Skip to main content

Commending rather than condemning: Moral elevation and stigma for male veterans with military sexual trauma

Abstract

Background

Using an experimental study, we examined the link between state moral elevation and stigmatic beliefs surrounding male veterans with military sexual trauma (MST).

Methods

Undergraduate students were presented with a video or written narrative of a male veteran self-disclosing how they struggled with and overcame MST (nā€‰=ā€‰292). Participants completed measures regarding trait and demographic characteristics at baseline, then measures immediately after the disclosure stimulus to assess immediate elevation and stigma-related reactions.

Results

Results suggest state-level elevation in response to a veteran self-disclosing their experience with MST was negatively correlated with harmful stigmatic beliefs about MST. A greater predisposition to experience elevation and PTSD symptoms were linked with stronger elevation responses to the stimulus.

Conclusion

Findings support the need for further exploration of elevation and its potential to impact public stigma for male veterans with MST.

Peer Review reports

Introduction

Broadly, stigma is based on a belief that a person or group with a specific trait is considered deviant, which is associated with negative stereotypes or perceptions of that person [1]. Stigma surrounding mental health in particular can include a wide range of negative stereotypes such as believing someone has personal responsibility for experiencing mental health issues, feelings of pity, anger, or fear toward that person, believing one should withhold help or avoid those with distress, and support for segregation and coercion [2]. Unfortunately, there is a substantial amount of stigma related to veteran mental health problems, particularly for male sexual assault [3]. Additional research is needed to understand novel pathways that could positively impact the public stigma surrounding military sexual trauma, which some veterans are forced to confront. Previous studies suggest one potentially relevant construct is moral elevationā€”an emotion experienced after witnessing another person perform a virtuous act [4]. This experimental study aims to investigate the link between moral elevation and stigma for male veterans who experienced sexual assault.

Military sexual trauma and public stigma

The military, along with most workplaces in the United States (US), is not immune from sexual harassment or assault. The rate of sexual assault experienced in the military is believed to mirror the rate in civilians with reports ranging from 22 to 33% for women and 1Ā toĀ 12% for men [5]. The actual rate of military sexual trauma (MST) is unknown for several reasons including potential biases, stigma, or fears associated with reporting assaults; however, it is estimated that 15.7% of current military personnel and veterans have reported MST during their time in service [6]. Despite the myth that men are rarely sexually assaulted, several nationwide surveys found that men are assaulted at a similar rate to women [7]. This is the same for the military, given that approximately 50% of MST survivors are male [8]. Following an assault, experiences of MST can lead to severe consequences including significant distress, posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), and potential functional impairment [9], Thus, many veterans are forced to confront the serious problem of experiencing MST and subsequent negative outcomes.

In the context of military service and post-deployment, previous work has established there are stigmatic beliefs surrounding mental health concerns and treatment-seeking in general [10, 11]. However, public stigma for MST in male veterans is particularly salient given societal expectations for gender and sexuality, along with myths about who can be raped or assaulted. Male rape myths can include beliefs that real men cannot be raped or can defend themselves to prevent it from happening, along with beliefs regarding homosexuality such as only gay men are raped or that rape survivors become gay [12]. Researchers have proposed that these societal expectations or stigma for assault survivors is an important source of psychological distress. For example, a qualitative study with a civilian sample found that young men who experienced sexual assault reported feeling shame, embarrassment, disempowerment, and emasculation, which was fueled by stigmatic beliefs regarding male victimization [13]. Findings suggest these feelings contributed to low rates of disclosure and an underreporting of assault in this sample. The issue of disclosure hesitancy is further compounded for service members given that both men and women may not report an assault out of fear of social retaliation in the military [14]. A separate study of male veterans also reported frequent negative reactions to disclosure that were harmful, whereas positive reactions reportedly aided the recovery process [15]; further highlighting that public perceptions of MST survivors can play an important role. Overall, public stigma surrounding this topic is particularly problematic insofar as it could contribute to reduced treatment seeking and support for male veteran with MST who are in need of additional care. Therefore, efforts to address consequences of stigma should consider novel pathways to reduce public stigma. One potential pathway that could help address this concern is moral elevation.

Moral elevation and links with stigma

Moral elevation (hereafter, elevation) is a positive emotion experienced after witnessing someone perform a virtuous or altruistic act [4, 16,17,18]. Elevation is further distinguished from other positive psychology constructs like admiration or gratitude by its emotional responses (feeling inspired, uplifted, moved), physical responses (tears in eyes, lump in throat) and subsequent motives to perform similar virtuous acts or engage in prosocial behavior [19,20,21,22,23]. Because the motives associated with elevation are antithetical to the negative attitudes and behaviors of stigma, it follows that eliciting elevation might be a useful counteracting agent to stigmatic beliefs.

Accordingly, elevation has been linked with a wide range of benefits and positive correlates including reduced stigma or prejudice against marginalized groups. For example, one study found participants who were exposed to elevating videos compared to a control condition reported reduced implicit and explicit prejudice toward gay men [24]. There is also evidence that inducing elevation is associated with more prosocial behaviors towards an outgroup or a minoritized group by socially dominant groups [25]. However, there are mixed findings that suggest elevation may not lead to significant differences in prejudice, such as reducing homophobia [26]. Therefore, further research is needed to better understand the link between elevation and stigma, particularly for stigmatic beliefs surrounding MST in male veterans. The authors are unaware of previous studies that have formally tested the potential impact of elevation on this specific stigmatic belief or examined the role of elevation in stigma towards veterans in general.

If elevation does in fact reduce stigma for MST, it would be important to expand our understanding of how to elicit this emotion and identify any characteristics that might predispose someone to feel elevated in this context. One approach that could possibly elicit elevation within the context of MST is to present a story about a male veteran disclosing his experience with assault. Given the societal expectations surrounding sexual assault survivors [12] and previous findings that highlight the hesitancy and fear associated with self-disclosure [13, 15], a story of a male veteran who openly shares his experiences with MST could be perceived as a remarkable act of courage. A story that also includes a description of his path to recovery and engagement with treatment could also demonstrate significant perseverance and hope. In terms of practical steps for using such a story to elicit elevation, past work has demonstrated elevation can be induced in experimental designs with different approaches such as viewing videos that display virtuous acts [16, 20, 21, 27], reading stories about virtuous behavior [25, 28], or using a recall technique [29, 30]. Yet few studies have directly examined the impact of induction method types to determine if there are significant differences in elevation responses between viewing videos versus reading written narratives, for example. Thus, attempts to elicit elevation using a story of MST self-disclosure should examine both video and written narrative formats to determine if either format is more effective for this subject.

In addition to the content and format of potential elevation stimuli, it is important to understand predisposing characteristics that might indicate who is likely to report a stronger elevation response to self-disclosure of MST. Although no known studies have examined relevant factors for eliciting elevation around this specific topic, there is some evidence that the capacity for inspiration or elevation is higher when the witness shares similarities with the target exemplar (i.e., person demonstrating virtuous act) or perceives that exemplar as relatable [31, 32]. In this case, the gender of the witness could play an important role since men might perceive the story differently in the context of societal beliefs or pressures regarding masculinity. Another important set of factors could be whether or not witnesses endorse PTSD symptoms themselves and whether they know someone who is a survivor of sexual assault. Experiencing PTSD symptoms could make the exemplar more relatable, and having endured those symptoms or known someone who suffered from a similar trauma might lead the viewer to have a greater appreciation for the level of courage, perseverance, or hope that is being demonstrated by an exemplar (thus, leading to higher elevation). Age might be another relevant characteristic to consider. Significant differences in age between the witness and the exemplar could be a barrier to perceived relatability and might negatively impact the elevation response. Additionally, previous studies found that age was positively correlated with elevation responses [33].

Beyond shared characteristics, innate beliefs about male rape myths could play an important role in elevation responses. As previously noted, existing myths include the belief that men cannot be truly raped, men who were raped are gay, or will become gay. Although no studies have directly examined the relation between elevation responses and male rape myths, it is plausible that if present, these biases and reductive perspectives could present a barrier to feeling inspired by a male veteran who discloses his experience with MST.

On the other hand, some trait characteristics might positively impact state elevation. The most relevant would be a greater predisposition to experience elevation, which could be described as trait-like elevation or the tendency to engage with moral beauty [34]. Past work found that people who report a greater tendency to experience elevation in response to acts of moral beauty or virtue endorse higher levels of state elevation following relevant stimuli [27, 35]. Another potential predictor could be dispositional empathy or the tendency to express empathy for others, which may impact a witnessā€™s perception of the exemplarā€™s moral character [36]; thus, contributing to positive assessments of a male veteran who experienced MST. Few studies have tested the direct relation between trait empathy and state elevation; however, there is preliminary support that empathy and elevation are positively correlated at the trait-level [27] and state-level [37], separately.

State elevation in response to a male veteran who self-discloses their history with MST could be impacted by a wide range of factors including shared characteristics or experiences, predispositions to respond to moral acts, and beliefs surrounding male rape myths. Predictors of state elevation in this context are largely unknown, yet it is important to identify who is likely to endorse positive responses, especially if elevation is linked with lower levels of stigma for MST.

Current study

The purpose of this study is to examine moral elevation and stigmatic beliefs about MST as potential responses to witnessing a veteran self-disclose their experience with MST. Specifically, this study included three aims. First, we examined elevation responses to the disclosure by inspecting descriptive statistics and assessed whether elevation responses differed based on the format of the disclosure (video versus transcribed formats). Next, we explored potential predictors of elevation responses, including theoretically relevant traits and characteristics that align with the veteran featured in the disclosure. Aims one and two were considered exploratory and did not include a priori hypotheses. Lastly, we assessed if higher elevation following a disclosure of MST was correlated with lower levels of stigma across nine domains. We hypothesized a negative correlation between elevation and harmful stigma reported.

Method

Participants

Participants were recruited from an undergraduate subject pool of psychology students from a university in the Southern United States. The initial sample size recruited was 323 participants. Seven participants were removed because they did not consent, 23 participants were excluded because they failed an attention screen, and 1 participant was removed because they had a large portion of missing data (29%). The final sample included 292 participants (Mage = 22.85, SDā€‰=ā€‰7.47; 82.53% female) with 46.6% identifying as White, 29.5% as Black, 22.6% as Hispanic, 2.7% as American Indian or Alaska Native, 2.4% as Asian or Asian American, 0.7% Native Hawaiian or Pacific Islander, and 3.1% as Other.

Procedure

Interested students followed a survey link that led to a brief summary of the study and then provided online consent. Within the survey, first, participants were presented with demographic questions that asked for their age, race/ethnicity, and gender. Participants were also asked whether they know someone who experienced sexual assault (yes/no). Next, participants were given a set of randomized baseline questionnaires that assessed traits and history relevant to MST. Following the baseline measures, all participants were presented with a vignette about a male veteran who experienced a sexual assault in the military and who sought treatment after struggling to disclose that experience for a period of time. Participants were randomly assigned to either (a) watch a video of that male veteran discussing his experience or (b) read a transcript of that same video describing the experiences of the same veteran. Lastly, the participants completed follow-up questionnaires immediately after the vignette that assessed their responses to the vignette, including a state elevation measure and a questionnaire about stigma toward that specific veteran. An attention screen (ā€œWhat branch was the veteran from?ā€) was also included for all participants within the final set of questionnaires to verify the participant watched or read the vignette. Participants with incorrect responses were excluded from data analysis. All participants consented to participate and study procedures were approved by the local Institutional Review Board.

Measures

Empathy

The Toronto Empathy Questionnaire [38] measured trait-level empathy. Participants rated the 16 items on a 0 (never) to 4 (always) scale. Sample items include ā€œIt upsets me to see someone being treated disrespectfullyā€ and ā€œI enjoy making other people feel better.ā€ All items were summed to create a total score ranging between 0 and 64 with higher scores representing greater empathy (Ī±ā€‰=ā€‰.89 [95% CI: .87, .91]).

Trait Moral Elevation

The Engagement with Beauty Scale [34] is designed to assess trait-like tendencies to feel moved or inspired by natural, artistic, and moral beauty. The moral beauty subscale was used to assess predisposition to experience moral elevation (i.e., trait elevation). Participants rated six items on a scale from 1 (very unlike me) to 7 (very like me). Sample items include ā€œI notice moral beauty in human beingsā€ and ā€œWhen perceiving an act of moral beauty, I find that I desire to become a better person.ā€ The items were summed to create a subscale score ranging between 6 and 42 with higher scores representing higher trait elevation (Ī±ā€‰=ā€‰.87 [.85, .89]).

PTSD symptoms

The Posttraumatic Diagnostic Scale [39] is a 17-item self-reportĀ questionnaire that was used to assess PTSD symptoms. Participants were asked to rate the extent they experienced items on a scale from 0 (not at all) to 3 (3ā€“5 or more time a week/very much/almost always). Sample items include ā€œHaving bad dreams or nightmares about the traumatic eventā€ and ā€œTrying not to think or talk about the traumatic event.ā€ Due to an error during survey construction, one item was missing in the survey that was administered to all participants (item 7: avoiding activities, people, and places). Therefore, we calculated the mean item score for the remaining 16 items as the total score ranging between 0 and 3 with higher scores representing more severe PTSD symptoms. Although this deviation from standard administration is a study limitation, internal consistency was still high (Ī±ā€‰=ā€‰.94 [.93, .95]).

Male rape myth beliefs

The Male Rape Myth Scale [40, 41] assessed false and stereotypical beliefs about male rape. Participants rated 22 items on a scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 6 (strongly agree). Sample items include ā€œAny healthy man can successfully resist a rapist if he really wants toā€ and ā€œA man who has been raped has lost his manhood.ā€ All items were summed to create a total score ranging between 22 and 132 with higher scores indicating greater acceptance of male rape myths (Ī±ā€‰=ā€‰.92 [.91, .93]).

Stigma response

The Attributions Questionnaire-27 [2] measured nine stereotypes about the specific veteran presented in the randomized vignette. Immediately after reviewing the vignette, participants rated the extent to which they agreed with 27 items, as they relate to the veteran self-disclosing MST, on a scale from 1 (none at all) to 9 (very much). This questionnaire was designed to assess nine factors that pertain to public attitudes, emotional affect, and behaviors surrounding stigma for mental health [42]. Accordingly, nine subscale scores were created by summing the three items for each factor, ranging between 3 and 27, with higher scores representing stronger endorsement of that stereotype. Subscales included stereotypes related to anger (e.g., ā€œI would feel aggravated by the veteran.ā€), blame (e.g., ā€œI would think it was the veteranā€™s own fault that he is in the present condition.ā€), pity (e.g., ā€œI would feel pity for the veteran.ā€), help (e.g., ā€œI would be willing to talk to the veteran about his problems.ā€), dangerousness (e.g., ā€œI would feel unsafe around the veteran.ā€), fear (e.g., ā€œThe veteran would terrify me.ā€), avoidance (e.g., ā€œIf I were an employer, I would interview the veteran for a job.ā€; reverse-scored), segregation (e.g., ā€œI think the veteran poses a risk to his neighbors unless he is hospitalized.ā€), and coercion (e.g., ā€œIf I were in charge of the veteranā€™s treatment, I would force him to take his medication.ā€). Internal consistency for subscales in this study were mostly adequate (see TableĀ 2) with the exception of blame (Ī±ā€‰=ā€‰.61 [.54, .69]) and coercion (Ī±ā€‰=ā€‰.47 [.37, .57]).

State moral elevation response

The State Moral Elevation Scale [35] measured state-level elevation in response to reviewing the veteran vignette. Participants rated the extent they experienced nine items using a scale from 0 (not at all) to 4 (extremely). Sample items include ā€œSomehow lifted up or in touch with the better parts of myselfā€ and ā€œMotivated to live in a nobler or virtuous way.ā€ All items were summed to create a total score ranging between 0 and 36 with higher scores representing greater elevation experienced (Ī±ā€‰=ā€‰.89 [.87, .91]).

Data analysis

All data management and analyses were conducted with R [43]. First, to examine whether elevation responses differed between the video versus transcript condition, we calculated an independent samples t-test with the t-test function from the base R stats package; Cohenā€™s d was calculated with the psych package. We interpreted dā€‰>ā€‰0.20, dā€‰>ā€‰0.50, and dā€‰>ā€‰0.80 as small, medium, and large effects [44]. For the second aim that examined baseline characteristics as predictors of the state elevation response, we used the lm function from the base R stats package to fit a linear regression model that included the following predictors, standardized: gender, age, trait elevation, trait empathy, PTSD symptoms, male rape myth beliefs, and whether or not they know someone who is a sexual assault survivor (yesā€‰=ā€‰1, noā€‰=ā€‰0). Lastly, to test our hypothesis that elevation is negatively correlated with stigma, correlations were calculated with the psych package [45].

Results

First, an independent samples t-test was performed to compare the mean level of state elevation experienced following the video vignette (Mā€‰=ā€‰28.08, SDā€‰=ā€‰7.83, nā€‰=ā€‰137) and transcript vignette (Mā€‰=ā€‰28.68, SDā€‰=ā€‰7.50, nā€‰=ā€‰155). Results indicated elevation was not statistically different between the two groups with no meaningful effect size (t = -0.66, pā€‰=ā€‰.509, dā€‰=ā€‰0.08).

Regarding baseline characteristics, results from the linear regression model indicated the only significant predictors of state elevation experienced after the vignette were trait elevation and PTSD symptoms (see TableĀ 1). Gender, age, empathy, male rape myth beliefs, and knowing a sexual assault survivor were all nonsignificant.

Table 1 Baseline characteristics as predictors of state elevation response to male MST disclosure

Bivariate correlations between state elevation and separate stigma domains, along with descriptive statistics for domain subscales are described in TableĀ 2. Elevation experienced following the MST vignette demonstrated a small, positive correlation with greater pity, and greater willingness to help, whereas there was a small, negative correlation with avoidance. All other correlations with the remaining stigma domains were not statistically significant with correlation sizes below 0.10.

Table 2 Descriptive statistics and bivariate correlations between state moral elevation and stigma domains

Discussion

The purpose of this study was to examine moral elevation and stigmatic beliefs as responses to a male veteran who disclosed his experience with MST. Results for exploratory aims indicated there was no significant difference in state elevation between participants who were presented with a video versus written narrative format of the disclosure. Additionally, results identified trait elevation and PTSD symptoms as baseline characteristics associated with higher state elevation responses. Lastly, we found evidence that partially supported our hypothesis that higher elevation after witnessing someone disclose MST would be correlated with lower negative stigma for that person.

Comparing moral elevation responses to video and written formats

First, there was no significant difference in elevation responses after watching a video or reading a transcript of that exact same video. These results could offer preliminary support to studies that have used either approach to elicit elevation in past work. One potential reason neither approach was superior could be that elevation is posited to be, in part, a cognitive experience that involves making a specific attribution about an observed act [34]. In this case, it appears that one stimulus type (i.e., video or transcript) did not elicit more elevation than the other when the content was the same.

Notably, our study only compared one vignette focusing on MST and treatment-seeking behavior across both induction methods. Perhaps, demonstrations of moral beauty with different stories or protagonist characteristics might elicit elevation more easily through either method. Future research should examine this further by assessing potential differences in elicited elevation by format when using a wide range of stories or persons (i.e., extending beyond stories about MST). If this approach is to be used to target public stigma, future work should also examine other potential formats of elevation induction methods (e.g., audio), which may expand options for conducting elevation research more broadly.

Predictors of moral elevation response to MST disclosure

Findings suggest trait elevation and PTSD symptoms were significantly associated with having a higher elevation response to the MST vignette. This is consistent with previous studies that found trait-like elevation is highly correlated with a state elevation response to witnessing moral beauty [27, 34]. However, to our knowledge, no known studies have examined current PTSD symptoms as a predictor of state elevation. Perhaps the narrative of this particular veteranā€™s trauma and recovery journey elicits more elevation in people with PTSD symptoms because they have a greater appreciation for the challenges that veteran faced, which would also be consistent with previous work that indicates similarities with the exemplar is a predictor of stronger responses [31, 32]. If experiencing state elevation can lead to desirable outcomes or correlates as indicated in previous research, results could suggest these types of narratives and disclosures might also benefit those who have experienced MST.

These findings are also important insofar as they may inform who should be targeted for attempts to elicit elevation in response to stigmatized topics, particularly within groups like veterans who are at risk for experiencing problems associated with MST. Creating opportunities for people to feel inspired by veterans self-disclosing a stigmatized experience of sexual assault, or mental health issues broadly, could be of interest to organizations that want to increase engagement for veterans with similar backgrounds (e.g., Veterans Affairs Medical Centers, Veteran Support Networks) or facilitate community engagement and support when veterans are transitioning to civilian life.

Correlations between moral elevation and stigma domains

Results demonstrated that higher elevation was correlated with greater pity, greater willingness to help, and lower avoidance. These findings are somewhat consistent with other research that found elevation was linked with lower levels of public stigma, broadly defined. For example, Freeman et al. [25] demonstrated that inducing elevation using a video about ā€œoutside groupsā€ could be used to reduce stigma and increased willingness to help outgroups. As previously noted, another study found evidence that elevation can reduce implicit and explicit sexual prejudice against gay men [24], aligning with the negative correlation found between elevation and avoidance-related stigma associated with MST. Although prejudice against homosexuality is different than stigma for males who were sexually assaulted regardless of sexuality, the two share similar prejudices or biases among the public [13, 46].

Notably, the correlations with elevation were positive for help and pity, whereas it was negatively correlated with avoidance. Corrigan et al. [2] define the help stereotype as, ā€œthe provision of assistance to people with mental illnessā€ and the pity stereotype as, ā€œsympathy because people are overcome by their illness.ā€ The fact that elevation was positively correlated with these two domains is consistent with the body of evidence that suggests elevation is linked with strong urges or motives to connect with and help others [16, 33]. Additionally, researchers have demonstrated that elevation is an approach-oriented emotion [23], which is also consistent with the negative correlation for the avoidance stereotype described as, ā€œstay away from people with mental illnessā€ [2].

However, somewhat unexpectedly, results indicated the significant associations were limited to pity, help, and avoidance. State elevation was not correlated with other stereotypes, including anger, blame, fear, segregation, and coercion. These results suggest that elevation may only relate to specific features of public stigma, but it is unclear why that might be the case. More research is needed to replicate these findings and examine potential mechanisms or shared features that would explain the differences in associations across stigma types. For example, future studies could consider the attribution model of discrimination against mental health [2] and assess whether elevation induction can influence different stages of the model such as beliefs about personal responsibility for causing the condition, affective reactions, and behavioral responses. This approach may inform if and how elevation could be used as an intervention tool to target public stigma.

Limitations and future directions

These findings should be interpreted while considering several limitations. First, participants were limited to undergraduate students at a university in the southern US, which is not representative of the overall population. It would be important to replicate these findings with a more diverse sample. Second, stigma was only measured after participants were exposed to the veteran narrative and we did not include a baseline assessment of stigma; therefore, it is unclear if significant correlations are a function of changes initiated by elevation or if they represent low levels of stigma at baseline. Future studies should aim to investigate the link between elevation and within-person changes in MST-related stigma. Third, we used a PTSD symptom measure that was based on the fourth version of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV-TR) rather than the recent fifth version (DSM-5). Additionally, this measure was missing one item in the final survey administration; therefore, future studies should aim to replicate the findings that PTSD symptoms are associated with elevation response to MST stimuli with updated measurement tools. Lastly, the results are primarily correlational and cannot establish a causal link between elevation and stigma. To fully understand how elevation is experienced and whether it can be used to target stigma, future research should assess causal relationships (e.g., the attribution model [2]) by using control conditions and explore changes in specific motivations or perceptions that might facilitate a reduction in stigmatic beliefs.

Conclusion

In this study, there was preliminary evidence that moral elevation was correlated with stigmatic beliefs for MST. Additionally, some of the characteristics that were linked with greater elevation responses were trait elevation and PTSD symptomsā€”a shared experience with the veteran exemplar. Overall, these findings help expand the current understanding of the link between elevation and stigma within the context of a stigmatized issue for male veterans. Furthermore, results inform the use of elevating induction methods and suggest the need for further research to explore the use of elevation in efforts to impact public perceptions of male MST as a stigmatized issue.

Data availability

The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on request.

References

  1. Goffman E. Embarrassment and social organization. In: Smelser NJ, Smelser WT, editors. Personality and social systems. Hoboken: Wiley; 1963. p. 541ā€“8. https://doi.org/10.1037/11302-050.

    ChapterĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  2. Corrigan P, Markowitz FE, Watson A, Rowan D, Kubiak MA. An attribution model of public discrimination towards persons with mental illness. J Health Soc Behav. 2003;44(2):162ā€“79.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  3. Ralston KM. An intersectional approach to understanding stigma associated with male sexual assault victimization. Sociol Compass. 2012;6(4):283ā€“92.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  4. Haidt J. Elevation and the positive psychology of morality. In: Flourishing: positive psychology and the life well-lived. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2003. p. 275ā€“89.

  5. Castro C, Kintzle S, Hassan A. The combat veteran paradox: Paradoxes and dilemmas encountered with reintegrating combat veterans and the agencies that support them. Traumatology. 2015;21(4):299ā€“310.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  6. Wilson LC. The prevalence of military sexual trauma: a meta-analysis. Trauma Violence Abuse. 2018;19(5):584ā€“97.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  7. Stemple L, Meyer IH. The sexual victimization of men in America: new data challenge old assumptions. Am J Public Health. 2014;104(6):e19-26.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  8. Oā€™Brien C, Keith J, Shoemaker L. Donā€™t tell: military culture and male rape. Psychol Serv. 2015;12(4):357ā€“65.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  9. Suris A, Lind L. Military sexual trauma: a review of prevalence and associated health consequences in veterans. Trauma Violence Abuse. 2008;9(4):250ā€“69.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  10. Kulesza M, Pedersen E, Corrigan P, Marshall G. Help-seeking stigma and mental health treatment seeking among young adult veterans. Mil Behav Health. 2015;3(4):230ā€“9.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  11. Richards LK, Goetter EM, Wojtowicz M, Simon NM. Stigma and health services use among veterans and military personnel. In: Parekh R, Childs EW, editors. Stigma and prejudice: Touchstones in understanding diversity in healthcare. Cham: Springer; 2016. p. 203ā€“26. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-27580-2_12.

    ChapterĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  12. Turchik JA, Edwards KM. Myths about male rape: a literature review. Psychol Men Masc. 2012;13:211ā€“26.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  13. Hlavka HR. Speaking of stigma and the silence of shame: young men and sexual victimization. Men Masc. 2017;20(4):482ā€“505.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  14. Warner CM, Armstrong MA. The role of military law and systemic issues in the militaryā€™s handling of sexual assault cases. Law Soc Rev. 2020;54(1):265ā€“300.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  15. Monteith LL, Gerber HR, Brownstone LM, Soberay KA, Bahraini NH. The phenomenology of military sexual trauma among male veterans. Psychol Men Masc. 2019;20(1):115ā€“27.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  16. Algoe SB, Haidt J. Witnessing excellence in action: the ā€˜other-praisingā€™ emotions of elevation, gratitude, and admiration. J Posit Psychol. 2009;4(2):105ā€“27.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  17. Haidt J. The moral emotions. In: Davidson RJ, Scherer KR, Goldsmith HH, editors. Handbook of affective sciences. Oxford University Press: Oxford; 2003. p. 852ā€“70.

    Google ScholarĀ 

  18. Keltner D, Haidt J. Approaching awe, a moral, spiritual, and aesthetic emotion. Cogn Emot. 2003;17(2):297ā€“314.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  19. Landis SK, Sherman MF, Piedmont RL, Kirkhart MW, Rapp EM, Bike DH. The relation between elevation and self-reported prosocial behavior: incremental validity over the five-factor model of personality. J Posit Psychol. 2009;4(1):71ā€“84.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  20. Schnall S, Roper J, Fessler DMT. Elevation leads to altruistic behavior. Psychol Sci. 2010;21(3):315ā€“20.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  21. Schnall S, Roper J. Elevation puts moral values into action. Social Psychol Pers Sci. 2012;3(3):373ā€“8.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  22. Van de Vyver J, Abrams D. Testing the prosocial effectiveness of the prototypical moral emotions: elevation increases benevolent behaviors and outrage increases justice behaviors. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2015;1:23ā€“33.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  23. de Vyver JV, Abrams D. Is moral elevation an approach-oriented emotion? J Posit Psychol. 2017;12(2):178ā€“85.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  24. Lai CK, Haidt J, Nosek BA. Moral elevation reduces prejudice against gay men. Cogn Emot. 2014;28(5):781ā€“94.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  25. Freeman D, Aquino K, McFerran B. Overcoming beneficiary race as an impediment to charitable donations: social dominance orientation, the experience of moral elevation, and donation behavior. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2009;35(1):72ā€“84.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  26. Bartoș SE, Sophie Russell P, Hegarty P. Heroes against homophobia: Does elevation uniquely block homophobia by inhibiting disgust? Cogn Emot. 2020;34(6):1123ā€“42.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  27. Diessner R, Iyer R, Smith MM, Haidt J. Who engages with moral beauty? J Moral Educ. 2013;42(2):139ā€“63.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  28. Siegel JT, Thomson AL, Navarro MA. Experimentally distinguishing elevation from gratitude: oh, the morality. J Posit Psychol. 2014;9(5):414ā€“27.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  29. Haidt J. The positive emotion of elevation. Prev Treat. 2000;3(3).

  30. Van Cappellen P, Saroglou V, Iweins C, Piovesana M, Fredrickson BL. Self-transcendent positive emotions increase spirituality through basic world assumptions. Cogn Emot. 2013;27(8):1378ā€“94.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  31. Han H, Kim J, Jeong C, Cohen GL. Attainable and relevant moral exemplars are more effective than extraordinary exemplars in promoting voluntary service engagement. Front Psychol. 2017;8:283.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  32. Lockwood P, Kunda Z. Superstars and me: Predicting the impact of role models on the self. J Personal Soc Psychol. 1997;73(1):91ā€“103.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  33. Aquino K, McFerran B, Laven M. Moral identity and the experience of moral elevation in response to acts of uncommon goodness. J Personal Soc Psychol. 2011 Apr;100(4):703ā€“18.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  34. Diessner R, Solom RD, Frost NK, Parsons L, Davidson J. Engagement with beauty: appreciating natural, artistic, and moral beauty. J Psychol. 2008;142(3):303ā€“32.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  35. McGuire AP, Hayden CL, Tomoum R, Kurz AS. Development and validation of the State Moral Elevation Scale: assessing state-level elevation across nonclinical and clinical samples. J Happiness Stud. 2022;23:2923ā€“46.

  36. Schaumberg RL, Mullen E. From incidental harms to moral elevation: the positive effect of experiencing unintentional, uncontrollable, and unavoidable harms on perceived moral character. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2017;73:86ā€“96.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  37. Xie C, Bagozzi RP, GrĆønhaug K. The impact of corporate social responsibility on consumer brand advocacy: the role of moral emotions, attitudes, and individual differences. J Bus Res. 2019;1:514ā€“30.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  38. Spreng RN, McKinnon MC, Mar RA, Levine B. The Toronto Empathy Questionnaire: Scale development and initial validation of a factor-analytic solution to multiple empathy measures. J Pers Assess. 2009;91(1):62ā€“71.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  39. Foa EB, Cashman L, Jaycox L, Perry K. The validation of a self-report measure of posttraumatic stress disorder: the posttraumatic diagnostic scale. Psychol Assess. 1997;9(4):445ā€“51.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  40. Melanson PK. Belief in male rape myths: a test of two competing theories (Doctoral dissertation). Ontario, Canada: Queenā€™s University, Kingston; 1999.

  41. Sleath E, Bull R. Male rape victim and perpetrator blaming. J Interpers Violence. 2010;25(6):969ā€“88.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  42. Corrigan PW, Watson AC, Warpinski AC, Gracia G. Stigmatizing attitudes about mental illness and allocation of resources to mental health services. Community Ment Health J. 2004;40(4):297ā€“307.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  43. R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing [Internet]. 2021. Available from: https://www.R-project.org/.

  44. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioural science. 2nd ed. Academic Press Inc.; 1988.

  45. Revelle W. psych: Procedures for Psychological, Psychometric, and Personality Research (Version 1.9.12.31) [computer software and manual] [Internet]. 2020. Available from: https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/psych/index.html.

  46. Lewis RJ, Derlega VJ, Griffin JL, Krowinski AC. Stressors for gay men and lesbians: life stress, gay-related stress, stigma consciousness, and depressive symptoms. J Soc Clin Psychol. 2003;22(6):716ā€“29.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This material is the result of work with resources and support by Texas A&M University Commerce and the University of Texas at Tyler. The views expressed herein are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the position or policy of the Department of Veterans Affairs or the United States Government.

Funding

None.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

SL, RD, and AM collected the data. AM analyzed the data. GS, AZ, KH, LC, and AM drafted the manuscript. SL, RD, and AM provided administrative, technical, and material support. All authors provided critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content. All authors read, discussed, and approved the final version of the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Adam P. McGuire.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects. All subjects were 18 years or older. Study procedures were approved by the Texas A&M University Commerce Institutional Review Board (Study ID# 1961). All methods and procedures were performed in accordance with the relevant guidelines and regulations of the local Institutional Review Board.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

None.

Additional information

Publisherā€™s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Staley, G., Zaidan, A.C.V., Henley, K. et al. Commending rather than condemning: Moral elevation and stigma for male veterans with military sexual trauma. BMC Psychol 10, 292 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-022-01002-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-022-01002-4

Keywords