in adolescent well-being. Media Psychol. 2020;23(1):52–78. https://doi.org/10.1080/15213269.2018.1554494.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. De Vries DA, Kühne R. Facebook and self-perception: individual susceptibility to negative social comparison on Facebook. Pers Indiv Differ. 2015;86:217–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Luong KT, Knobloch-Westerwick S, Frampton J. Temporal self impacts on media exposure & effects: a test of the selective exposure self-and affect-management (SESAM) model. Media Psychol. 2021;24(1):48–78. 10.108015213269.2019.1657898.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Verduyn P, Gugushvili N, Massar K, Täht K, Kross E. Social comparison on social networking sites. Curr Opin Psychol. 2020;36:32–7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2020.04.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Cataldo I, Lepri B, Neoh MJY, Esposito G. Social media usage and development of psychiatric disorders in childhood and adolescence: a review. Front Psychiatry. 2021;11:508595. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2020.508595.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Barry CT, Sidoti CL, Briggs SM, Reiter SR, Lindsey RA. Adolescent social media use and mental health from adolescent and parent perspectives. J Adolesc 2017, 61:1–11. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2017.08.005.

  22. Chaudhari B, Menon P, Saldanha D, Tewari A, Bhattacharya L. Internet addiction and its determinants among medical students. Industrial psychiatry journal. 2015;24(2):158. https://doi.org/10.4103/0972-6748.181729.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Al-Yafi K, El-Masri M, Tsai R. The effects of using social network sites on academic performance: the case of Qatar. J Enterp Inform Manage. 2018. https://doi.org/10.1108/JEIM-08-2017-0118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Kim SY, Kim M-S, Park B, Kim J-H, Choi HG. The associations between internet use time and school performance among korean adolescents differ according to the purpose of internet use. PLoS ONE. 2017;12(4):e0174878. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174878.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Wacks Y, Weinstein AM. Excessive smartphone use is associated with health problems in adolescents and young adults. Front Psychiatry. 2021;12:669042. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2021.669042.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Farchakh Y, Dagher M, Barbar S, Haddad C, Akel M, Hallit S, Obeid S. Association between Problematic Social Media Use and Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity disorder in a sample of lebanese adults. Prim Care Companion CNS Disorders. 2022;24(2):39795. https://doi.org/10.4088/PCC.21m03025.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Cacioppo JT, Patrick W. Loneliness: human nature and the need for social connection. WW Norton & Company; 2008. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11126-022-10006-7.

  28. Badcock JC, Shah S, Mackinnon A, Stain HJ, Galletly C, Jablensky A, Morgan VA. Loneliness in psychotic disorders and its association with cognitive function and symptom profile. Schizophr Res. 2015;169(1–3):268–73. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2015.10.027.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Achterbergh L, Pitman A, Birken M, Pearce E, Sno H, Johnson S. The experience of loneliness among young people with depression: a qualitative meta-synthesis of the literature. BMC Psychiatry. 2020;20(1):1–23. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-020-02818-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Bhanderi DJ, Pandya YP, Sharma DB. Smartphone use and its addiction among adolescents in the age group of 16–19 years. Indian J Community Medicine: Official Publication Indian Association Prev Social Med. 2021;46(1):88. https://doi.org/10.4103/ijcm.IJCM_263_20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Hajek A, König H-H. Social isolation and loneliness of older adults in times of the COVID-19 pandemic: Can use of online social media sites and video chats assist in mitigating social isolation and loneliness? Gerontology 2021, 67(1):121–4. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1159/000512793.

  32. Saadeh H, Al Fayez RQ, Al Refaei A, Shewaikani N, Khawaldah H, Abu-Shanab S, Al-Hussaini M. Smartphone use among university students during COVID-19 quarantine: an ethical trigger. Front public health. 2021;9. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpubh.2021.600134.

  33. Berryman C, Ferguson CJ, Negy C. Social media use and mental health among young adults. Psychiatr Q. 2018;89(2):307–14. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11126-017-9535-6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Chakraborty K, Basu D, Kumar KV. Internet addiction: consensus, controversies, and the way ahead. In.; 2010. PMID: 22348866.

  35. Nahas M, Hlais S, Saberian C, Antoun J. Problematic smartphone use among lebanese adults aged 18–65 years using MPPUS-10. Comput Hum Behav. 2018;87:348–53. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2018.06.009.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Zeidan J, Hallit S, Akel M, Louragli I, Obeid S. Problematic smartphone use and affective temperaments among lebanese young adults: scale validation and mediating role of self-esteem. BMC Psychol. 2021;9(1):1–11. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-021-00638-y.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Viner RM, Gireesh A, Stiglic N, Hudson LD, Goddings A-L, Ward JL, Nicholls DE. Roles of cyberbullying, sleep, and physical activity in mediating the effects of social media use on mental health and wellbeing among young people in England: a secondary analysis of longitudinal data. The Lancet Child & Adolescent Health. 2019;3(10):685–96. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2352-4642(19)30186-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Twenge JM, Martin GN, Spitzberg BH. Trends in US Adolescents’ media use, 1976–2016: the rise of digital media, the decline of TV, and the (near) demise of print. Psychol Popular Media Cult. 2019;8(4):329. https://doi.org/10.1037/ppm0000203.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Fritz MS, MacKinnon DP. Required sample size to detect the mediated effect. Psychol Sci. 2007;18(3):233–9. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01882.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. World Health Organization. Body Mass Index- BMI. Available from: https://www.euro.who.int/en/health-topics/disease-prevention/nutrition/a-healthy-lifestyle/body-mass-index-bmi.

  41. Melki I, Beydoun H, Khogali M, Tamim H, Yunis K. Household crowding index: a correlate of socioeconomic status and inter-pregnancy spacing in an urban setting. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2004;58(6):476–80. https://doi.org/10.1136/jech.2003.012690.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  42. Weary-Smith KA. Validation of the physical activity index (PAI) as a measure of total activity load and total kilocalorie expenditure during submaximal treadmill walking. University of Pittsburgh; 2007. https://doi.org/10.1123/jpah.9.6.757.

  43. Wilson RS, Krueger KR, Arnold SE, Schneider JA, Kelly JF, Barnes LL, Tang Y, Bennett DA. Loneliness and risk of Alzheimer disease. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64(2):234–40. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.64.2.234.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. De Jong Gierveld J, Van Tilburg T. The De Jong Gierveld short scales for emotional and social loneliness: tested on data from 7 countries in the UN generations and gender surveys. Eur J Ageing. 2010;7(2):121–30. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10433-010-0144-6.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  45. Awad E, Rogoza R, Gerges S, Obeid S, Hallit S. Association of social media use disorder and orthorexia nervosa among lebanese university students: the indirect effect of loneliness and factor structure of the social media use disorder short form and the jong-gierveld loneliness scales. Psychol Rep 2022:00332941221132985. doi: https://doi.org/10.1177/00332941221132985.

  46. Annunziata M, Muzzatti B, Giovannini L, Lucchini G. Cognitive functioning self-assessment scale (CFSS): preliminary psychometric data. Psychol health Med. 2012;17(2):207–12. https://doi.org/10.1080/13548506.2011.596552.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Hair JF Jr, Hult GTM, Ringle CM, Sarstedt M. A primer on partial least squares structural equation modeling (PLS-SEM). Sage publications; 2021. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-80519-7.

  48. Walhovd KB, Krogsrud SK, Amlien IK, Bartsch H, Bjørnerud A, Due-Tønnessen P, Grydeland H, Jr Hagler DJ, Håberg AK, Kremen WS. Neurodevelopmental origins of lifespan changes in brain and cognition. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2016;113(33):9357–62. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1524259113.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  49. Giedd JN, Raznahan A, Alexander-Bloch A, Schmitt E, Gogtay N, Rapoport JL. Child psychiatry branch of the National Institute of Mental Health longitudinal structural magnetic resonance imaging study of human brain development. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2015;40(1):43–9. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2014.236.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Pfefferbaum A, Rohlfing T, Pohl KM, Lane B, Chu W, Kwon D, Nolan Nichols B, Brown SA, Tapert SF, Cummins K. Adolescent development of cortical and white matter structure in the NCANDA sample: role of sex, ethnicity, puberty, and alcohol drinking. Cereb Cortex. 2016;26(10):4101–21. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhv205.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Gold AL, Sheridan MA, Peverill M, Busso DS, Lambert HK, Alves S, Pine DS, McLaughlin KA. Childhood abuse and reduced cortical thickness in brain regions involved in emotional processing. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2016;57(10):1154–64. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12630.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Besteher B, Gaser C, Spalthoff R, Nenadić I. Associations between urban upbringing and cortical thickness and gyrification. J Psychiatr Res. 2017;95:114–20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2017.08.012.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Li B, Friston KJ, Liu J, Liu Y, Zhang G, Cao F, Su L, Yao S, Lu H, Hu D. Impaired frontal-basal ganglia connectivity in adolescents with internet addiction. Sci Rep. 2014;4(1):1–8. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep05027.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Horowitz-Kraus T, Hutton JS. Brain connectivity in children is increased by the time they spend reading books and decreased by the length of exposure to screen‐based media. Acta Paediatr. 2018;107(4):685–93. https://doi.org/10.1111/apa.14176.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Hu Y, Zhou M, Shao Y, Wei J, Li Z, Xu S, Maguire P, Wang D. The effects of social comparison and depressive mood on adolescent social decision-making. BMC Psychiatry. 2021;21(1):1–15. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-020-02928-y.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Aalbers G, McNally RJ, Heeren A, De Wit S, Fried EI. Social media and depression symptoms: a network perspective. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2019;148(8):1454. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000528.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Azhari A, Toms Z, Pavlopoulou G, Esposito G, Dimitriou D. Social media use in female adolescents: Associations with anxiety, loneliness, and sleep disturbances. Acta Psychol. 2022;229:103706. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2022.103706.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Donovan NJ, Wu Q, Rentz DM, Sperling RA, Marshall GA, Glymour MM. Loneliness, depression and cognitive function in older US adults. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2017, 32(5):564–73. doi: https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.4495.

  59. Lara E, Caballero FF, Rico-Uribe LA, Olaya B, Haro JM, Ayuso‐Mateos JL, Miret M. Are loneliness and social isolation associated with cognitive decline? Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2019;34(11):1613–22. https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.5174.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Smith L, Bloska J, Jacob L, Barnett Y, Butler L, Trott M, Odell-Miller H, Veronese N, Kostev K, Bettac EL. Is loneliness associated with mild cognitive impairment in low‐and middle‐income countries? Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2021;36(9):1345–53. https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.5524.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Orben A, Tomova L, Blakemore S-J. The effects of social deprivation on adolescent development and mental health. The Lancet Child & Adolescent Health. 2020;4(8):634–40. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2352-4642(20)30186-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Sawyer SM, Azzopardi PS, Wickremarathne D, Patton GC. The age of adolescence. The Lancet Child & Adolescent Health. 2018;2(3):223–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2352-4642(18)30022-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  63. Blakemore S-J, Mills KL. Is adolescence a sensitive period for sociocultural processing? Ann Rev Psychol. 2014;65:187–207. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  64. Matthews GA, Tye KM. Neural mechanisms of social homeostasis. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2019;1457(1):5–25. https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.14016.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Hall FS. Social deprivation of neonatal, adolescent, and adult rats has distinct neurochemical and behavioral consequences. Crit Reviews™ Neurobiol. 1998;12(1–2). https://doi.org/10.1615/critrevneurobiol.v12.i1-2.50.

  66. Burke AR, McCormick CM, Pellis SM, Lukkes JL. Impact of adolescent social experiences on behavior and neural circuits implicated in mental illnesses. Neurosci Biobehavioral Reviews. 2017;76:280–300. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.01.018.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  67. Luan X, Liu J, Luo X. Examining the Link between physical activity and cognitive function: a parallel mediation model of Health and Wellbeing among Adolescents. Front Psychol. 2022;13:764842–2. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.764842.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Donnelly JE, Hillman CH, Castelli D, Etnier JL, Lee S, Tomporowski P, Lambourne K, Szabo-Reed AN. Physical activity, fitness, cognitive function, and academic achievement in children: a systematic review. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2016;48(6):1197. https://doi.org/10.1249/MSS.0000000000000901.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Pluck G, Córdova MA, Bock C, Chalen I, Trueba AF. Socio-economic status, executive functions, and theory of mind ability in adolescents: Relationships with language ability and cortisol. Br J Dev Psychol. 2021;39(1):19–38. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjdp.12354. Epub 2020 Oct 14.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank all students who participated in this study.

Funding

None.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

SH and SO involved in study design. RT and SG wrote the manuscript. SH involved in data analysis and interpretation. ASED and DM collected the data. All authors reviewed the paper for intellectual content. All authors approved its final version.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Souheil Hallit.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

None to declare.

Ethics Approval and Consent to Participate

The Ethics and Research Committee of the Psychiatric Hospital of the Cross approved this study protocol (HPC-037-2021). Informed consent was obtained from parents and/or legal guardians of all minor participants involved in this study. All methods were performed in accordance with the relevant guidelines and regulations.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rami Tarabay and Sarah Gerges are first co-authors.

Sahar Obeid, Souheil Hallit, and Michel Soufia are last co-authors.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Tarabay, R., Gerges, S., Sarray El Dine, A. et al. Exploring the indirect effect of loneliness in the association between problematic use of social networks and cognitive function in Lebanese adolescents. BMC Psychol 11, 152 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-023-01168-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-023-01168-5

Keywords