Skip to main content
  • Study protocol
  • Open access
  • Published:

Underlying mechanisms in the relationship between stress and alcohol consumption in regular and risky drinkers (MESA): methods and design of a randomized laboratory study

Abstract

Background

Excessive alcohol consumption and alcohol use disorders (AUD) are among the leading preventable causes of premature morbidity and mortality and are considered a major public health concern. In order to reduce the individual and societal burden of excessive alcohol use, it is crucial to identify high-risk individuals at earlier stages and to provide effective interventions to prevent further progression. Stressful experiences are important risk factors for excessive alcohol consumption and AUDs. However, the underlying biological and psychological mechanisms are still poorly understood.

Methods

The project “Underlying mechanisms in the relationship between stress and alcohol consumption in regular and risky drinkers (MESA)” is a randomized controlled study that started in December 2018 and is conducted in a laboratory setting, which aims to identify moderators and mediators of the relationship between acute stress and alcohol consumption among regular and risky drinkers. Regular and risky drinkers are randomly assigned to a stress induction or a control condition. Several processes that may mediate (emotional distress, endocrine and autonomic stress reactivity, impulsivity, inhibitory control, motivational sensitization) or moderate (trait impulsivity, childhood maltreatment, basal HPA-axis activity) the relation between stress and alcohol consumption are investigated. As primary dependent variable, the motivation to consume alcohol following psychosocial stress is measured.

Discussion

The results of this study could help to provide valuable targets for future research on tailored interventions to prevent stress-related alcohol consumption.

Peer Review reports

Background

Excessive alcohol consumption and alcohol use disorders (AUD) are among the leading preventable causes of premature morbidity and mortality [1, 2]. They come along with an immense individual and societal burden and are considered a major public health problem [3]. The World Health Organization reported 3 million deaths due to harmful use of alcohol in their latest report [4]. In the age group 20–39 years, approximately 13.5% of the total deaths are attributable to alcohol [5]. In particular, men are considered at high risk to develop AUD [2] with global prevalence five times that in women with 8.6% and 1.7% for males and females, respectively [4]. However, latest data indicated that this gap is narrowing in recent years [6, 7]. Treatments for excessive alcohol use and AUD are initiated at a very late stage of symptom progression when adverse somatic and mental consequences have already occurred [8, 9]. It is therefore necessary to identify high-risk individuals at an earlier stage of alcohol consumption in order to reduce individual and societal burden and to implement effective interventions to prevent further progressions. Risk factors and underlying mechanisms promoting excessive alcohol use need to be identified for tailoring new preventive approaches.

Stress and alcohol use

Alcohol consumption is a commonly used coping strategy to reduce stress [10]. It is very well known that Stress increases the amount of alcohol consumed and the risk of relapse, but little is known about the psychological mechanisms that underlie these effects [11]. The experience of stressful events, defined as unpredictable or uncontrollable events that exceed the regulatory capacity of an organism and that could threaten an organism’s physical or psychosocial integrity [10, 11] has been identified as a major risk factor for excessive alcohol use and AUD [12, 13]. The impact of stress on alcohol use and the risk of AUDs depends on the type, age, duration, and severity of the stress experienced [14]. The consumption of alcohol is a habitual response to stressful situations in people with AUD [15]. Stress plays an important role at all levels of alcohol consumption, beginning with facilitation of initial use through early stages of transition to regular use and from regular to excessive use [16, 17, 18]. In AUD, alcohol use also represents a habitual response to stressful situations [15].

Mediators and moderators

Despite this well-established association between stress and alcohol use, the underlying mechanisms are complex and still not well understood. Studies trying to explain this association show inconsistent results. Stress does not necessarily lead to alcohol consumption in every person [19], which suggests the relevance of potential moderating factors. Several environmental, biological, and psychological factors that could moderate the relation between stress and alcohol consumption at different stages of alcohol use progression are discussed in the existing literature. The hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis plays an important role in this context as it is a major stress response pathway and has been studied extensively in relation to alcohol use [20]. Altered HPA axis regulation is associated with problematic alcohol use and dependence and the nature of this dysregulation varies with respect to the stages of progression toward AUD [21]. Glucocorticoid secretion upon activation of (HPA) axis by stressors is normally adaptive, and was discussed to promote coping after stressful events whereas excessive and prolonged HPA axis activation results in wear-and-tear on numerous physiological systems [22]. Furthermore, dysregulation in stress-related cortisol production is a risk factor for developing AUD [20]. Therefore, studies suggest that there might be a moderating effect on the relationship between stress and alcohol consumption by individual differences in basal cortisol secretion [18, 19]. Further, there is evidence from observational studies that childhood maltreatment moderates the association between stressful experiences and the development of alcohol use problems [23, 24]. Individuals with childhood trauma exposure, particularly abuse, neglect, or chaotic home environments, are at heightened risk for heavy alcohol consumption [24]. Further childhood maltreatment is associated with early alcohol use initiation, alcohol-related problem behaviors, and alcohol use disorders in adulthood [25]. Other possible moderators considered in this context are personality traits. Personality traits such as trait impulsivity reflect people’s characteristic patterns of thoughts, feelings, and behaviors and imply consistency over time and stability across situations [26]. Trait impulsivity was found to predict risk for alcohol use problems in general [27, 28, 29] and further moderates the association between stress and alcohol use [30, 31]. Although the consideration of these moderating factors might help to elucidate previous inconsistent findings on the association between stress and alcohol use and develop more targeted interventions, they have barely been considered in studies on its underlying mechanisms.

Regarding the underlying mechanisms of the relationship between stress and alcohol consumption, the idea of alcohol use as a dysfunctional coping strategy to self-medicate aversive emotional states following stressful experiences has long been the predominant model [32]. Although there is considerable empirical support for the self-medication hypothesis [32, 33, 34, 35], it is not able to fully explain the association between stressful experiences and alcohol use. Alcohol consumption does not necessarily reduce aversive emotional states [36, 37], violating the negative reinforcement assumption underlying the self-medication hypothesis. Therefore, knowledge on additional mechanisms beyond self-medication at different stages of alcohol use progression is required to explain the association between stress and alcohol use. Several relevant psychological and biological factors that might affect this relationship have been described in the literature [20, 38]. Acute stress activates an immediate reaction increasing cerebral and peripheral adrenalin and noradrenalin and a delayed endocrine response (via HPA axis) increasing glucocorticoids (mainly cortisol in humans) [39]. These systems affect different mechanisms relevant to alcohol use depending on the stage of alcohol use progression. At early stages of alcohol use progression, alcohol use leads to increased autonomic arousal and HPA axis activation. These effects potentiate both stress and alcohol-related effects on motivation and reinforcement learning [40] which can further facilitate alcohol use as a stress-related coping mechanism [38]. It further promotes the salience of drug-related cues known as attentional bias as these cues ‘grab the attention’ and further increase alcohol craving [41]. At later stages of alcohol use progression, binge and excessive alcohol consumption results in larger-scale adaptations in terms of a neuroendocrine tolerance response to stress and alcohol intake [10, 42] which may be involved in the transition from controlled to compulsive alcohol consumption [10, 43]. Also, a sensitization of motivational systems can manifest, again, in priority processing alcohol-related cues, i.e. attentional bias [44, 45]. The stress-induced sensitization at later stages of alcohol use progression is assumed to be active in parallel to the noradrenalin-related mechanisms [18]. Taken together, stress and stress system alterations by alcohol consumption could be associated with biased information processing, increased impulsivity and impaired control functions; a pattern that is known to be a key mechanism in the development of excessive alcohol use [46, 47].

Need for controlled laboratory studies

Most studies, addressing the association between stress and alcohol consumption are based on clinical populations with limited sample sizes and participants who already developed AUD. In this context different moderators and mediators leading to alcohol dependence are often center of the research question [48, 49]. There is need for research that investigates the underlying mechanisms that lead to AUD before it is manifested. Therefore, especially laboratory settings with non-clinic samples are suitable to investigate mediators and moderators on this relationship as they allow the investigation of specific mechanisms through randomized manipulation of the factor of interest and at the same time allow to control for confounding variables [50].

Aims and hypotheses

The present and ongoing study aims to fill this research gap by conducting an experimental laboratory design to investigate the underlying mechanisms of the association between stress and alcohol consumption (MESA) in the at-risk population of young men. Since these mechanisms are expected to differ depending on the stage of alcohol use, they are examined in regular and risky drinkers. Therefore, several processes that could mediate the relation between stress and alcohol consumption at different stages of alcohol use progression are assessed.

The research questions are as follows:

  1. 1.

    Does acute stress increase alcohol consumption in a laboratory setting?

  2. 2.

    What are the mediators of the association between acute stress and alcohol use?

  3. 3.

    What are the moderators of the association between acute stress and alcohol use?

  4. 4.

    Are effects of acute stress on alcohol use as well as moderators and mediators of this association different in risky drinkers compared to regular drinkers?

The following a priori hypothesis were formulated:

  1. 1.

    Acute stress increases alcohol consumption in a laboratory setting.

  2. 2.

    This effect is stronger in risky compared to regular drinkers.

  3. 3.

    Emotional distress, endocrine and autonomic stress reactivity as well as impulsivity account for most of the effect of stress on alcohol use in regular drinkers (mediation).

  4. 4.

    Emotional distress, endocrine and autonomic stress reactivity, impulsivity, attentional bias and craving account for most of the effect of stress on alcohol use in risky drinkers (mediation).

  5. 5.

    A history of childhood maltreatment, basal HPA-axis activity and impulsivity are related to a stronger effect of acute stress on alcohol consumption in regular and risky drinkers (moderation).

Methods/design

Study design

The MESA study is a randomized controlled study that started in December 2018 and is being conducted in a laboratory setting at the Medical School Hamburg. The study is divided into an online screening and a main examination, with detailed description in the following (“Procedure” section). The study has a four-group design. Participants are stratified into equal groups of regular and risky drinkers (with regular drinking being defined as average daily alcohol consumption of less than 24 g over the past 30 days and risky drinking being defined as average daily alcohol consumption of more than 24 g over the past 30 days [51]). Regular and risky drinkers are then randomly assigned to either an experimental (acute stress) or a control condition (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Study design

Research is conducted in accordance with national data protection acts, the revised declaration of Helsinki and Good Clinical Practice Guidelines. After complete description of the study, written informed consent is obtained from all participants. The study is approved by the Institutional Review Boards of Technische Universität Dresden (EK 522122016) and Medical School Hamburg (MSH-2020/114).

Inclusion and exclusion criteria

Males have a higher risk of developing drinking problems compared to females [52] and are more likely to report stress-induced drinking [6]. Therefore, only male individuals are included to reduce heterogeneity and potential of confounding factors (e.g. intake of oral contraceptives, menstrual cycle), especially in the biological measures. All participants have to be between 18–40 years old. The upper age limit results from the fact that most alcohol use problems develop during adolescence and young adulthood [53, 54, 55]. Further, eligible individuals have to drink alcohol at least occasionally and have beer as their favorite alcoholic drink since it is necessary for the success of the study that participants are familiar with alcoholic brands to recognize them in the attentional bias paradigm (“Assessment” section). In addition, it might be perceived as unethical to provide abstinent individuals with alcoholic beverages. Additionally, having a hair length of at least 2 cm is required to analyze hair cortisol concentrations [56] as a cumulative measure for basal cortisol secretion of the association between stress and alcohol use (for detailed description see 2.4 Biological measures). Exclusion criteria are lifetime psychotic symptoms, lifetime alcohol or any other substance use disorder, current psychological or psycho-pharmacological interventions and acute suicidality, current psychotropic or other medication or any somatic diseases that might confound the study measures, especially with regard to the endocrine measures, and alcohol consumption on the study day. All subjects meeting the inclusion criteria will be stratified into the reported groups.

A priori power analysis

A non-clinical target sample of 400 young men is aimed for the MESA study. A power analysis was conducted to calculate the needed sample size. A series of Monte Carlo simulations (each simulating 1000 ANOVA F tests) using the simpower program in STATA 12.1 [57] was run. The Monte Carlo simulations revealed that assuming a sample size of n = 200 per drinking stratum, statistical power ranges between 0.80 and 0.95 for different group size ratios. Given the stratified randomized design of the study, this results in the final group size of n = 100 (Fig. 1).

Recruitment and screening procedures

Participants are recruited via personal contacts, flyers and advertisement in university and public settings in Hamburg (cafés, bars, supermarkets, sports clubs; student dormitories) as well as via social media (e.g. Instagram, Facebook) and student job markets. In addition, advertising is made in lectures and on the university website.

All individuals willing to participate in the study have to complete an online screening in advance of the main assessment, where basic demographic variables as well as all in- and exclusion criteria are assessed. Further, the usual alcohol consumption is measured using a self-administered timeline follow-back consisting of a calendar on which participants provide retrospective reports of average daily alcohol intake for the past 30 days [58]. The information on daily alcohol consumption is used to allocate participants to the groups of regular and risky drinkers. All individuals meeting the inclusion criteria are then invited to participate in the main study.

Assessment

Person-related measures

Participants complete a comprehensive baseline assessment (questionnaire package) including the measures of the proposed moderators (childhood maltreatment, trait impulsivity), mediators (attentional bias to alcohol related stimuli, inhibitory control and impulsivity, and stress reactivity during the acute stressor), and variables that might affect the associations of interest (usual alcohol consumption, drinking motives, perceived stress, trait anxiety, difficulties in emotion regulation, psychological flexibility) (Table 1).

Table 1 Variables and assessment instruments

Biological measures

Hair strands are taken to reflect cumulative long-term cortisol secretion within two months prior to the respective assessment point [56]. The cumulative cortisol secretion consisting of basal cortisol secretion as well as stress-induced cortisol secretion, has been shown to be an important moderator of stress-related adverse consequences including increase in alcohol use [85, 86]. In addition, during the study four saliva samples are collected using Salivettes® “code blue” (Sarstedt, Nümbrecht, Germany) with synthetic swabs to measure free cortisol levels and alpha-amylase activity as biological indicators of stress reactivity (Fig. 5). The first saliva sample is taken immediately before the stress induction (for detailed description for the stress induction see “Sample storage, biochemical analyses and data preparation” section). The second saliva sample is taken right after the stress induction as well as 12 (3rd salvia sample) and 24 (4th salvia sample) minutes after the stress induction. Cortisol is the final output of the hypothalamic pituitary adrenal (HPA) axis, and is among the most frequently used biological markers of psychological stress [87, 88]. Moreover, given that the biologically active (free) fraction of cortisol is reflected in saliva, it can be a preferred measure relative to serum cortisol [87, 89]. In addition, alpha-amylase is an enzyme component of saliva and has been proposed as a marker for stress-induced activity of the sympathetic nervous system (SNS). The advantage of a saliva-based measure of SNS activity is the convenience of assessing activity of both major stress systems (i.e. SNS and HPA-axis) in a single test tube, without the need for technically sophisticated instrumentation [63].

Behavioral measures

In addition to self-report measures, three behavioral tasks are conducted to measure attentional bias to alcohol related stimuli, inhibitory control and impulsivity as possible mediators in the association between stress and alcohol use [18, 90]. Attentional bias towards alcohol-related cues is measured using a dot-probe task (Fig. 2), which was programmed based on previous tasks in similar settings [41, 90]. Subjects are presented with pairs of matched alcoholic (beer) and non-alcoholic beverages for 500 ms (stimulus-onset asynchrony, SOA). Another SOA of 100 ms will be added to the paradigm in the proposed study to be able to capture automatic initial reactions (see [91]). Stimuli were chosen based on expert ratings regarding similarity in color, shape and recognition. Subjects respond to a probe that appears behind either the alcoholic or the non-alcoholic beverage. The difference in reaction time between alcoholic and non-alcoholic stimuli is a measure of attentional bias towards alcohol-related cues. Although the dot-probe task is a widely used paradigm to measure attentional biases, there is debate about its reliability [92, 93]. A new trial-based conceptualization of attentional bias has been proposed, which can increase reliability [94].

Fig. 2
figure 2

Dot-probe task

Inhibitory control is measured using a go/no-go task (Fig. 3) where participants are presented with 320 trials (280 go and 40 nogo trials) of stimuli containing two dots. Each dot pair is displayed for 500 ms and is arranged horizontally or vertically. Horizontally arranged dots indicate go-trials where participants have to press the response key as fast as possible while participants are instructed to withhold when seeing vertically arranged dots. Since there is evidence that participants balance the speed-accuracy trade-off differently [95], the dependent measure of the go/nogo task is the balanced integration score (BIS). This score is calculated in two steps. First, the responsive times (RTs) as well as the proportions of correct responses (PCs) are standardized. Second, one standardized score is subtracted from the other [96].

Fig. 3
figure 3

Go-nogo task

The delay discounting task as measure of impulsivity was taken from a task battery developed by Pooseh et al. ([65]; MATLAB scripts available from https://github.com/spooseh/VBDM) and is described in detail in Kräplin et al. [66]. The task consisted of 30 trials. Participants had to decide between a smaller financial gain delivered sooner and a larger financial gain delivered later. The two options were simultaneously presented on a computer screen using the Psychophysics Toolbox [97] in MATLAB R2018a (MathWorks Inc., Natick, MA). Between the shorter and later choice options, delays were 3, 7, 14, 31, 61, 180, and 365 days. Monetary gains ranged from 0.30 to 10 €. A Bayesian adaptive algorithm was implemented. This way, the parameter estimation is updated after each trial and serves as the basis for the calculation of the options in the next trial. The method was used to determine the most informative offers nearest to the individual’s point of indifference between two choice alternatives (i.e. indifference point). Thus, decision-making parameters can be efficiently inferred without the use of post-hoc parameter estimations. A hyperbolic value function was generated to describe the decline of subjective values of delayed reward according to the discounting rate k (Mazur 1987). Individuals with higher impulsivity are assumed to display higher k values (Fig. 4).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Schematic overview of the tasks in the decision-making battery. a Delay discounting task. b Probability discounting for gains. c Probability discounting for losses. d Mixed gambles task [66]

Stress induction

Stress is induced with the Trier Social Stress Test (TSST) [98] as one of the most frequently inserted research tools for the induction of acute psychosocial stress in experimental, laboratory research worldwide. The TSST is a standardized laboratory protocol, which provides a reliable and ecologically valid stressor [98]. The TSST contains elements of social evaluative threat and uncontrollability, which are associated with high cortisol responses [99]. The test is divided in three equal five-minute parts. It begins with a preparation period, followed by a free speech for a job interview and finishes with an arithmetic task. All tasks are held in front of a two-person audience. The TSST leads to robust changes in the hypothalamus–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis and the autonomic stress response compared to other stress induction paradigms [39, 100].

In the control condition, subjects participate in a Placebo-TSST, which is comparable in time and task division but without any audience and stress exposure for the participants [101]. It starts with a preparation period, followed by a free speech about the last vacation and finishes with a simple task of counting forward. Furthermore, participants are standing during the two tasks. This creates a setting that is as close as possible to the TSST, but does not contain stressful components (evaluative threat and uncontrollability).

Ad-libitum taste test

After completion of the behavioral tasks and the intervention, participants are asked to take part at an ad-libitum taste test as a covert measure for alcohol consumption. The ad-libitum taste test is a widely used method, which provides an unobtrusive and indirect measure of participants’ motivation to drink alcohol [84]. All participants are given two 0.33 l glasses of beer (two brands each containing 5% alcohol) and two 0.33 l glasses containing different soft drinks. Participants are instructed that they have 15 min to taste each glass to rate qualities about each drink (e.g. gassy, bitter). Participants are told to drink whatever amount necessary to make accurate judgements. The dependent variable is the amount of alcoholic beverage (beer) consumed and can range between 0 and 666 ml (equals 26.64 g ethanol). Non-alcoholic drinks are presented to control for the potential effect of thirst. The ad-libitum taste test is a valid method for the assessment of alcohol intake in the laboratory supported by strong associations between ad-libitum consumption and typical alcohol consumption [84]. It is also robust against several potential confounders such as time of day or participant awareness [84]. The taste test has been used to investigate a number of potential influences on alcohol consumption, including alcohol cues [102, 103, 104], impulse control [105, 106], and social influences [107], and it has been used to establish initial proof of concept for novel behavioral interventions [108, 109, 110].

Procedure

The main assessments are conducted between 14–20 p.m. in order to reduce the variance in biological measures (e.g. saliva cortisol) due to diurnal rhythms [111]. It is also likely that the willingness to drink alcohol is smaller in the morning than in the evening while there is no influence of day time on alcohol consumption in the ad libitum taste test between 14p.m. and 20p.m. [84]. Figure 5 gives an overview of the main study procedure. First, participants are asked to provide written informed consent. Participants’ absence from alcohol is verified by taking a breathalyzer reading with any value above zero leading to the immediate end of the examination. Hair strands for basal cortisol secretion are taken scalp-near from a posterior vertex position to be able to reflect basal cortisol secretion within two months prior to the respective assessment point. Then participants complete the baseline questionnaires including the measures of the proposed moderators (childhood maltreatment, trait impulsivity) and variables that might affect the associations of interest (usual alcohol consumption, drinking motives, stressful life events, trait anxiety, difficulties in emotion regulation, psychological flexibility). Subsequently, participants either take part in the stress induction (experimental condition) or placebo intervention (control condition) followed by behavioral assessments. Deviating from previous TSST protocols, the stress condition is maintained during the behavioral assessments. Therefore, participants are instructed that the TSST panel remain observing and evaluating the given performance during the computer tasks and further the camera is still pointed on the participant. The ad libitum taste-test is the last assessment of the procedure. After the taste test, all participants are debriefed about the true study purposes including the TSST procedure. Moreover, repeated breathalyzer readings are taken until blood alcohol concentration reaches 0.0‰ in two consecutive measures. Participants willing to leave before blood alcohol concentration reaches 0.0‰ have to confirm that they do not drive when leaving the laboratory. Participants who insist to leave with a blood alcohol concentration still being higher than 0.4‰ (only expected in rare cases) are sent home with a taxi.

Fig. 5
figure 5

Study procedure. Note: IC Informed consent, TSST Trier social stress test 

Sample storage, biochemical analyses and data preparation

The saliva samples are taken using salivette ‘code blue’ devices (Sarstedt, Nümbrecht, Germany) directly before the intervention and at three time points after the intervention (Fig. 5). Saliva samples are stored at − 20 °C in a laboratory freezer. After thawing, saliva samples will be centrifuged for 10 min at 4000 rpm. Salivary cortisol concentrations will be determined using a commercially available chemiluminescence assay (CLIA, IBL-Hamburg, Germany). Concentrations of salivary alpha-amylase will be detected by using an in-house enzyme kinetic method according to the protocol described in [63]. Hair cortisol concentrations will be determined via liquid chromatography tandem mass spectrometry (for detailed information on analysis methods see [56]).

Dimensional variables that are not normally distributed (expected e.g. for hair cortisol concentration, salivary cortisol and alpha amylase) will be Box–Cox transformed towards normal distribution. For all biological and behavioral variables, participants with outlying values of more than three standard deviations above the mean will be excluded from the respective analysis. Besides, robust linear regressions will complement conventional linear regressions because they down weight observations with large residuals to meet the assumption of equal variances of residuals. Composite measures of the entire cortisol secretion during the TSST (area under the curve with respect to ground; AUCG) and the cortisol stress reactivity (area under the curve with respect to increase; AUCi) will be calculated [112]. Analysis with these variables will be adjusted for initial cortisol concentration to alleviate confounding risk as AUC variables may be comprised of variance due to stress reactivity and stress-unrelated HPA axis activity [113]. All analysis including cortisol secretion during the TSST will be run twice with all participants in the first and with exclusion of non-responders to the TSST (increase of 1.5 nmol/l compared to baseline [114]) in the second run. With regard to alpha-amylase, both AUC measures and peak minus baseline levels will be calculated.

Statistical analyses

Main effects of stress exposure (stress vs control group) on alcohol consumption (amount of alcoholic beverage consumed) will be determined using linear regressions adjusting for the amount of non-alcoholic beverages consumed (which reduces unspecific variance in outcome). However, in case of considerable by chance differences in baseline characteristics between the two groups despite randomization, these characteristics will be included in the regression model if they are associated with alcohol consumption. To address potential biases related to missing data, we will conduct sensitivity analyses using multiple imputation.

Moderation analyses: Moderators are defined causally [50]. Linear regressions with interaction terms will be applied to test whether stress effects on alcohol consumption are moderated by childhood maltreatment, hair cortisol concentration and trait impulsivity (with main effects terms and interaction term, e.g. group × childhood maltreatment). Significant interactions indicate that a respective factor (moderator) predicts different effects of stress on alcohol assumptions. To approach causal conclusions, we will fit these models again while adjusting for shared factors of moderators and outcomes (e.g. previous stressful events, previous alcohol use) [50].

Mediation analyses: Mediators are also defined causally according to the counterfactual definition of Robins and Greenland [115] that is implemented in the ‘paramed’ package in Stata. This module allows dividing the estimated total stress effect (stress vs. control group) on alcohol consumption into a direct effect and an indirect effect mediated through stress reactivity (saliva cortisol, alpha amylase, self-reported stress), impulsivity (delay discounting), inhibitory control, and motivational sensitization (attentional bias). Mediation analyses will be adjusted for putative sociodemographic (e.g. age) and other shared factors of a potential mediator and outcome (e.g. time of day, preference of beer) as well as for the mentioned factors for moderation. The alpha level will be specified at two-sided 0.05. If necessary, the analyses will be repeated with robust standard errors (via the sandwich estimator) and robust linear regressions [116]].

Study progress and preliminary feasibility data

The data collection of the presented MESA study started in December 2018. From December 2018 until March 2022 N = 623 persons participated in the online screening. A total of 213 complete data sets have been collected so far. All participants are male and between 18–40 (M = 25) years old. 97 of the 213 participants took part in the stress condition, stratified in 40 risky drinkers and 57 regular drinkers. Further, 117 participants took part in the control condition stratified in 36 risky drinkers and 81 regular drinkers. More than half reported they were university students.

Due to the Covid-19 pandemic, the study was paused in the beginning of March 2020 in order to protect the safety and health of all personnel involved in the study and to comply with legislative regulations. The laboratories reopened in September 2021 and data collection was continued.

Discussion

The present MESA study was developed in response to the incomplete understanding of the underlying mechanisms of the relationship between stress and alcohol consumption. As pointed out, there is a significant amount of people suffering from AUDs with tremendous consequences for the individual as well as for society and health care systems. There is need for preventive interventions at the biological, psychological or social level for individuals at high risk of problematic alcohol consumption before the manifestation of AUD. Research to date has focused primarily on secondary prevention, which aims to prevent AUD progression and relapse, and tertiary prevention, which aims to minimize functional deterioration in chronic AUDs [117]. The present study focuses on the identification of targets for primary prevention, which is focused on the protection of healthy individuals, and may be provided on a universal, selective or indicated level. The various tasks designed to examine different, potential moderators and mediators can then be used to develop interventions and provide information for the at-risk population. The identification of specific mediators is of key importance as they help to elucidate what mechanisms underlie the association between stress and alcohol consumption. Knowledge about specific mechanisms are of high relevance as it can be used to allocate existing interventions. For example, there are already trainings for many of the investigated mediators (e.g. Attentional Bias Modification, Inhibitory Control trainings), which, should these factors prove to be relevant, could then be specifically adapted and applied in the context of stress [118, 119, 120, 121]. It can also be used to develop novel interventions that might be useful to prevent stress-related alcohol consumption. Identifying specific moderators will help to tailor these preventive interventions to at high-risk individuals, which increases their potential efficacy and cost-effectiveness.

Given its focus on internal validity using a carefully controlled design in a laboratory setting, external validity will be a limitation of this study. Thus, findings will have to be complemented by investigations in real world settings to make definite conclusions about the association between stress and alcohol use and its underlying mechanisms. This could be achieved for example with ecological momentary assessments, which have shown good feasibility in a couple of promising recent studies on stress-related alcohol use and the role of craving, alterations in mood and inhibitory control [122, 123, 124, 125].

Taken together, the presented study has a high potential to advance our understanding of stress-related alcohol use. In the long-term, it could stimulate the development of tailored preventive interventions and contribute to a reduction of problematic alcohol use.

Availability of data and materials

The data will be made available on the OSF after completion of the first data analyses.

Abbreviations

AUC:

Area under the curve

AUCG :

Area under the curve with respect to ground

AUCI :

Area under the curve with respect to increase

AUD:

Alcohol use disorder

BIS:

Balanced integration score

DERS:

Difficulties in emotion regulation scale

DMQ-R:

Drinking Motive questionnaire—revised

HPA-axis:

Hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis

MBDF:

Multidimensional Mood State Questionnaire

MESA:

Underlying mechanisms in the relationship between stress and alcohol consumption in regular and risky drinkers

PCs:

Proportion of correct responses

PSS:

Perceived Stress Scale

RTs:

Response times

SNS:

Sympathetic nervous system

STAI:

State-trait-anxiety-inventory

TSST:

Trier Social Stress Test

References

  1. Carvalho AF, Heilig M, Perez A, Probst C, Rehm J. Alcohol use disorders. The Lancet. 2019;394(10200):781–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Rehm J, Shield KD. Global burden of disease and the impact of mental and addictive disorders. Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2019;21(2):10.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Rehm J, Baliunas D, Borges GLG, Graham K, Irving H, Kehoe T, et al. The relation between different dimensions of alcohol consumption and burden of disease: an overview. Addiction. 2010;105(5):817–43.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. World Health Organization, World Health Organization, World Health Organization, Management of Substance Abuse Team. Global status report on alcohol and health 2018. 2018.

  5. Bloomfield K, Kraus L, Soyka M, Koch-Institut R. Gesundheitsberichterstattung des Bundes. Berlin, Heidelberg: Robert-Koch-Institut; 2008 S. 34. Report No.: 40.

  6. Peltier MR, Verplaetse TL, Mineur YS, Petrakis IL, Cosgrove KP, Picciotto MR, et al. Sex differences in stress-related alcohol use. Neurobiol Stress. 2019;10:100149.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. White A, Castle IJP, Chen CM, Shirley M, Roach D, Hingson R. Converging patterns of alcohol use and related outcomes among females and males in the United States, 2002 to 2012. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2015;39(9):1712–26.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Rehm J, Allamani A, Elekes Z, Jakubczyk A, Manthey J, Probst C, et al. Alcohol dependence and treatment utilization in Europe—a representative cross-sectional study in primary care. BMC Fam Pract. 2015;16(1):90.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Trautmann S, Pieper L, Kuitunen-Paul S, Manthey J, Wittchen HU, Bühringer G, et al. Prävalenz und Behandlungsraten von Störungen durch Alkoholkonsum in der primärärztlichen Versorgung in Deutschland. SUCHT. 2016;62(4):233–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Sinha R. How does stress lead to risk of alcohol relapse? Alcohol Res Curr Rev. 2012;34(4):432.

    Google Scholar 

  11. McGrath E, Jones A, Field M. Acute stress increases ad-libitum alcohol consumption in heavy drinkers, but not through impaired inhibitory control. Psychopharmacology. 2016;233(7):1227–34.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Becker HC. Influence of stress associated with chronic alcohol exposure on drinking. Neuropharmacology. 2017;1(122):115–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Hasin D, Keyes KM. The epidemiology of alcohol and drug disorders. In: Addiction medicine. Berlin: Springer; 2011. p. 23–49.

  14. Keyes KM, Hatzenbuehler ML, Grant BF, Hasin DS. Stress and alcohol: epidemiologic evidence. Alcohol Res Curr Rev. 2012;34:391–400.

    Google Scholar 

  15. Marlatt G. Taxonomy of high-risk situations for alcohol relapse: evolution and development of a cognitive-behavioral model. Addict (Abingdon Engl). 1997;91(1):S37-49.

    Google Scholar 

  16. Blomeyer D, Treutlein J, Esser G, Schmidt MH, Schumann G, Laucht M. Interaction between CRHR1 gene and stressful life events predicts adolescent heavy alcohol use. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;63(2):146–51.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Kingston S, Raghavan C. The relationship of sexual abuse, early initiation of substance use, and adolescent trauma to PTSD. J Trauma Stress. 2009;22(1):65–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Lijffijt M, Hu K, Swann AC. Stress modulates illness-course of substance use disorders: a translational review. Front Psychiatry. 2014. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00083/abstract.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Spanagel R, Noori HR, Heilig M. Stress and alcohol interactions: animal studies and clinical significance. Trends Neurosci. 2014;37(4):219–27.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Stephens MAC, Wand G. Stress and the HPA Axis. Alcohol Res Curr Rev. 2012;34(4):468–83.

    Google Scholar 

  21. Wand G. The influence of stress on the transition from drug use to addiction. Alcohol Res Health J Natl Inst Alcohol Abuse Alcohol. 2008;31(2):119–36.

    Google Scholar 

  22. McEwen BS, Gianaros PJ. Stress-and allostasis-induced brain plasticity. Annu Rev Med. 2011;62:431–45.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Keyes KM, Shmulewitz D, Greenstein E, McLaughlin K, Wall M, Aharonovich E, et al. Exposure to the Lebanon War of 2006 and effects on alcohol use disorders: the moderating role of childhood maltreatment. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2014;134:296–303.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Kim JH, Martins SS, Shmulewitz D, Santaella J, Wall MM, Keyes KM, et al. Childhood maltreatment, stressful life events, and alcohol craving in adult drinkers. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2014;38(7):2048–55.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Young-Wolff KC, Kendler KS, Prescott CA. Interactive effects of childhood maltreatment and recent stressful life events on alcohol consumption in adulthood. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2012;73(4):559–69.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Diener E, Lucas RE. Personality traits. Gen Psychol Required Read. 2019;278.

  27. de Wit H. Impulsivity as a determinant and consequence of drug use: a review of underlying processes. Addict Biol. 2009;14(1):22–31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. de Wit H, Richards JB. Dual determinants of drug use in humans: reward and impulsivity. Neb Symp Motiv Neb Symp Motiv. 2004;50:19–55.

    Google Scholar 

  29. Stamates AL, Lau-Barraco C. Momentary patterns of impulsivity and alcohol use: A cause or consequence? Drug Alcohol Depend. 2020;217:108246.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Boileau I, Dagher A, Leyton M, Gunn RN, Baker GB, Diksic M, et al. Modeling sensitization to stimulants in humans: an [11C]raclopride/positron emission tomography study in healthy men. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006;63(12):1386–95.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Cuomo C, Sarchiapone M, Giannantonio MD, Mancini M, Roy A. Aggression, impulsivity, personality traits, and childhood trauma of prisoners with substance abuse and addiction. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse. 2008;34(3):339–45.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Khantzian EJ. The self-medication hypothesis of substance use disorders: a reconsideration and recent applications. Harv Rev Psychiatry. 1997;4(5):231–44.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Crum RM, La Flair L, Storr CL, Green KM, Stuart EA, Alvanzo AAH, et al. Reports of drinking to self-medicate anxiety symptoms: longitudinal assessment for subgroups of individuals with alcohol dependence: research article: self-medication and alcohol dependence. Depress Anxiety. 2013;30(2):174–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Kushner M. The relationship between anxiety disorders and alcohol use disorders A review of major perspectives and findings. Clin Psychol Rev. 2000;20(2):149–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Lazareck S, Robinson JA, Crum RM, Mojtabai R, Sareen J, Bolton JM. A longitudinal investigation of the role of self-medication in the development of comorbid mood and drug use disorders: findings from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions (NESARC). J Clin Psychiatry. 2012;73(5):e588-593.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Le Berre AP. Emotional processing and social cognition in alcohol use disorder. Neuropsychology. 2019;33(6):808–21.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  37. Petit G, Luminet O, Maurage F, Tecco J, Lechantre S, Ferauge M, et al. Emotion regulation in alcohol dependence. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2015;39(12):2471–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Blaine SK, Sinha R. Alcohol, stress, and glucocorticoids: from risk to dependence and relapse in alcohol use disorders. Neuropharmacology. 2017;122:136–47.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Schommer NC, Hellhammer DH, Kirschbaum C. Dissociation between reactivity of the hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal axis and the sympathetic-adrenal-medullary system to repeated psychosocial stress. Psychosom Med. 2003;65(3):450–60.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Lee S, Rivier C. Alcohol increases the expression of type 1, but not type 2α corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) receptor messenger ribonucleic acid in the rat hypothalamus. Mol Brain Res. 1997;52(1):78–89.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Field M, Powell H. Stress increases attentional bias for alcohol cues in social drinkers who drink to cope. Alcohol Alcohol. 2007;42(6):560–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Koob GF, Le Moal M. Plasticity of reward neurocircuitry and the „dark side“ of drug addiction. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8(11):1442–4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Sinha R. The clinical neurobiology of drug craving. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2013;23(4):649–54.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  44. Field M, Munafò MR, Franken IHA. A meta-analytic investigation of the relationship between attentional bias and subjective craving in substance abuse. Psychol Bull. 2009;135(4):589–607.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  45. Franken IHA. Drug craving and addiction: integrating psychological and neuropsychopharmacological approaches. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2003;27(4):563–79.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Stamates AL, Lau-Barraco C. The dimensionality of impulsivity: Perspectives and implications for emerging adult drinking. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2017;25(6):521–33.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Wiers RW, Bartholow BD, van den Wildenberg E, Thush C, Engels RCME, Sher KJ, et al. Automatic and controlled processes and the development of addictive behaviors in adolescents: a review and a model. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2007;86(2):263–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Blaine SK, Nautiyal N, Hart R, Guarnaccia JB, Sinha R. Craving, cortisol and behavioral alcohol motivation responses to stress and alcohol cue contexts and discrete cues in binge and non-binge drinkers. Addict Biol. 2019;24(5):1096–108.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Kim ST, Hwang SS, Kim HW, Hwang EH, Cho J, Kang JI, et al. Multidimensional impulsivity as a mediator of early life stress and alcohol dependence. Sci Rep. 2018;8(1):4104.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  50. VanderWeele T. Explanation in causal inference: methods for mediation and interaction. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2015. p. 729S.

    Google Scholar 

  51. Herrick C. Governing health and consumption: sensible citizens, behaviour and the city. Bristol: Policy Press; 2011. p. 265S.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  52. Institut Für Therapieforschung (IFT), München, Institut Für Klinische Psychologie Und Psychotherapie Der Technischen Universität Dresden, Bundesministerium Für Gesundheit Und Soziale Sicherung, Berlin. Epidemiological Survey on Substance Abuse in Germany 2012 (ESA)Repräsentativerhebung zum Gebrauch und Missbrauch psychoaktiver Substanzen bei Erwachsenen in Deutschland (Epidemiologischer Suchtsurvey 2012) [Internet]. GESIS Data Archive; 2014 [zitiert 8. Dezember 2021]. https://doi.org/10.4232/1.12042

  53. Crum RM, Chan YF, Chen LS, Storr CL, Anthony JC. Incidence rates for alcohol dependence among adults: prospective data from the Baltimore Epidemiologic Catchment Area Follow-Up Survey, 1981–1996. J Stud Alcohol. 2005;66(6):795–805.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Grant BF, Goldstein RB, Chou SP, Huang B, Stinson FS, Dawson DA, et al. Sociodemographic and psychopathologic predictors of first incidence of DSM-IV substance use, mood and anxiety disorders: results from the Wave 2 National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. Mol Psychiatry. 2009;14(11):1051–66.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Kessler RC, Amminger GP, Aguilar-Gaxiola S, Alonso J, Lee S, Ustün TB. Age of onset of mental disorders: a review of recent literature. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2007;20(4):359–64.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  56. Stalder T, Kirschbaum C. Analysis of cortisol in hair–state of the art and future directions. Brain Behav Immun. 2012;26(7):1019–29.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Corp S. Stata statistical software: release 17. College Station: StataCorp LLC.; 2021.

    Google Scholar 

  58. Collins RL, Kashdan TB, Koutsky JR, Morsheimer ET, Vetter CJ. A self-administered Timeline Followback to measure variations in underage drinkers’ alcohol intake and binge drinking. Addict Behav. 2008;33(1):196–200.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Wittchen HU, Zaudig M, Fydrich T. Strukturiertes Klinisches Interview f€ ur DSM-IV (SKID-I und SKID-II). Achse Psych Sto Rungen Achse II Perso Nlichkeitssto Rungen Go Ttingen Hogrefe. 1997.

  60. Gröschl M. Current status of salivary hormone analysis. Clin Chem. 2008;54(11):1759–69.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Whiteside SP, Lynam DR, Miller JD, Reynolds SK. Validation of the UPPS impulsive behaviour scale: a four-factor model of impulsivity. Eur J Pers. 2005;19(7):559–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Bernstein DP, Fink L, Handelsman L, Foote J. Childhood trauma questionnaire. Assess Fam Violence Handb Res Pract. 1998.

  63. Rohleder N, Nater UM. Determinants of salivary alpha-amylase in humans and methodological considerations. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2009;34(4):469–85.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Wolff M, Krönke KM, Venz J, Kräplin A, Bühringer G, Smolka MN, et al. Action versus state orientation moderates the impact of executive functioning on real-life self-control. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2016;145(12):1635–53.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Pooseh S, Bernhardt N, Guevara A, Huys QJM, Smolka MN. Value-based decision-making battery: a Bayesian adaptive approach to assess impulsive and risky behavior. Behav Res Methods. 2018;50(1):236–49.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Kräplin A, Höfler M, Pooseh S, Wolff M, Krönke KM, Goschke T, et al. Impulsive decision-making predicts the course of substance-related and addictive disorders. Psychopharmacology. 2020;237(9):2709–24.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  67. Cohen S, Kamarck T, Mermelstein R. Perceived stress scale. Meas Stress Guide Health Soc Sci. 1994;10:1–2.

    Google Scholar 

  68. Bond FW, Hayes SC, Baer RA, Carpenter KM, Guenole N, Orcutt HK, et al. Preliminary psychometric properties of the Acceptance and Action Questionnaire-II: a revised measure of psychological inflexibility and experiential avoidance. Behav Ther. 2011;42(4):676–88.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Kuntsche E, Kuntsche S. Development and validation of the drinking motive questionnaire revised short form (DMQ–R SF). J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2009;38(6):899–908.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Kräplin A, Dshemuchadse M, Behrendt S, Scherbaum S, Goschke T, Bühringer G. Dysfunctional decision-making in pathological gambling: pattern specificity and the role of impulsivity. Psychiatry Res. 2014;215(3):675–82. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2013.12.041.

  71. Raabe A, Grüsser SM, Wessa M, Podschus J, Flor H. The assessment of craving: psychometric properties, factor structure and a revised version of the Alcohol Craving Questionnaire (ACQ). Addict Abingdon Engl. 2005;100(2):227–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  72. Gratz KL, Roemer L. Multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation and dysregulation: development, factor structure, and initial validation of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. 2003;14:41–54.

    Google Scholar 

  73. Spielberger CD. Test anxiety inventory. Corsini Encycl Psychol. 2010;1–1.

  74. Steyer R, Schwenkmezger P, Notz P, Eid M. Testtheoretische Analysen des Mehrdimensionalen Befindlichkeitsfragebogen (MDBF). [Theoretical analysis of a multidimensional mood questionnaire (MDBF).]. Diagnostica. 1994;40(4):320–8.

  75. Derryberry D, Reed MA. Anxiety-related attentional biases and their regulation by attentional control. J Abnorm Psychol. 2002;111(2):225.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. Gratz KL, Roemer L. Multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation and dysregulation: Development, factor structure, and initial validation of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. 2004;26(1):41–54.

  77. Ehring T, Zetsche U, Weidacker K, Wahl K, Schönfeld S, Ehlers A. The Perseverative Thinking Questionnaire (PTQ): validation of a content-independent measure of repetitive negative thinking. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 2011;42(2):225–32.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  78. Smyth C. The Pittsburgh sleep quality index (PSQI). Bd. 25, Journal of gerontological nursing. SLACK Incorporated Thorofare, NJ; 1999. S. 10–10.

  79. Gerlach AL, Andor T, Patzelt J. Die bedeutung von unsicherheitsintoleranz für die generalisierte angststörung modellüberlegungen und entwicklung einer deutschen version der unsicherheitsintoleranz-skala. Z Für Klin Psychol Psychother. 2008;37(3):190–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  80. Carver CS. You want to measure coping but your protocol’too long: consider the brief cope. Int J Behav Med. 1997;4(1):92–100.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  81. Beck AT, Steer RA, Brown GK. Beck depression inventory. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich; 1987.

    Google Scholar 

  82. Tangney JP, Baumeister RF, Boone AL. High self-control predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success. J Pers. 2004;72(2):271–324.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  83. Connor KM, Davidson JR. Development of a new resilience scale: the Connor-Davidson resilience scale (CD-RISC). Depress Anxiety. 2003;18(2):76–82.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  84. Jones A, Button E, Rose AK, Robinson E, Christiansen P, Di Lemma L, et al. The ad-libitum alcohol ‘taste test’: secondary analyses of potential confounds and construct validity. Psychopharmacology. 2016;233(5):917–24.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  85. Steudte-Schmiedgen S, Stalder T, Schönfeld S, Wittchen HU, Trautmann S, Alexander N, et al. Hair cortisol concentrations and cortisol stress reactivity predict PTSD symptom increase after trauma exposure during military deployment. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2015;59:123–33.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  86. Trautmann S, Muehlhan M, Kirschbaum C, Wittchen HU, Höfler M, Stalder T, et al. Biological stress indicators as risk markers for increased alcohol use following traumatic experiences. Addict Biol. 2018;23(1):281–90.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  87. Hellhammer DH, Wüst S, Kudielka BM. Salivary cortisol as a biomarker in stress research. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2009;34(2):163–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  88. Kirschbaum C, Wüst S, Hellhammer D. Consistent sex differences in cortisol responses to psychological stress. Psychosom Med. 1992;54(6):648–57.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  89. Kudielka BM, Gierens A, Hellhammer DH, Wüst S, Schlotz W. Salivary cortisol in ambulatory assessment—some dos, some don’ts, and some open questions. Psychosom Med. 2012;74(4):418–31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  90. Field M, Quigley M. Mild stress increases attentional bias in social drinkers who drink to cope: a replication and extension. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2009;17(5):312–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  91. Field M, Cox WM. Attentional bias in addictive behaviors: a review of its development, causes, and consequences. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2008;97(1–2):1–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  92. Ataya AF, Adams S, Mullings E, Cooper RM, Attwood AS, Munafò MR. Internal reliability of measures of substance-related cognitive bias. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2012;121(1–2):148–51.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  93. Field M, Christiansen P. Commentary on; “Internal reliability of measures of substance-related cognitive bias.” Drug Alcohol Depend. 2012;124(3):189–90.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  94. Zvielli A, Bernstein A, Koster EH. Temporal dynamics of attentional bias. Clin Psychol Sci. 2015;3(5):772–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  95. Bogacz R, Wagenmakers EJ, Forstmann BU, Nieuwenhuis S. The neural basis of the speed-accuracy tradeoff. Trends Neurosci. 2010;33(1):10–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  96. Liesefeld HR, Janczyk M. Combining speed and accuracy to control for speed-accuracy trade-offs(?). Behav Res Methods. 2019;51(1):40–60.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  97. Brainard DH, Vision S. The psychophysics toolbox. Spat Vis. 1997;10(4):433–6.

  98. Kirschbaum C, Pirke KM, Hellhammer DH. The ’Trier Social Stress Test’—a tool for investigating psychobiological stress responses in a laboratory setting. Neuropsychobiology. 1993;28(1–2):76–81.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  99. Labuschagne I, Grace C, Rendell P, Terrett G, Heinrichs M. An introductory guide to conducting the Trier Social Stress Test. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2019;107:686–95.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  100. Dickerson SS, Kemeny ME. Acute stressors and cortisol responses: a theoretical integration and synthesis of laboratory research. Psychol Bull. 2004;130(3):355–91.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  101. Het S, Rohleder N, Schoofs D, Kirschbaum C, Wolf OT. Neuroendocrine and psychometric evaluation of a placebo version of the ‘Trier Social Stress Test.’ Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2009;34(7):1075–86.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  102. Colby SM, Rohsenow DJ, Monti PM, Gwaltney CJ, Gulliver SB, Abrams DB, et al. Effects of tobacco deprivation on alcohol cue reactivity and drinking among young adults. Addict Behav. 2004;29(5):879–92.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  103. Jones A, Rose AK, Cole J, Field M. Effects of alcohol cues on craving and ad libitum alcohol consumption in social drinkers: the role of disinhibition. J Exp Psychopathol. 2013;4(3):239–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  104. Van Dyke N, Fillmore MT. Operant responding for alcohol following alcohol cue exposure in social drinkers. Addict Behav. 2015;47:11–6.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  105. Christiansen P, Cole JC, Field M. Ego depletion increases ad-lib alcohol consumption: investigating cognitive mediators and moderators. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2012;20(2):118–28.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  106. Jones A, Guerrieri R, Fernie G, Cole J, Goudie A, Field M. The effects of priming restrained versus disinhibited behaviour on alcohol-seeking in social drinkers. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2011;113(1):55–61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  107. Quigley BM, Collins RL. The modeling of alcohol consumption: a meta-analytic review. J Stud Alcohol. 1999;60(1):90–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  108. Bowley C, Faricy C, Hegarty B, Johnstone S, Smith J, Kelly P, et al. The effects of inhibitory control training on alcohol consumption, implicit alcohol-related cognitions and brain electrical activity. Int J Psychophysiol Off J Int Organ Psychophysiol. 2013;89(3):342–8.

    Google Scholar 

  109. Field M, Eastwood B. Experimental manipulation of attentional bias increases the motivation to drink alcohol. Psychopharmacology. 2005;183(3):350–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  110. Jones A, Field M. The effects of cue-specific inhibition training on alcohol consumption in heavy social drinkers. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2013;21(1):8–16.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  111. Miller R, Stalder T, Jarczok M, Almeida DM, Badrick E, Bartels M, et al. The CIRCORT database: reference ranges and seasonal changes in diurnal salivary cortisol derived from a meta-dataset comprised of 15 field studies. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2016;73:16–23.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  112. Pruessner JC, Kirschbaum C, Meinlschmid G, Hellhammer DH. Two formulas for computation of the area under the curve represent measures of total hormone concentration versus time-dependent change. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2003;28(7):916–31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  113. Miller R, Wojtyniak JG, Weckesser LJ, Alexander NC, Engert V, Lehr T. How to disentangle psychobiological stress reactivity and recovery: a comparison of model-based and non-compartmental analyses of cortisol concentrations. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2018;90:194–210.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  114. Miller R, Plessow F, Kirschbaum C, Stalder T. Classification criteria for distinguishing cortisol responders from nonresponders to psychosocial stress: evaluation of salivary cortisol pulse detection in panel designs. Psychosom Med. 2013;75(9):832–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  115. Robins JM, Greenland S. Identifiability and exchangeability for direct and indirect effects. Epidemiol Camb Mass. 1992;3(2):143–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  116. Royall RM. Model robust confidence intervals using maximum likelihood estimators. Int Stat Rev Int Stat. 1986;221–6.

  117. Trova AC, Paparrigopoulos T, Liappas I, Ginieri-Coccossis M. Prevention of alcohol dependence. Psychiatr Psychiatr. 2015;26(2):131–40.

    Google Scholar 

  118. Bickel WK, Jarmolowicz DP, Mueller ET, Koffarnus MN, Gatchalian KM. Excessive discounting of delayed reinforcers as a trans-disease process contributing to addiction and other disease-related vulnerabilities: emerging evidence. Pharmacol Ther. 2012;134(3):287–97.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  119. Houben K, Nederkoorn C, Wiers RW, Jansen A. Resisting temptation: decreasing alcohol-related affect and drinking behavior by training response inhibition. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2011;116(1–3):132–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  120. López-Caneda E, Rodríguez Holguín S, Cadaveira F, Corral M, Doallo S. Impact of alcohol use on inhibitory control (and vice versa) during adolescence and young adulthood: a review. Alcohol Alcohol Oxf Oxfs. 2014;49(2):173–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  121. Schoenmakers T, Wiers RW, Jones BT, Bruce G, Jansen ATM. Attentional re-training decreases attentional bias in heavy drinkers without generalization. Addict Abingdon Engl. 2007;102(3):399–405.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  122. Szeto EH, Schoenmakers TM, van de Mheen D, Snelleman M, Waters AJ. Associations between dispositional mindfulness, craving, and drinking in alcohol-dependent patients: an ecological momentary assessment study. Psychol Addict Behav J Soc Psychol Addict Behav. 2019;33(5):431–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  123. Mayhugh RE, Rejeski WJ, Petrie MR, Laurienti PJ, Gauvin L. Differing patterns of stress and craving across the day in moderate-heavy alcohol consumers during their typical drinking routine and an imposed period of alcohol abstinence. PLoS ONE. 2018;13(4):e0195063.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  124. Jones A, Tiplady B, Houben K, Nederkoorn C, Field M. Do daily fluctuations in inhibitory control predict alcohol consumption? An ecological momentary assessment study. Psychopharmacology. 2018;235(5):1487–96.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  125. Duif M, Thewissen V, Wouters S, Lechner L, Jacobs N. Associations between affect and alcohol consumption in adults: an ecological momentary assessment study. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse. 2020;46(1):88–97.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We acknowledge Helen Lenhardt and Lisa Hofmann for their assistance in data collection.

Disclaimer

The funder has no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of any kind of manuscript.

Funding

Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL. The study is funded by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG), Grant No. TR 1489/1-1.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

All authors have read and provided substantial contributions to the final version of the study protocol. ST is the central and principal investigator of the project and is responsible for drafting the initial proposal with all subsequent revisions. CW is the principal investigator for the study and for drafting the final protocol for publication. MM contributed substantially to drafting the initial proposal and to the revision of the initial proposal as well as to the revision and final approval of the manuscript. AK contributed substantially to the revision of the initial proposal and to the revision and final approval of the manuscript. MW provided the paradigms for the proposal and contributed to the revision and final approval of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Charlotte Wittgens or Sebastian Trautmann.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The ethics approval for the study was given by ethics committee of Technische Universität Dresden (EK 522122016) and the ethics committee of Medical School Hamburg (MSH) (MSH-2020/114). The participants give their written informed consent.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Wittgens, C., Muehlhan, M., Kräplin, A. et al. Underlying mechanisms in the relationship between stress and alcohol consumption in regular and risky drinkers (MESA): methods and design of a randomized laboratory study. BMC Psychol 10, 233 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-022-00942-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-022-00942-1

Keywords