Skip to main content

The psychometric properties and gender invariance of the Italian version of the Perceived Vulnerability to Disease Questionnaire (I-PVDQ) during the COVID-19 pandemic

Abstract

Background

The Perceived Vulnerability to Disease Questionnaire (PVDQ) measures beliefs associated with personal susceptibility to infectious diseases and behaviors or perceptions in the presence of potential risk of pathogen transmission. Given the onset of the Severe Acute Respiratory Syndrome Coronavirus 2 global pandemic, otherwise known as the COVID-19 pandemic, the construct being measured may function differently based on affective, behavioral, and cognitive changes along with the need to change norms and lifestyles in a global context. The present study aims to test the psychometric properties and the gender invariance of the Italian adaptation of the PVDQ to confirm that the scale can be used with Italian-speaking people, and that it functions effectively during a pandemic.

Methods

A total of 509 participants filled out an online questionnaire including the Italian version of the I-PVDQ (I-PVDQ) and several measures of psychological constructs. Reliability and factor analyses (single and multigroup) were conducted. Bayesian correlation tests and Bayesian independent sample t-tests were used to assess the validity of I-PVDQ.

Results

Exploratory factor analysis supported the two-factor structure of the I-PVDQ, and factor loadings loaded appropriately onto perceived infectability (PI) and germ aversion (GA). In terms of invariance, the scale showed configural, metric, scalar, and strict invariance across genders. Decisive evidence in favor of correlation with the measure of COVID-19 related fears for both PI and GA was found. There was strong evidence for observed correlations with COVID-19 related constructs such as intolerance to uncertainty, psychological inflexibility, resilience, stress, and anxiety. Women showed higher GA than men, while there were no gender differences in PI.

Conclusions

Taken together, these results suggest that the I-PVDQ confirms the psychometric properties of the original version and that can be used to detect PVD when it is affected by environmental circumstances since its functioning is preserved during a pandemic.

Peer Review reports

Background

During the course of human evolution, humans were forced to face numerous pathogens, and their immune systems subsequently developed and adapted in response to them [1]. In addition to these innate and adaptive immune responses, individuals have also developed a proactive system that enables them to recognize and avoid potential risks that augment the existing reactive system [2]. Its functioning was explained by the evolutionary disease avoidance mechanism proposed by Faulkner et al. [3], later known as the Behavioural Immune System (BIS) [4]. The authors describe a set of proactive protective behaviors against infection as a complementary mechanism to the immune system. The execution of these behaviors linked to how vulnerable a person feels. Indeed, individual differences in perceived vulnerability activate to different extents the processes directed to avoid diseases, such as detecting cues that characterize ill subjects, to knowing and acting on the strategies that prevent the transmission [4,5,6].

Nonetheless, along with these individual differences, there are specific situations in which this disposition becomes pervasive. When the World Health Organisation declared the Severe Acute Respiratory Syndrome Coronavirus 2 (SARS-CoV-2) global pandemic, otherwise known as the COVID-19 pandemic, humankind was confronted with a new and potentially fatal disease. To contain the spread of the pandemic, most countries have imposed strong political and societal measures (e.g., use of facemasks, strict social distancing, washing hands frequently) introducing vast behavioral changes in individuals’ daily routine. Therefore, the COVID-19 pandemic provides a unique research framework to investigate peoples’ perceived vulnerability to disease (PVD).

To conduct this investigation, the problem is approached from a psychometric point of view. Before the pandemic, one of the most widely used scales to measure PVD was the Perceived Vulnerability to Disease Questionnaire (PVDQ) [7]. Based on Faulkner et al.’s [3] disease avoidance mechanisms studies, the scale measures beliefs associated with personal susceptibility to infectious diseases and behaviors or perceptions in the presence of potential risk of pathogen transmission. During the last two years, many papers have been published containing data collected after the pandemic outbreak administering the PVDQ (e.g., [8,9,10,11,12,13,14,15]). However, it is unclear if the questionnaire’s psychometric properties remain unchanged once the fear of infection contributes to the perception of a largely uncontrollable and unpredictable threat, and the necessity to avoid exposure to pathogens is highly stressed or even imposed by the government (e.g., the explicit requirement to avoid shaking hands and maintain a physical distance, to stay with face masks at the workplace, on public transport, etc.). At the onset of COVID-19, the disease etiology and course were uncertain, with no proven treatment protocols, no immunity, and no vaccines. Thus, the exponential spread and high mortality associated with the prevention measures proposed by health authorities added to the altered perception of a potential threat [16, 17]. In this setting, it becomes important to investigate whether the psychometric properties can be considered equivalent across times marked or not marked by a pandemic. Indeed, when the construct being measured becomes dramatically relevant and pervasive, such as the subjective perception of the exposure to COVID-19, the PVDQ may need to be adjusted in response to environmental changes [14, 18] because the measured construct is changing or is changed. Studies on the effects of COVID-19 have shown a negative impact on people's mental health [19, 20]. Concerns about the possibility of being infected, or for life itself, increased during the pandemic [21] and had a psychological impact (e.g., higher levels of anxiety, depression, and stress) [22, 23]. Moreover, the COVID-19 pandemic social distancing norms and intensive hygiene practices have changed the perception of the vulnerability to the desease. Therefore, researchers that have employed the PVDQ during the pandemic suggested that such variations may have altered the psychometric properties of the whole questionnaire and the single items. Do Bú and colleagues [24] find high response scores to some items because most respondents perceived themselves as highly susceptible to infections and disgusted with sharing items perceived as “dirty” (e.g., handling money, sharing the same water bottle). Thus, they have advanced doubts as to how this situation might have affected people's responses to some items of the scale. Indeed, when administered during a pandemic, items exploring disgust and perceived infectability may not produce the same variability of responses, and to capture that issues it is necessary to modify the scale. Therefore, using a scale created for situations not marked by the pandemic might result in biased measures of PVD until the psychometric properties of that scale are confirmed when a pandemic is on.

Objectives

Starting from this premise, the present study has three objectives as described below.

Factorial structure and reliability of the I-PVDQ (Aim 1). We aimed at conducting a psychometric analysis to confirm the factor structure and reliability of the Italian version of the VDQ (I-PVDQ) when PVD becomes pervasive as it happens during a pandemic. Specifically, two moderately correlated factors were proposed for the scale [7]: the first one was named Perceived Infectability (PI) which assesses personal beliefs about perceived vulnerability to illness (i.e., how easily a person believes he or she can be infected if exposed to a possible source of illness), the second one was called Germ Aversion (GA) and it measures the discomfort experienced when a potential pathogen transmission is more likely to occur. Both factors showed a satisfactory internal consistency, with a higher value for the PI when compared to GA. More recently, using the Spanish version of the PVDQ, Diaz and colleagues [25] supported the presence of these two factors (even if they proposed to analyze separately each dimension), and they reported that internal consistency was good for PI, but failed to reach an adequate value for GA. Similarly, Ferreira and colleagues [26] were able to replicate, the two-factor structure of the scale with a Portuguese sample, but good internal consistency was obtained only after eliminating five items from the questionnaire. By and large, only a few studies have reported satisfactory internal consistency for both subscales [7, 27, 28], while inadequate levels of internal consistency have often been reported for the GA subscale [7, 25, 26, 29,30,31] raising doubts about the appropriateness of the items that comprise it. For this shortcoming, different scoring solutions were proposed: (two subscales separate scores [27, 28, 30, 31, 35], overall score [3, 29, 31,32,33], and only PI subscale score [34]) and to reinforce the factorial structure and general psychometric properties of the questionnaire, frequently adopted solutions involved dropping off some of the original items [24,25,26, 35]. Thus, the first aim of the current study was to ascertain if, mantaining all the items of the original version, the I-PVDQ can be deemed bidimensional with adequate reliability of each factor.

Invariance of the I-PVDQ across genders (Aim 2). The current study also aimed to test the invariance of the I-PVDQ across male and female respondents because gender is a relevant factor in most of the analyses concerning PVD but, to make unbiased between-group comparisons, it is necessary to provide evidence of measurement equivalence of the scale across groups [36]. Specifically, previous studies found differences in PVD between men and women, with women showing a higher level of Perceived Infectability [3, 7, 25, 37], and Germ Aversion [3, 7, 37]. These gender differences have been explained by the higher pathogen disgust sensitivity of women (e.g., [38, 39]) which increases their avoidance behaviors associated with a possible risk of infection through contact or physical proximity to other people. It has been suggested that the two processes underlying BIS are disgust emotion and the fear of contamination, which together activate our pathogen avoidance system [5, 6]. Such a mechanism is easily understood from an evolutionary point of view: the ability to identify likely sources of infection would allow people to avoid them and ensure a longer life expectancy. The higher levels of PI and GA reported for women reaffirm this very function of the BIS. Indeed, women's evolutionary role which sees them involved in identifying better possible fitness in partners to ensure healthy offspring and guarantee their survival has been widely adopted in the literature [40,41,42]. Thus, to confirm the presence of gender differences, it is necessary to test the I-PVDQ invariance and ascertain that the same scoring and interpretation rules can be used in subgroups of women and men. To the best of our knowledge, only one study has tested and confirmed measurement invariance across genders [24]. Indeed, Diaz et al. [25] showed that PI and GA have the same factorial structure across genders, but they did not perform an invariance analysis.

Correlations with related constructs and gender differences (Aim 3). The study aimed to explore the pattern of relationships between the I-PVDQ dimensions and psychological constructs linked to the pandemic, such as fear of COVID-19 [43], psychological inflexibility [44,45,46], intolerance to uncertainty [47, 48], resiliency [49,50,51], well-being [52, 53], and psychological distress (anxiety, stress, and depression) [54, 55].

Validation studies of the PVDQ scale, for the most part, have assessed the construct in relation to measures of disgust sensitivity, hypochondria, health beliefs, and personality traits [7, 24,25,26]. However, Duncan and colleagues [7] argue that although there is a relationship between PVD, disgust sensitivity, and health beliefs, they are to be considered different constructs. Disgust sensitivity has to do with the triggering circumstances, health beliefs refer to general concerns and attitudes about the possibility of getting sick, while PVDQ specifically assesses beliefs about susceptibility to infectious diseases, intended as the extent to which people consider themselves vulnerable once exposed to certain pathogens, and the emotional distress this perception of risk causes [7, 56]. The COVID-19 pandemic affected people’s perceived vulnerability and increased fear of contamination levels. Therefore, PVD was placed in close relationship with several psychological constructs studied during the COVID-19 pandemic that allows studying the construct beyond the previously proposed framework. Fear is an adaptive emotion that enables coping with a potential threat like triggering safety behaviors (e.g., washing hands regularly) [57]. However, excessive or low levels of fear compared to the actual threat become maladaptive [58]. People who underestimate the risk may disregard government measures to slow the propagation of coronavirus and spread the disease, conversely excessive fear may lead to exaggerated safety behaviors that have been linked to mental health problems such as phobia, stress, or social anxiety [59,60,61,62,63,64,65]. High levels of fear related to infectious diseases such as COVID-19 can induce people not to reason rationally [66]. Several studies have been concerned with assessing the levels, causes, and consequences of Covid-19 pandemic-related fear [21, 59, 67,68,69,70,71,72,73,74]. If fear of COVID-19 altered safety behaviors, it should be possible to find this relationship between the two dimensions of the PVDQ and the Fear of COVID-19 Scale.

Another construct related to COVID-19 and PVD is the intolerance of uncertainty. When lacking sufficient information, a distorted cognitive appraisal can lead to the “fear of the unknown”. Intolerance of uncertainty is an individual disposition related to this ability [75] and is recognized as a cognitive vulnerability [76, 77] that can amplify the effects of stressors on mental health [78, 79]. Intolerance to uncertainty is considered a major vulnerability factor involved in the development of several psychological disorders [80]. Similarly, increased levels of stress, depression, and anxiety evidence the impact of COVID-19 on mental health outcomes [22, 61, 81,82,83]. Therefore, it is relevant to assess levels of depression, anxiety, and stress, along with intolerance to uncertainty in relation to PVD. In contrast, psychological resilience and psychological flexibility can have buffering effects when facing adversities and improve levels of psychological well-being. A large body of research supports the association between psychological flexibility and mental well-being. The moderating effect of psychological flexibility on stressor outcomes and negative life events has been proven [84, 85]. Conversely, psychological inflexibility negatively affects levels of depression and anxiety [86,87,88]. Similarly, high levels of resiliency, conceived as a result of the interactions between internal resilience factors and environmental factors [89], facilitate successful adaptation and promote positive changes when confronting adversities, trauma, and stress [90], and consequently provide mental health protection [91, 92]. Therefore, higher levels of psychological resilience and flexibility can provide protection from psychological disorders and increase levels of well-being. Studies employing the PVDQ have shown that despite the protective role of BIS, it can contribute to aversive responses toward people perceived as potentially infectious [2, 93]. Moreover, perceived vulnerability to disease induces people to engage in more proactive preventive behaviors (particularly beneficial behavior when the risk of contagion is high). As noted for protective behaviors, higher levels of PVD have also been associated with greater psychological distress, and increased levels of anxiety and depression [94, 95]. In line with the literature, it was expected to find a relationship between PVD and these variables during the pandemic. If this is the case, this study could provide evidence of a decisive impact of the pandemic on the subjective perception of infectability and the related behavioral responses, suggesting that given the highly stressful pandemic scenario (i.e., the specific prevention regulations implemented during the global COVID-19 pandemic that led to increased awareness of possible pathogen transmission sources), environmental variables affect not only PVD fluctuations [14, 94], but also the PVDQ properties.

Finally, gender differences were explored to highlight similarities and differences with the previous study [3, 7, 25, 37].

Methods

Participants and procedure

A total of 509 Italian participants (58.7% female, age range from 18 to 76 years, Mage = 27.24, SD = 12.16) were recruited. Sampling was based on the “snowball” method [96], in which undergraduate students from a large university in Central Italy were invited to participate in an online questionnaire study and were also encouraged to recruit their acquaintances and relatives to participate. Since, the minimum sample size is 200 for factor analyses with ordinal data [97, 98] (see below Statistical Analysis), the sample size was deemed adequate.

Data collection began in mid-March 2021 and was completed at the end of April 2021. At that time, Italy was going through a new (third) pandemic wave. The prevention measures adopted through ministerial decrees provided for the existence of geographical areas of high, medium, and low risk based on the incidence of COVID-19 cases recorded in proportion to the number of inhabitants. The areas of central Italy where data collection was concentrated fell within the high-risk zone. Throughout the country, there was a requirement to wear FFP2-type masks outdoors and in all indoor places, an interpersonal distancing of at least one meter, and a general curfew from 10 PM to 5 AM. In high-risk areas, all movements had to be justified by proven work requirements, situations of absolute necessity or health, preschool educational services were suspended, and educational activities in schools of all levels could be conducted only remotely (online). In addition, all recreational and entertainment activities were suspended, while restaurants, bars, and other facilities closed. Thus, during the period of data collection for this study, the normal course of daily life was severely hampered.

All participants provided informed consent and they voluntarily took part in the study. No compensation or incentives were provided. None of the tests included were intended to be clinical diagnostic, which would require direct administration by people with specific professional qualifications, but they are tests that measure individual dispositions that are generally used for research purposes. The questionnaire was built so that it could be self-administered using the written information provided as a foreword to the study and the specific completion instructions included as a foreword to each scale. Once completed, the data were automatically recorded, and no further action was required from participants.

The study was approved by the university’s local institutional review board (Commissione Etica per la Ricerca dell’Università degli Studi di Firenze, n. 148 - prot. 0134386).

Measures

The online questionnaire consisted of the following scales.

Italian Perceived Vulnerability to Disease Questionnaire (I-PVDQ). The PVDQ [7] is a self-report measure that contains 15 questions investigating two distinct dimensions of Perceived Infectability (PI) with seven items (e.g., “If an illness is “going around”, I will get it.”) and Germ Aversion (GA) with eight items (e.g., “It really bothers me when people sneeze without covering their mouths.”). The responses are coded on a seven-point scale (with endpoints labeled “strongly disagree” and “strongly agree”). The Italian version was obtained following a standard translation procedure [99] by two Italian psychologists fluent in English who discussed the differences between the two translated versions. The wording of item 15 has been changed to make it more up-to-date and modified as follows: “I avoid using public telephones because of the risk that I may catch something from the previous user.” to “I avoid using other people’s mobile phones or going to Internet point because of the risk that I may catch something from the previous user” (Internet Point is a common way of defining the different types of public spaces, widely used in Italy, where computers and the Internet access are available. Public telephones have fallen into disuse, but Internet Points are the places that come closest to what used to be the sharing and use of public telephone booths.). Once a single version was obtained, a native English speaker fluent in Italian, who was not exposed to content from the original scale, back-translated it into Italian. The back-translation was largely similar to the original scale. Therefore, this preliminary version was presented to a small group of native Italians (N = 6) who were asked to read the items and judge whether they were understandable or to indicate any unclear/ambiguous word, sentence, or meaning. No problems were reported and thus, no further adjustments were made.

Multidimensional Assessment of COVID-19-Related Fears (MAC-RF) [43]. It is an eight-item self-report measure that assesses clinically relevant domains of fear (i.e., cognitive, relational, bodily, and behavioural) during the COVID-19 pandemic. Responses are collected on a 5-point Likert scale (ranging from 0 = very unlike me to 4 = very like me). A total score is obtained by summing up the eight-item answers. The higher it is, the stronger fears are. Cronbach’s Alpha calculated on the current sample was adequate (α = 0.74).

Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale short form (IUS-12) [100], Italian version [101]. It is a 12-item self-report measure that assesses one’s ability to cope with unpredictable changes and respond appropriately in inherently ambiguous situations. The items are rated on a five-point Likert scale (ranging from 1 = not at all characteristic of me to 5 = entirely characteristic of me). A total score was computed. In the present study, the overall internal consistency for the scale was good (α = 0.87).

Acceptance and Action Questionnaire-II (AAQ-II) [102], Italian version [103]. The AAQ-II is a measure of psychological inflexibility and experiential avoidance which are related to a wide range of psychological disorders and quality of life. In this study, the ten-item Italian version is evaluated on a seven-point Likert scale (from 1 = never true to 7 = always true). It is a unidimensional measure where higher scores indicate greater psychological inflexibility. Cronbach’s Alpha in this study was good (α = 0.83).

Connor Davidson Resilience Scale 10© (CD-RISC 10) [104, 105], Italian version [106]. The CD-RISC 10 is a brief measure of resilience that examines one’s ability to cope with adversity. It consists of ten items are rated on a scale from 0 to 4 (0 = not true at all to 4 = true nearly all the time). A higher score indicates greater resilience. In the current sample, the scale demonstrated good reliability (α = 0.85).

Depression Anxiety Stress Scales-21 (DASS-21) [107], Italian version [108]. It is a self-report questionnaire with 21 items measuring depression, stress, and anxiety (seven items for each subscale) based on a four-point rating scale (with endpoints labelled 0 = did not apply to me at all and 3 = applied to me much, or most of the time). Higher scores indicate higher levels of depression, anxiety, or stress. In the current sample, Cronbach’s Alpha for Stress and Depression subscales were excellent (α = 0.90), and good for the Anxiety subscale (α = 0.84).

Well-Being Numerical Rating Scales (WB-NRSs) [109]. It is a five-item instrument that assesses physical, psychological, relational, spiritual, and general well-being. Each of the five items uses a 10-point numerical rating scale (with 1 indicating a state of “absolute distress” and 10 a state of complete well-being). The respondent selects an integer that best reflects the magnitude of the characteristic being investigated. Single ratings can be used to assess each specific component of well-being.

Statistical analysis

Before conducting the analyses, the missing values in the data were examined. Listwise deletion was used when one or more answers to the I-PVDQ were missing. For the other scales, listwise deletion was used when a case had more than 10% of missing answers [110]. Otherwise, the case item mean was used to replace the missing value.

In the preliminary phase of the statistical analyses, item descriptives were computed to examine the distribution of responses across the items. Specifically, mean, standard deviation, range, skewness, and kurtosis values were examined to test the variability in the item responses and departures from normal distributions. Values outside the range of − 1 and 1 were considered indicators of non-normal distributions [111].

To test structural/internal validity, the factor structure of the I-PVDQ was assessed. The data file was randomly split into two parts to perform Exploratory factor analysis (EFA; N = 256) and Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA; N = 245). EFA was performed on FACTOR [112]–Version 12.1. Optimal implementation of Parallel Analysis (PA) [113] was conducted to identify the number of recommended factors and the Robust Unweighted Least Squares (RULS) estimation method was used with the Robust Promin rotation. CFA model testing was based on EFA results conducted using JASP 0.16.3 [114]. The diagonal weighted least squares (DWLS) estimation method was employed because the data measured with Likert-type ratings are ordinal in nature.

Chi-square test, Comparative Fit Index (CFI), Tucker–Lewis index (TLI), and Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) were used to evaluate the goodness-of-fit. Specifically, an RMSEA value of approximately 0.08 and 0.05 would suggest moderate and excellent model fit, respectively, and CFI and TLI in the range of 0.90–0.95 would suggest moderate and excellent model fit, respectively [36, 110].

Multigroup CFA was used to evaluate whether the scale was invariant across genders (male sample: N = 205, and female samples: N = 295). Specifically, a hierarchically nested series of CFA were applied. An unconstrained model was used as a baseline to test configural invariance (i.e., the two groups share the same factor structure). Then, three more restrictive models were tested, which include: a model in which factor loadings were constrained to be equal across groups (Metric), a model in which factor loadings plus intercepts were constrained to be equal across groups (Scalar), and a model in which factor loadings, intercepts plus error variances were constrained to be equal across groups (Strict). Models were compared using the chi-square-based likelihood ratio and the equality constraints were tested using the Comparative Fit Index difference (ΔCFI), and Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (ΔRMSEA). A ΔCFI value ≤ 0.01 supplemented by a change ≤ 0.015 in RMSEA would indicate invariance [116, 117].

Reliability was measured as the internal consistency of the PI and GA subscales using Cronbach's alpha (for comparison with previous studies) and McDonald’s omega (that must be preferred for short scales) [118] with a relative 95% confidence.

Bayesian statistical analyses were used to evaluate the relationships among I-PVDQ factors (PI and GA) and the variables in the study, and gender differences in GA and PI. Jeffreys’ Bayes Factor described the observed data using a priori and posterior distribution [120], which allowed quantification of evidence in favor of the alternative and null hypothesis [121]. Bayes Factors for evidence of alternative hypotheses is presented as an easy-to-interpret odds ratio that represents the magnitude of the difference: 1–3 as weak, 3–10 as substantial, 10–30 as strong, 30–100 as very strong, and > 100 as decisive [121]. All the Bayesian tests were performed using JASP 0.16.3. Specifically, Bayesian correlation tests and Bayesian independent sample t-tests were used to investigate construct and criterion validity. Positive low to medium correlations (0.20 < r < 0.45) between the I-PVDQ factors and fear of COVID-19, intolerance to uncertainty, psychological inflexibility, stress, anxiety, and depression measures were expected, while negative low to medium correlations were expected with resiliency and well-being measures. Finally, it was hypothesized that women showed higher levels of GA and PI. Therefore, a one-tail hypothesis was tested (i.e., the women’s scores are higher when compared to men’s scores).

Results

After examining the missing values, five cases (1.0%) were deleted because of missing responses in the I-PVDQ scale, three cases (0.6%) were deleted since there were more than 10% of missing values and the other nine single case item entries were replaced by the mean value for the respective item.

Descriptives

Item descriptive statistics are presented in Table 1. All response options were selected, and item means ranged from 2.40 (SD = 1.40) to 5.98 (SD = 1.60). Values of skewness and kurtosis were above the −1/+1 range for 8 items.

Table 1 Descriptives and factor loadings of the items of the Italian Perceived Vulnerability to Disease Questionnaire (I-PVDQ)

Factorial structure and reliability of the I-PVDQ (single group analyses)

Exploratory factor analysis (EFA) The Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin (KMO) measure of sampling adequacy indicated that the strength of the relationships between the 15 items was fair (KMO = 0.84). Bartlett’s test of sphericity, which tests the overall significance of all the correlations within the correlation matrix, was significant (Bartlett’s χ2(N = 256, df = 105) = 1566.7, p < 0.001), and indicated acceptability to proceed with the analysis. Parallel analysis results indicated that two factors should be retained based on the 95-percentile of random eigenvalues. The two-factor model accounted for 47% of the variance and all items loaded on the appropriate dimension (EFA factor loadings are presented in Table 1). Factor PI and Factor GA accounted for 32% and 15% of the variance in the data, respectively. Exploratory factor analysis supported the two-factor structure of the I-PVDQ, and factor loadings attested that each factor included the items that are deemed to measure perceived infectability (PI) and Germ Aversion (GA). The correlation between the two factors’ scores was low (0.15).

Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) By and large, CFA replicated the EFA results. Specifically, the fit of the two-factor model (Fig. 1) was good (χ2 (89) = 149.96, p < 0.001; TLI = 0.95; CFI = 0.94; RMSEA = 0.053 [90% CI: 0.038–0.067]). Factor loadings (Table 1) were all significant at p < 0.001 and ranged from 0.23 to 0.81. Factor covariance was equal to 0.16, indicating that the PI and GA were weakly correlated.

Fig. 1
figure 1

The two-factor model (F1 = Germ Aversion, F2 = perceived infectability) of the Italian Perceived Vulnerability to Disease Questionnaire (I-PVDQ)

Internal consistency The internal consistency of the I-PVDQ was adequate for the PI (α = 0.76 [95%: 0.71–0.80], ω = 0.77 [95%: 0.72-0.81]), but slightly low for the GA (α = 0.67 [95%: 0.60–0.72], ω = 0.69 [95%: 0.63–0.75]).

Invariance of the I-PVDQ across genders (multi-group analysis)

All the models showed an adequate fit and the difference in CFI and RMSEA values were all < 0.01 (Table 2). Thus, the scale maintains the equivalence of the two-factor structure (Configural invariance), factor loadings (Metric invariance), intercepts (Scalar invariance), and error variances (Strict invariance) across male and female groups.

Table 2 Fit Statistics of the Italian Perceived Vulnerability to Disease Questionnaire (I-PVDQ) invariance models across genders

Correlations with related constructs and gender differences

Perceived Infectability (PI). There is decisive evidence in favour of correlations between PI and other related constructs. A medium positive correlation was found with the measure of COVID-19-related fears (r = 0.40, BF10 > 100). Intolerance of uncertainty, psychological inflexibility, and resiliency were weakly related to PI (r = 0.21, BF10 > 100; r = 0.26, BF10 > 100; and r = − 0.24, BF10 > 100 respectively). Similarly, Stress, Anxiety, and Depression scores had low positive correlations with PI (r = 0.24, BF10 > 100; r = 0.22, BF10 > 100; and r = 0.24, BF10 > 100 respectively). Overall, correlations with wellbeing ranged from − 0.17 to − 0.21 with BF10 > 100 indicating negative low relationships with PI.

Germ Aversion (GA) There was decisive evidence of a medium positive correlation with the measure of COVID-19 related fears during the pandemic (r = 0.42, BF10 > 100). The evidence of the relationship was very strong between GA and IUS-12 (r = 0.16, BF10 > 30), and strong between GA and measure of Stress (r = 0.14, BF10 > 10). Bayesian correlations between GA and AAQ-II (r = 0.14), CD-RISC (r = − 0.10), and Anxiety (r = 0.10) were non-significant (BF10 < 10) while there was no correlation with the measure of Depression (r = 0.05, BF10 < 10). As for the well-being measures, decisive weak negative correlations have been observed GA and General well-being (r = − 0.18, BF10 > 100), and Relational well-being (r = − 0.17, BF10 > 100). Furthermore, there was evidence in favor of a strong negative relationship between GA and Physical well-being (r = − 0.14, BF10 > 10), and Psychological well-being (r = − 0.15, BF10 > 10). Finaly, there was not a correlation with Spiritual well-being (r = − 0.10, BF10 < 10). All correlations are reported in Table 3.

Table 3 Correlations between I-PVDQ subscales and all the other measures in the study

Gender differences The one-sided Bayesian t-test showed a BF10 value of 0.44 for the PI factor, pointing out evidence in favor of the null hypothesis., i.e., perceived infectability was equal in men and women (M = 22.66, SD = 7.28, and M = 23.57, SD = 7.45, respectively). The result obtained for the GA factor (BF10 = 189.78) suggests substantial evidence in favor of the alternative hypothesis. The Germ Aversion score was substantially higher in women compared to men (Mmen = 33.19, SD = 9.30 and Mwomen = 36.57, SD = 9.98, respectively).

Discussion

The current study aims to investigate the psychometric properties of the I-PVDQ and considers the emotional, cognitive, and behavioral impact that the COVID-19 pandemic had on an individual’s perception of their vulnerability to illness. The investigation also provides evidence of the adequate psychometric properties of the Italian version of the scale which, to the best of our knowledge, has not been formally translated and validated.

Specifically, this study tested the I-PVDQ’s factorial structure and the gender invariance of the Italian version of the scale. In line with Duncan et al. [7], the exploratory factor analysis supported the two-factor structure of the I-PVDQ, and factor loadings attested that each factor included all the items that are deemed to measure Perceived Infectability (PI) and Germ Aversion (GA). Confirmatory factor analysis replicated these results. However, from both EFA and CFA, a low factor loading for PI item 5 was observed (“My past experiences make me believe I am not likely to get sick even when my friends are sick”). In fact, this item showed low factor loadings and was removed in other validation studies [25, 26], with an explanation that its reverse wording makes it harder to comprehend. The other possible explanation is that COVID-19 confronted people with a new and unknown, potentially fatal disease and beliefs about their vulnerability were challenged because they did not have “past” experience as a reference point. It is possible to consider that the pattern of responses to this item was influenced by the time of data collection when the main concern of the population was to protect themselves from contracting COVID-19, a highly infectious disease. As such, no one could consider themselves immune or rely on their previous experiences. A similar pattern of responses was evidenced by Do Bú and colleagues [24] who suggested that responses to the questions could be influenced by the prominent perceived contagiousness if the data collection period is during the strong prevention measures imposed by the government. Additionally, the relationship between beliefs about own vulnerability to illness and the discomfort due to potential pathogen transmission seems to be very weak during a pandemic. The prevention measures imposed to limit the spread of the disease (e.g., wearing masks, physical distancing, washing hands) had impacted people’s everyday spontaneous behavioural responses. Thus, the interpretation of the I-PVDQ items referring to GA changed, because some behaviours have been strongly discouraged (e.g., shaking hands) or recognized as highly dangerous (e.g., sharing the same drink bottle), and people may pay more attention to all those behaviours that usually were of no concern, such as handling money or sharing phones, regardless of how they judge their infectability.

Nonetheless, despite the low factor loading of item 5, this study provides evidence of the structural validity of the I-PVDQ replicating the original bi-factorial structure of the PVDQ [7] and maintaining all of the items. This result was not obtained in other studies that had to eliminate some items from the scale to reproduce the two factors and proposed shorter versions [24,25,26, 35].

As for internal consistency, the low indices for the GA subscale found in this study are in line with the pre- and post-pandemic literature [25, 29, 31, 34] and it could be explained by the heterogeneity of its questions. As noted by Diaz and colleagues [25], the GA subscale investigates a wide range of different behaviors (e.g., touching money, sharing a mobile phone, shaking hands), and the this characteristic may affect the internal consistency of the subscale [122].

The pattern of correlations of the I-PVDQ with pandemic-related constructs offered strong support for the existence of the expected relationships and provide evidence of the construct and criterion validity of the scale. The measure of COVID-19 related fears and both I-PVDQ factors (PI and GA) are linked, and the strength of the correlation was very similar to those reported by Ahorsu et al. [67], which employed the scale during the pandemic. As expected, we found evidence of the positive relationship between PI and intolerance to uncertainty, psychological inflexibility, stress, anxiety, and depression, as it was for the negative associations between PI, resiliency, and well-being. Although all these correlations were low, they suggest that perceived infectability is directly related to individual dispositions that impede coping with stressful situations, such as psychological inflexibility and intolerance to uncertainty, and inversely related to personal resources that help facing difficulties, such as resiliency. Consistently, all the aforementioned constructs related to PI have been identified as risk factors related to distress regarding the pandemic [47, 50, 52]. Moreover, the lack of previous experience and reference points, such as reliable information on transmission, pathogenicity, treatment, and prognosis of the new disease, probably affected PI and linked it to intolerance to uncertainty that is considered a major vulnerability factor involved in the development of several psychological disorders [80]. At the same time, we can suppose that PI contributed to the increase stress, depression, and anxiety [22, 61, 81,82,83]. The same pattern of correlations for GA subscale was not observed for depression, anxiety, psychological inflexibility, resiliency, and spiritual wellbeing, but we observed low correlations in the expected direction with stress and intolerance to uncertainty, (positive), and well-being (negative). A tentative explanation is that the attentive focus on the imposed rules to limit the spread of the disease, the lockdown restrictions, and the news on the pandemic statistics (e.g., infected and deceased people daily reports) impacted people’s behavioural responses to avoid germs, regardless their personal resources and level of distress.

Taken together, these results suggest that the I-PVDQ maintains its psychometric properties once translated and that when PVD becomes pervasive as it happens during a pandemic can be used to capture PVD changes in response to situations that are perceived as threatening to one’s health (e.g., [38, 39]). On one hand, perceived infectability refers to personal infection history and the belief that one is likely or unlikely to fall ill “outside” the pandemic context. Thus, people may lose their reference points “inside” a pandemic. Additionally, when the risk of infection is extremely high and dangerous (i.e., during the pandemic), PI appears linked to some psychological dispositions and well-being/distress indicators. On the other hand, GA is more related to behavioral responses. When all persons could be potential carriers of the virus and, consequently, everyone is asked to abstain from close social contacts and adopt hygiene practices, individual avoidance of potential infection sources inevitably changes [125,126,127] and appears to be unrelated (or very weakly associated) to psychological dispositions and distress indicators.

The impact of the pandemic on PVD is confirmed by gender differences observed in the current study. Contrary to Diaz et al. [25] a difference in GA, but not in PI was found. These findings could be a result of cultural differences or the fact that the broad perception of infectiousness during the pandemic overcomes gender distinctions. Previous findings noted gender differences in GA given higher pathogen disgust sensitivity in women (e.g., [39, 128]). Therefore, women can reinforce protective behavior by adopting appropriate hygiene practices and by avoiding situations that are associated with an increased risk of contracting the infection, such as close contact or physical proximity with other persons.

Limitations

The present study is not without its limitations. One of the main limitations was the lack of pre-pandemic data to compare with those collected during the pandemic [129]. Furthermore, measures of other relevant variables (e.g., measures of fear of contamination, hypochondria, obsessive–compulsive symptoms, health status information) that might disentangle the nature of the PVD construct were not collected. Because of the particular historical moment (COVID-19 pandemic), a further limitation of this study is its local nature, as the sample consisted mainly of people residing in areas of Central Italy, as well as the lack of clinical sample. Indeed, whereas psychometric validation studies of PVDQ mostly availed of convenience samples, this aspect limits the generalizability of the results and did not allow certain aspects of PVD to be investigated, such as the relationship between age and the perceived infectability and germ aversion. Finally, future studies should aim to revise items to enhance the psychometric properties of the I-PVDQ and to highlight potential cultural differences in reactions to the pandemic. For example, item 4 includes content referring to using a pencil someone has chewed, item 15 refers to using public telephones, and item 11 refers to touching money. Given that the original measure was constructed in the early 2000s, some items may be less representative of people's everyday lives today (i.e., most people have their cell phones, many restaurants and shops encourage credit card payments, a lot of people write on their PC’s instead of using a paper and pencil). Furthermore, future research should consider revising the items so that they better reflect risk-reducing behaviors that have become part of our current way of behaving.

Conclusions

Although environmental circumstances have changed substantively since the COVID-19 pandemic, the present study revealed that the I-PVDQ maintained its reliability and validity in an Italian sample albeit with a low factor loading in a single item. The Italian version of the PVDQ can be used to evaluate vulnerability to disease in the Italian-speaking sample, whereas future studies should explore the revision of specific items to reflect more accurately the current post-pandemic environment.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Abbreviations

AAQ-II:

Acceptance and Action Questionnaire-II

CD-RISC 10:

Connor Davidson Resilience Scale 10©

CFA:

Confirmatory factor analysis

CFI:

Comparative Fit Index

DASS-21:

Depression Anxiety Stress Scales-21

DWLS:

Diagonal weighted least squares estimation method

EFA:

Exploratory factor analysis

GA:

Germ Aversion

IUS-12:

Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale short form

KMO:

Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin measure of sampling adequacy

M:

Mean

MAC-RF:

Multidimensional Assessment of COVID-19-Related Fears

PA:

Parallel Analysis

PI:

Perceived Infectability

PVD:

Perceived Vulnerability to Disease

PVDQ:

Perceived Vulnerability to Disease Questionnaire

I-PVDQ:

Italian Perceived Vulnerability to Disease Questionnaire

RMSEA:

Root Mean Square Error of Approximation

RULS:

Robust Unweighted Least Squares

SD:

Standard deviation

TLI:

Tucker–Lewis index

WB-NRSs:

Well-Being Numerical Rating Scales

ΔCFI:

Comparative Fit Index difference

ΔRMSEA:

Root Mean Square Error of Approximation

References

  1. Simon AK, Hollander GA, McMichael A. Evolution of the immune system in humans from infancy to old age. Proc Biol Sci. 1821;2015(282):20143085. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2014.3085.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Schaller M. The behavioural immune system and the psychology of human sociality. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2011;366(1583):3418–26. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2011.0029.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Faulkner J, Schaller M, Park JH, Duncan LA. Evolved disease-avoidance mechanisms and contemporary xenophobic attitudes. Group Process Intergroup Relat. 2004;7(4):333–53. https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430204046142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Murray DR, Schaller M. The behavioral immune system: implications for social cognition, social interaction, and social influence. Adv Exp Soc Psychol. 2016;53:75–129. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.aesp.2015.09.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Schaller M, Duncan LA. The behavioral immune system: Its evolution and social psychological implications. In: Forgas JP, Haselton MG, von Hippel W, editors. Evolution and the social mind: evolutionary psychology and social cognition. UK: Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group; 2007. p. 293–307.

    Google Scholar 

  6. Schaller M, Park JH. The behavioral immune system (and why it matters). Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2011;20(2):99–103. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721411402596.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Duncan LA, Schaller M, Park JH. Perceived Vulnerability to Disease: development and validation of a 15-item self-report instrument. Pers Individ Dif. 2009;47(6):541–6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2009.05.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Boyraz G, Legros DN, Tigershtrom A. COVID-19 and traumatic stress: the role of perceived vulnerability, COVID-19-related worries, and social isolation. J Anxiety Disord. 2020;76:102307. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2020.102307.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Goh J. Perceived vulnerability to disease predicts restrictive policy supports in response to the 2019-nCoV (“Wuhan virus”) outbreak. PsyArXiv [Preprint].2020.

  10. Hromatko I, Tonković M, Vranic A. Trust in Science, Perceived Vulnerability to Disease, and adherence to pharmacological and non-pharmacological COVID-19 recommendations. Front Psychol. 2021;12:664554. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.664554.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Moran JB, Goh JX, Kerry N, Murray DR. Outbreaks and outgroups: three tests of the relationship between disease avoidance motives and xenophobia during an emerging pandemic. Evol Psychol Sci. 2021;7(4):419–29. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40806-021-00283-z.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Rokvić N, Karan V. Serbian adaptation of the Perceived Vulnerability to Disease (PVD) scale. Engrami. 2022. https://doi.org/10.5937/engrami43-31095.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Shook NJ, Sevi B, Lee J, Oosterhoff B, Fitzgerald HN. Disease avoidance in the time of COVID-19: the behavioral immune system is associated with concern and preventative health behaviors. PLoS ONE. 2020;15(8):e0238015. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0238015.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Stangier U, Kananian S, Schüller J. Perceived Vulnerability to Disease, knowledge about COVID-19, and changes in preventive behavior during lockdown in a German convenience sample. Curr Psychol. 2021. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-021-01456-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Tybur JM, Lieberman D, Fan L, Kupfer TR, de Vries RE. Behavioral Immune trade-offs: interpersonal value relaxes social pathogen avoidance. Psychol Sci. 2020;31(10):1211–21. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797620960011.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Asmundson GJG, Taylor S. How health anxiety influences responses to viral outbreaks like COVID-19: what all decision-makers, health authorities, and health care professionals need to know. J Anxiety Disord. 2020;71:102211. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2020.102211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Taylor S, Landry CA, Paluszek MM, Fergus TA, McKay D, Asmundson GJG. COVID stress syndrome: concept, structure, and correlates. Depress Anxiety. 2020;37(8):706–14. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.23071.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Pelechano V, Rodríguez L, García J, Antuña M. Estabilidad, consistencia y cambio en personalidad [stability, consistency and change in personality]. In: Pelechano V, editor. Psicología sistemática de la personalidad [systematic psychology of personality]. Barcelona: Ariel; 2000. p. 135–76.

    Google Scholar 

  19. Kang L, Ma S, Chen M, Yang J, Wang Y, Li R, Yao L, Bai H, Cai Z, Xiang Yang B, Hu S, Zhang K, Wang G, Ma C, Liu Z. Impact on mental health and perceptions of psychological care among medical and nursing staff in Wuhan during the 2019 novel coronavirus disease outbreak: a cross-sectional study. Brain Behav Immun. 2020;87:11–7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbi.2020.03.028.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Mukhtar S. Psychological health during the coronavirus disease 2019 pandemic outbreak. Int J Soc Psychiatry. 2020;66(5):512–6. https://doi.org/10.1177/0020764020925835.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Pakpour AH, Griffiths MD. The fear of COVID-19 and its role in preventive behaviors. J Concurrent Disorders. 2020;2(1):58–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Wang C, Pan R, Wan X, Tan Y, Xu L, Ho CS, Ho RC. Immediate psychological responses and associated factors during the initial stage of the 2019 coronavirus disease (COVID-19) epidemic among the general population in China. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2020;17(5):1729. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17051729.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Zandifar A, Badrfam R. Iranian mental health during the COVID-19 epidemic. Asian J Psychiatr. 2020;51:101990. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajp.2020.101990.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Do Bú EA, de Alexandre MES, Rezende AT, Bezerra VADS. Perceived vulnerability to disease: adaptation and validation of the PVD-br. Curr Psychol. 2021;10:1–14. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-021-02424-w.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Díaz A, Soriano JF, Beleña Á. Perceived Vulnerability to Disease Questionnaire: factor structure, psychometric properties, and gender differences. Pers Individ Dif. 2016;101:42–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.05.036.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Ferreira J, Magalhães AC, Bem-Haja P, Alho L, Silva CF, Soares SC. Perceived Vulnerability to Disease Questionnaire: psychometric validation with a Portuguese sample. BMC Psychol. 2022 May 22;10(1):130. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-022-00838-0. Erratum in: BMC Psychol. 2022 Jun 15;10(1):150. PMID: 35599319; PMCID: PMC9124547.

  27. Murray DR, Jones DN, Schaller M. Perceived threat of infectious disease and its implications for sexual attitudes. Pers Individ Dif. 2013;54(1):103–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2012.08.021.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Makhanova A, Miller SL, Maner JK. Germs and the out-group: chronic and situational disease concerns affect intergroup categorization. Evol Behav Sci. 2015;9(1):8–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Miller SL, Maner JK. Overperceiving disease cues: the basic cognition of the behavioral immune system. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2012;102(6):1198–213. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027198.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Prokop P, Fančovičová J. The effect of owning animals on perceived vulnerability to, and avoidance of, parasitic diseases in humans. J Individ Differ. 2011;32(3):129–36. https://doi.org/10.1027/1614-0001/a000045.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Wu BP, Chang L. The social impact of pathogen threat: How disease salience influences conformity. Pers Individ Dif. 2012;53(1):50–4. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2012.02.023.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Ainsworth SE, Maner JK. Hunger moderates the activation of psychological disease avoidance mechanisms. Evol Behav Sci. 2014;8(4):303–13. https://doi.org/10.1037/ebs0000015.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Tybur JM, Lieberman D, Griskevicius V. Microbes, mating, and morality: individual differences in three functional domains of disgust. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2009;97(1):103–22. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015474.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Prokop P, Fančovičová J. Self-protection versus disease avoidance: the perceived physical condition is associated with fear of predators in humans. J Individ Differ. 2013;34(1):15–23. https://doi.org/10.1027/1614-0001/a000092.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Magallares A, Fuster-Ruiz De Apodaca MJ, Morales JF. Psychometric properties and criterion validity of the Perceived Vulnerability to Disease Scale (PVD) in the Spanish population. Rev Psicol Soc. 2017;32(1):164–95. https://doi.org/10.1080/02134748.2016.1248025.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Byrne BM. A primer of LISREL: basic applications and programming for confirmatory factor analytic models. New York: Springer Science & Business Media; 2012.

    Google Scholar 

  37. De Coninck D, d’Haenens L, Matthijs K. Perceived vulnerability to disease and attitudes towards public health measures: COVID-19 in Flanders ,Belgium. Pers Individ Dif. 2020;166:110220. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2020.110220.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Olatunji BO, Adams T, Ciesielski B, David B, Sarawgi S, Broman-Fulks J. The three domains of disgust scale: factor structure, psychometric properties, and conceptual limitations. Assessment. 2012;19(2):205–25. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191111432881.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Tybur J, Bryan A, Lieberman D, Caldwell Hooper A, Merriman L. Sex differences and sex similarities in disgust sensitivity. Pers Individ Dif. 2011;51(3):343–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.04.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Curtis V, Aunger R, Rabie T. Evidence that disgust evolved to protect from risk of disease. Proc R Soc Lond B. 2004. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2003.0144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Fessler DMT, Eng SJ, Navarrete CD. Elevated disgust sensitivity in the first trimester of pregnancy: evidence supporting the compensatory prophylaxis hypothesis. Evol Hum Behav. 2005;26(4):344–51. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2004.12.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Fessler DMT, Navarrete CD. Domain-specific variation in disgust sensitivity across the menstrual cycle. Evol Human Behav. 2003;24(6):406–17. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1090-5138(03)00054-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Schimmenti A, Starcevic V, Giardina A, Khazaal Y, Billieux J. Multidimensional Assessment of COVID-19-Related Fears (MAC-RF): a theory-based instrument for the assessment of clinically relevant fears during pandemics. Front Psychiatry. 2020;11:748. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00748.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Arslan G, Yıldırım M, Tanhan A, Buluş M, Allen KA. Coronavirus stress, optimism-pessimism, psychological inflexibility, and psychological health: psychometric properties of the coronavirus stress measure. Int J Ment Health Addict. 2021;19(6):2423–39. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-020-00337-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Crasta D, Daks JS, Rogge RD. Modeling suicide risk among parents during the COVID-19 pandemic: psychological inflexibility exacerbates the impact of COVID-19 stressors on interpersonal risk factors for suicide. J Contextual Behav Sci. 2020;18:117–27. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcbs.2020.09.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Kassim MAM, Pang NTP, Kamu A, Arslan G, Mohamed NH, Zainudin SP, Ayu F, Ho CM. Psychometric properties of the coronavirus stress measure with Malaysian young adults: association with psychological inflexibility and psychological distress. Int J Ment Health Addict. 2021. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-021-00622-y.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Satici B, Saricali M, Satici SA, Griffiths MD. Intolerance of uncertainty and mental wellbeing: serial mediation by rumination and fear of COVID-19. Int J Ment Health Addict. 2020. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-020-00305-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Smith BM, Twohy AJ, Smith GS. Psychological inflexibility and intolerance of uncertainty moderate the relationship between social isolation and mental health outcomes during COVID-19. J Contextual Behav Sci. 2020;18:162–74. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcbs.2020.09.005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Havnen A, Anyan F, Hjemdal O, Solem S, Gurigard Riksfjord M, Hagen K. resilience moderates negative outcome from stress during the COVID-19 pandemic: a moderated-mediation approach. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2020;17(18):6461. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17186461.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Song S, Yang X, Yang H, Zhou P, Ma H, Teng C, Chen H, Ou H, Li J, Mathews CA, Nutley S, Liu N, Zhang X, Zhang N. psychological resilience as a protective factor for depression and anxiety among the public during the outbreak of COVID-19. Front Psychol. 2021;11:618509. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.618509.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Zhang Y, Ma ZF. Impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on mental health and quality of life among local residents in Liaoning Province, China: a cross-sectional study. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2020;17(7):2381. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17072381.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Arslan G, Allen KA. Exploring the association between coronavirus stress, meaning in life, psychological flexibility, and subjective well-being. Psychol Health Med. 2021. https://doi.org/10.1080/13548506.2021.1876892.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Sønderskov KM, Dinesen PT, Santini ZI, Østergaard SD. The depressive state of Denmark during the COVID-19 pandemic. Acta Neuropsychiatr. 2020;32(4):226–8. https://doi.org/10.1017/neu.2020.15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Arslan G, Yıldırım M. Coronavirus stress, meaningful living, optimism, and depressive symptoms: a study of moderated mediation model. Aust J Psychol. 2021;73(2):113–24. https://doi.org/10.1080/00049530.2021.1882273.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Zhang J, Yang Z, Wang X, Li J, Dong L, Wang F, Li Y, Wei R, Zhang J. The relationship between resilience, anxiety and depression among patients with mild symptoms of COVID-19 in China: a cross-sectional study. J Clin Nurs. 2020;29(21–22):4020–9. https://doi.org/10.1111/jocn.15425.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Stewart SH, Watt MC. Illness Attitudes Scale dimensions and their associations with anxiety-related constructs in a nonclinical sample. Behav Res Ther. 2000;38(1):83–99. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(98)00207-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Steimer T. The biology of fear- and anxiety-related behaviors. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2002;4(3):231–49. https://doi.org/10.31887/DCNS.2002.4.3/tsteimer.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Mertens G, Gerritsen L, Duijndam S, Salemink E, Engelhard I. Fear of the coronavirus (COVID-19): predictors in an online study conducted in March 2020 [Internet]. PsyArXiv; 2020 Apr [cited 2022 Nov 10]. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/2p57j

  59. Rahman MA, Hoque N, Alif SM, Salehin M, Islam SMS, Banik B, et al. Factors associated with psychological distress, fear and coping strategies during the COVID-19 pandemic in Australia. Glob Health. 2020;16(1):95. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12992-020-00624-w.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Shigemura J, Ursano RJ, Morganstein JC, Kurosawa M, Benedek DM. Public responses to the novel 2019 coronavirus (2019-nCoV) in Japan: mental health consequences and target populations. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2020;74(4):281–2. https://doi.org/10.1111/pcn.12988.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  61. Qiu J, Shen B, Zhao M, Wang Z, Xie B, Xu Y. A nationwide survey of psychological distress among Chinese people in the COVID-19 epidemic: implications and policy recommendations. Gen Psychiatr. 2020 Mar 6;33(2):e100213. https://doi.org/10.1136/gpsych-2020-100213. Erratum in: Gen Psychiatr. 2020 Apr 27;33(2):e100213corr1. PMID: 32215365; PMCID: PMC7061893.

  62. Knowles KA, Olatunji BO. Anxiety and safety behavior usage during the COVID-19 pandemic: the prospective role of contamination fear. J Anxiety Disord. 2021;77:102323. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2020.102323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  63. Cox RC, Jessup SC, Luber MJ, Olatunji BO. Pre-pandemic disgust proneness predicts increased coronavirus anxiety and safety behaviors: evidence for a diathesis-stress model. J Anxiety Disord. 2020;76:102315. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2020.102315.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  64. Taylor S, McKay D, Abramowitz JS. Hypochondriasis and health-related anxiety. In: Sturmey P, Hersen M, editors. Handbook of evidence-based practice in clinical psychology. Hoboken: John Wiley & Sons; 2012. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118156391.ebcp002026.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  65. Salkovskis PM. The importance of behaviour in the maintenance of anxiety and panic: a cognitive account. Behav Psychother. 1991;19(1):6–19. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0141347300011472.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  66. Martínez-Lorca M, Martínez-Lorca A, Criado-Álvarez JJ, Armesilla MDC, Latorre JM. The fear of COVID-19 scale: validation in spanish university students. Psychiatry Res. 2020;293:113350. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2020.113350.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  67. Ahorsu DK, Lin CY, Imani V, Saffari M, Griffiths MD, Pakpour AH. The fear of COVID-19 scale: development and initial validation. Int J Ment Health Addict. 2020. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-020-00270-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  68. Ahorsu DK, Lin CY, Pakpour AH. The association between health status and insomnia, mental health, and preventive behaviors: the mediating role of fear of COVID-19. Gerontol Geriatr Med. 2020. https://doi.org/10.1177/2333721420966081.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  69. Ahorsu DK, Imani V, Lin CY, Timpka T, Broström A, Updegraff JA, Årestedt K, Griffiths MD, Pakpour AH. Associations between fear of COVID-19, mental health, and preventive behaviours across pregnant women and husbands: an actor-partner interdependence modelling. Int J Ment Health Addict. 2022;20(1):68–82. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-020-00340-x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  70. García-Reyna B, Castillo-García GD, Barbosa-Camacho FJ, Cervantes-Cardona GA, Cervantes-Pérez E, Torres-Mendoza BM, Fuentes-Orozco C, Pintor-Belmontes KJ, Guzmán-Ramírez BG, Bernal-Hernández A, González-Ojeda A, Cervantes-Guevara G. Fear of COVID-19 scale for hospital staff in regional hospitals in Mexico: a brief report. Int J Ment Health Addict. 2022;20(2):895–906. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-020-00413-x. Epub 2020 Nov 4. Erratum in: Int J Ment Health Addict. 2021 Jan 27;:1. PMID: 33169075; PMCID: PMC7640993.

  71. Harper CA, Satchell LP, Fido D, Latzman RD. functional fear predicts public health compliance in the COVID-19 pandemic. Int J Ment Health Addict. 2021;19(5):1875–88. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-020-00281-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  72. Mertens G, Gerritsen L, Duijndam S, Salemink E, Engelhard I. Fear of the coronavirus (COVID-19): Predictors in an online study conducted in March 2020 [Internet]. PsyArXiv; 2020 Apr [cited 2022 Nov 10]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2020.102258

  73. Vanni G, Materazzo M, Pellicciaro M, Ingallinella S, Rho M, Santori F, Cotesta M, Caspi J, Makarova A, Pistolese CA, Buonomo OC. Breast cancer and COVID-19: the effect of fear on patients’ decision-making process. In Vivo. 2020;34(3 Suppl):1651–9. https://doi.org/10.21873/invivo.11957.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  74. Wong LE, Hawkins JE, Langness S, Murrell KL, Iris P, Sammann A. Where are all the patients? Addressing Covid-19 fear to encourage sick patients to seek emergency care. NEJM Catalyst Innov Care Deliv. 2020. https://doi.org/10.1056/CAT.20.0193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  75. Carleton RN. Into the unknown: a review and synthesis of contemporary models involving uncertainty. J Anxiety Disord. 2016;39:30–43. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2016.02.007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  76. Koerner N, Dugas MJ. An investigation of appraisals in individuals vulnerable to excessive worry: the role of intolerance of uncertainty. Cogn Ther Res. 2008;32(5):619–38. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-007-9125-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  77. Mahoney AE, McEvoy PM. A transdiagnostic examination of intolerance of uncertainty across anxiety and depressive disorders. Cogn Behav Ther. 2012;41(3):212–22. https://doi.org/10.1080/16506073.2011.622130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  78. Chen CY, Hong RY. Intolerance of uncertainty moderates the relation between negative life events and anxiety. Personal Individ Differ. 2010;49(1):49–53. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.03.006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  79. Ciarrochi J, Said T, Deane FP. When simplifying life is not so bad: the link between rigidity, stressful life events, and mental health in an undergraduate population. Br J Guid Couns. 2005;33(2):185–97. https://doi.org/10.1080/03069880500132540.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  80. Merlo E, Sicari F, Frisone F, Costa G, Alibrandi A, Avena G, et al. Uncertainty, alexithymia, suppression and vulnerability during the COVID-19 pandemic in Italy. Health Psychol Rep. 2021;9(2):169–79. https://doi.org/10.5114/hpr.2021.104078.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  81. Marčinko D, Jakovljević M, Jakšić N, Bjedov S, Drakulić AM. The importance of psychodynamic approach during COVID-19 pandemic. Psychiatr Danub. 2020;32(1):15–21. https://doi.org/10.24869/psyd.2020.15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  82. Rossi R, Socci V, Pacitti F, Di Lorenzo G, Di Marco A, Siracusano A, Rossi A. Mental health outcomes among frontline and second-line health care workers during the coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) pandemic in Italy. JAMA Netw Open. 2020;3(5):e2010185. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2020.10185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  83. Yıldırım M, Solmaz F. COVID-19 burnout, COVID-19 stress and resilience: initial psychometric properties of COVID-19 burnout scale. Death Stud. 2022;46(3):524–32. https://doi.org/10.1080/07481187.2020.1818885.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  84. Gloster AT, Meyer AH, Lieb R. Psychological flexibility as a malleable public health target: evidence from a representative sample. J Contextual Behav Sci. 2017;6(2):166–71. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcbs.2017.02.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  85. Presti G, McHugh L, Gloster A, Karekla M, Hayes SC. the dynamics of fear at the time of Covid-19: a contextual behavioral science perspective. Clin Neuropsychiatry. 2020;17(2):65–71. https://doi.org/10.36131/CN20200206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  86. Doorley JD, Goodman FR, Kelso KC, Kashdan TB. Psychological flexibility: what we know, what we do not know, and what we think we know. Soc Personal Psychol Compass. 2020;14(12):1–11. https://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12566.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  87. Kashdan TB, Rottenberg J. Psychological flexibility as a fundamental aspect of health. Clin Psychol Rev. 2010;30(7):865–78. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2010.03.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  88. Bluett EJ, Homan KJ, Morrison KL, Levin ME, Twohig MP. Acceptance and commitment therapy for anxiety and OCD spectrum disorders: an empirical review. J Anxiety Disord. 2014;28(6):612–24. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2014.06.008.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  89. Luthar SS, Cicchetti D, Becker B. The construct of resilience: a critical evaluation and guidelines for future work. Child Dev. 2000;71(3):543–62. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00164.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  90. Southwick SM, Charney DS. The science of resilience: implications for the prevention and treatment of depression. Science. 2012;338(6103):79–82. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1222942.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  91. Poudel-Tandukar K, Chandler GE, Jacelon CS, Gautam B, Bertone-Johnson ER, Hollon SD. Resilience and anxiety or depression among resettled Bhutanese adults in the United States. Int J Soc Psychiatry. 2019;65(6):496–506. https://doi.org/10.1177/0020764019862312.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  92. Ristevska-Dimitrovska G, Stefanovski P, Smichkoska S, Raleva M, Dejanova B. Depression and resilience in breast cancer patients. Open Access Maced J Med Sci. 2015;3(4):661–5. https://doi.org/10.3889/oamjms.2015.119.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  93. Ackerman JM, Hill SE, Murray DR. The behavioral immune system: Current concerns and future directions. Soc Personal Psychol Compass. 2018. https://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12371.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  94. Makhanova A, Shepherd MA. Behavioral immune system linked to responses to the threat of COVID-19. Pers Individ Dif. 2020;167:110221. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2020.110221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  95. Stangier U, Kananian S, Schüller J. Perceived vulnerability to disease, knowledge about COVID-19, and changes in preventive behavior during lockdown in a German convenience sample. Curr Psychol. 2022;41(10):7362–70. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-021-01456-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  96. Biernacki P, Waldorf D. Snowball sampling: problems and techniques of chain referral sampling. Sociol Methods Res. 1981;10(2):141–63. https://doi.org/10.1177/004912418101000205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  97. Mundfrom DJ, Shaw DG, Ke TL. Minimum sample size recommendations for conducting factor analyses. Int J Test. 2005;5(2):159–68. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327574ijt0502_4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  98. Forero CG, Maydeu-Olivares A, Gallardo-Pujol D. Factor analysis with ordinal indicators: A monte carlo study comparing DWLS and ULS estimation. Struct Eqn Model. 2009;16:625-641. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705510903203573.

  99. Lonner WJ, Berry JW. Field methods in cross-cultural research. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications Inc; 1986.

    Google Scholar 

  100. Carleton RN, Norton MA, Asmundson GJ. Fearing the unknown: a short version of the intolerance of uncertainty scale. J Anxiety Disord. 2007;21(1):105–17. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2006.03.014.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  101. Lauriola M, Mosca O, Carleton RN. Hierarchical factor structure of the Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale short form (IUS-12) in the Italian version. TPM Test Psychom Methodol Appl Psychol. 2016;23(3):377–94. https://doi.org/10.4473/TPM23.3.8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  102. Bond FW, Hayes SC, Baer RA, Carpenter KM, Guenole N, Orcutt HK, Waltz T, Zettle RD. Preliminary psychometric properties of the acceptance and action questionnaire-II: a revised measure of psychological inflexibility and experiential avoidance. Behav Ther. 2011;42(4):676–88. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2011.03.007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  103. Pennato T, Berrocal C, Bernini O, Rivas T. Italian version of the acceptance and action questionnaire-II (AAQ-II): dimensionality, reliability, convergent and criterion validity. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. 2013;35(4):552–63. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10862-013-9355-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  104. Connor KM, Davidson JR. Development of a new resilience scale: the Connor-Davidson Resilience Scale (CD-RISC). Depress Anxiety. 2003;18(2):76–82. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.10113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  105. Campbell-Sills L, Stein MB. Psychometric analysis and refinement of the Connor-Davidson Resilience Scale (CD-RISC): validation of a 10-item measure of resilience. J Trauma Stress. 2007;20(6):1019–28. https://doi.org/10.1002/jts.20271.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  106. Di Fabio A, Palazzeschi L. Connor-Davidson Resilience Scale: psychometric properties of the Italian version. Couns Ital J Res Appl. 2012;5(1):101–9.

    Google Scholar 

  107. Henry JD, Crawford JR. The short-form version of the depression anxiety stress scales (DASS-21): construct validity and normative data in a large non-clinical sample. Br J Clin Psychol. 2005;44(Pt 2):227–39. https://doi.org/10.1348/014466505X29657.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  108. Bottesi G, Ghisi M, Altoè G, Conforti E, Melli G, Sica C. The Italian version of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales-21: factor structure and psychometric properties on community and clinical samples. Compr Psychiatry. 2015;60:170–81. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2015.04.005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  109. Bonacchi A, Chiesi F, Lau C, Marunic G, Saklofske DH, Marra F, Miccinesi G. Rapid and sound assessment of well-being within a multi-dimensional approach: the Well-Being Numerical Rating Scales (WB-NRSs). PLoS ONE. 2021;16(6):e0252709. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0252709.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  110. Kline RB. Principles and practices of structural equation modeling. 4th ed. New York: The Guilford Press; 2016.

    Google Scholar 

  111. Muthen B, Kaplan D. A comparison of some methodologies for the factor analysis of non-normal Likert variables: a note on the size of the model. Br J Math Stat Psychol. 1992;45(1):19–30. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8317.1992.tb00975.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  112. Lorenzo-Seva U, Ferrando PJ. FACTOR: a computer program to fit the exploratory factor analysis model. Behav Res Methods. 2006;38(1):88–91. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03192753.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  113. Timmerman ME, Lorenzo-Seva U. Dimensionality assessment of ordered polytomous items with parallel analysis. Psychol Methods. 2011;16(2):209–20. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023353.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  114. JASP Team. JASP (Version 0.16.3): 2022 [Computer software]. https://jasp-stats.org/.

  115. Li CH. The performance of ML, DWLS, and ULS estimation with robust corrections in structural equation models with ordinal variables. Psychol Methods. 2016;21(3):369–87. https://doi.org/10.1037/met0000093.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  116. Koğar H, Yılmaz KE. Comparison of different estimation methods for categorical and ordinal data in confirmatory factor analysis. J Meas Eval Educ Psychol. 2016. https://doi.org/10.21031/epod.94857.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  117. Chen FF. Sensitivity of goodness of fit indexes to lack of measurement invariance. Struct Equ Model. 2007;14(3):464–504. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705510701301834.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  118. Cheung GW, Rensvold RB. Evaluating goodness-of-fit indexes for testing measurement invariance. Struct Equ Model. 2002;9(2):233–55. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15328007sem0902_5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  119. Jeffreys H. The theory of probability. Oxford: OUP Oxford; 1998.

    Google Scholar 

  120. Ly A, Verhagen J, Wagenmakers EJ. An evaluation of alternative methods for testing hypotheses, from the perspective of Harold Jeffreys. J Math Psychol. 2016;72:43–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  121. Jarosz AF, Wiley J. What are the odds? A practical guide to computing and reporting Bayes factors. J Probl Solving. 2014;7(1):2. https://doi.org/10.7771/1932-6246.1167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  122. McCrae RR, Kurtz JE, Yamagata S, Terracciano A. Internal consistency, retest reliability, and their implications for personality scale validity. Pers Soc Psychol Rev. 2011;15(1):28–50. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868310366253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  123. Dong M, Zheng J. Letter to the editor: headline stress disorder caused by Netnews during the outbreak of COVID-19. Health Expect. 2020;23(2):259–60. https://doi.org/10.1111/hex.13055.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  124. Olatunji BO, Etzel EN, Tomarken AJ, Ciesielski BG, Deacon B. The effects of safety behaviors on health anxiety: an experimental investigation. Behav Res Ther. 2011;49(11):719–28. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2011.07.008.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  125. Rachman S, Radomsky AS, Shafran R. Safety behaviour: a reconsideration. Behav Res Ther. 2008;46(2):163–73. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2007.11.008.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  126. Oaten MJ, Stevenson RJ, Case TI. Compensatory up-regulation of behavioral disease avoidance in immuno-compromised people with rheumatoid arthritis. Evol Hum Behav. 2017;38(3):350–6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.11.006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  127. Stevenson RJ, Saluja S, Case TI. The impact of the Covid-19 pandemic on disgust sensitivity. Front Psychol. 2021;20(11):600761. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.600761.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  128. Miłkowska K, Galbarczyk A, Mijas M, Jasienska G. Disgust sensitivity among women during the COVID-19 outbreak. Front Psychol. 2021;12:622634. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.622634.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  129. Yamada Y, Xu H, Sasaki K. A dataset for the perceived vulnerability to disease scale in Japan before the spread of COVID-19. F1000Research. 2020;9:334. https://doi.org/10.12688/f1000research.23713.2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank all students who participated in this study and contributed to collecting data.

Funding

The authors received no specific funding for this work.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Conceptualization: FC; methodology: FC, GM, CT; formal analysis: FC, GM, CT; data curation: GM, CT; writing—original draft: GM, FC; writing—review and editing: FC, GM, CT, CL. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Francesca Chiesi.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the University of Florence and with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. The study was approved by the university’s local institutional review board (Commissione Etica per la Ricerca dell’Università degli Studi di Firenze, n. 148-prot. 0134386). Informed consent was obtained from all individual adult participants included in the study.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Chiesi, F., Marunic, G., Tagliaferro, C. et al. The psychometric properties and gender invariance of the Italian version of the Perceived Vulnerability to Disease Questionnaire (I-PVDQ) during the COVID-19 pandemic. BMC Psychol 10, 321 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-022-01023-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-022-01023-z

Keywords