Skip to main content

Identified or conflicted: a latent class and regression tree analysis explaining how identity constructs cluster within smokers

Abstract

Identity, or ‘who I am’, is important for smoking behaviour. Identity constructs (parts of a person’s identity) are typically examined as separate entities, but emerging evidence suggests that the multifaceted nature of identity is relevant in the context of smoking. This cross-sectional study examined how smoking-related self- and group-identity constructs cluster within adult daily smokers (N = 231), whether classes of smokers can be distinguished based on clusters of identity constructs, and which factors explain class membership. Data were collected online in The Netherlands and Belgium, 2017–2018. Latent class and regression tree analyses showed that participants in Class 1 of ‘Identified smokers’ (estimated population share 54%) reported stronger smoker self- and group-identities, stronger expected identity loss when quitting smoking, and weaker quitter self-identities and non-smoker self- and group-identities (vs. Class 2 of ‘Conflicted smokers’). Class membership was explained by the interaction between mental smoking dependence (dominant explanatory variable), consideration of future consequences, age of smoking onset, self-efficacy, and future self thought clarity. Models had good fit. The identity of more dependent smokers is more strongly oriented toward smoking. Smoking is also more strongly embedded in the identity of smokers who started smoking young, are less inclined to think about the future, and have lower self-efficacy.

Peer Review reports

Introduction

Identity is increasingly recognized as a key factor in explaining development, maintenance and cessation of addictive behaviours, including smoking. Identity refers to perceptions of ‘who I am’, and people preferably behave in line with their identity [1,2,3,4,5,6]. The overarching concept of ‘identity’ consists of different parts, or identity constructs (e.g. ‘I am a dancer’, ‘I am someone who helps others’), that together define a person’s identity. Research into identity in the context of smoking typically focuses on self-identity and group-identity. Self-identity is defined as a part of identity that is based on a behaviour. For smokers, this entails a smoker self-identity (“Smoking fits with who I am”), non-smoker self-identity (“Non-smoking fits with who I am”), or quitting self-identity (“Quitting fits with who I am”). Group-identity refers to parts of identity that are based on memberships of social categories or groups. A strong smoker group-identity means that “being part of a group of smokers is important for who I am”, and a strong non-smoker group-identity means that the individual identifies with non-smokers. A given smoker may identify more or less strongly with each of these behaviours and groups. This study focuses on smoker, non-smoker and quitter self-identity, smoker and non-smoker group-identity, and expected identity loss when quitting smoking (see below).

Self-identity is at the basis of rules that guide an individual’s behaviour, for example a non-smoker self-identity can be accompanied by a ‘not even a puff’ rule that helps quitters in their process to refrain from smoking (PRIME theory [1]). Behaviours that are associated with identity also feel more important to individuals than behaviours that are identity irrelevant, and are therefore more likely to be performed (identity value model [3]). As such, although a sustainable health behaviour change such as quitting smoking successfully requires effortful self-control, less effort is needed when the new behaviour becomes part of identity. The individual then becomes more empowered and resilient in maintaining the new behaviour (maintain IT model [2]). In addition to such representations of behaviour in self-identity, the social identity approach elaborates on the part of the ‘self’ that is derived from group membership, and states that people are motivated to behave in line with group norms when group identification is strong and the identity is salient in a given situation [4,5,6]. Two related theories on overcoming addiction (the SIMOR and SIMCM models) indeed underscore the importance of smokers’ social identification with groups that support cessation [7, 8]. Many empirical studies have shown that smoker, quitter and non-smoker self-identity and group-identity are uniquely associated with intentions to quit, smoking and quitting behaviour, and reactions to antismoking measures and stigmatisation, even after other relevant variables (e.g. physical nicotine dependence) are controlled for [9,10,11,12,13,14,15,16,17,18,19,20,21,22,23,24]. In addition, it appears that the future identity as a non-smoker or quitter is more relevant for explaining smokers’ behaviour than the current identity as a smoker, suggesting that smokers need to be able to see themselves as non-smokers in order to quit smoking successfully [9,10,11, 23,24,25,26,27]. Conversely, smokers who expect or experience identity loss (i.e. smokers who feel that they lose a part of their self-identity or group-identity) after quitting are more likely to relapse [23, 28, 29].

Studies into identity and smoking or quitting typically approach identity constructs as separate entities that independently contribute to outcomes such as smoking cessation [30]. In favour of this approach, correlations between identity constructs reported in the literature show that shared variance is relatively small, such that, for example, smoker and non-smoker self-identity are not merely semantic opposites but are essentially different constructs [9, 10, 12]. Importantly, a decade ago experts advocated for investigating self-concept as ‘a dynamic and multifaceted cognitive structure’ [30], but clusters of identity constructs have not been examined to date. Nevertheless, some emerging evidence suggests that the multifaceted nature of identity is relevant in the context of smoking. For instance, although most smokers in a qualitative study did not hold strong smoker self-identities, some of them did have strong non-smoker self-identities whereas others did not [23]. Another qualitative study showed that smokers may develop incompatible group identities as both smoker and non-smoker [31]. It is likely that someone who identifies both with smoking and non-smoking (i.e. self-identity), or with both the groups of smokers and non-smokers (i.e. group-identity) behaves differently than someone who primarily identifies with only one of these behaviours or groups [23]. The term identity preference was coined to convey the relative strength of one identity construct over another, implying that not the mere strength of one identity, for example non-smoker self-identity affects behaviour, but the relative strength compared to another identity construct, such as smoker self-identity [8, 32, 33]. Importantly, physical nicotine dependence is a known and consistent predictor of the success of quit attempts [34], but recent work showed that this relationship is mediated by the relative strength of ex-smoker identity over smoker identity [35]. However, despite the growing body of literature supporting the importance of smoking-related identity in general, and emerging indications that combinations/clusters of identity constructs within one individual may be important in particular, these clusters remain unstudied.

In addition, research shows that smoking-related identity constructs are associated with other smoker characteristics, but studies typically include only a few characteristics of interest and thus a more complete picture is, as yet, lacking. With regard to demographic characteristics, two quantitative studies found that lower socio-economic position (SEP) smokers typically report stronger smoker identities than their higher SEP counterparts, and weaker non-smoker and quitter identities [9, 36], although this was not found in another study [10]. Furthermore, smoker self-identity increases, and quitter self-identity decreases over time more strongly among lower SEP smokers compared to higher SEP groups [36]. Age appears positively related to smoker self-identity and negatively related to non-smoker self-identity [11, 12], but mixed findings have been reported for gender [9,10,11, 20]. Smoking history and behaviour also shape identity. Smokers who started smoking at a later age, have smoked for a longer time, and those who are more dependent on nicotine or smoke more cigarettes a day have stronger smoker identities and weaker non-smoker identities [9,10,11, 36, 37]. Furthermore, attempting to quit and quitting smoking successfully are associated with subsequent changes in the expected direction in non-smoker, quitter, and smoker self-identity, and in smoker group-identity [12, 18, 24, 38]. The strength and development of smoker and quitter self-identity over time is also related to psychological factors such as attitudes toward smoking and quitting, quitting self-efficacy, pro-quitting social norms, and inclination to smoke in order to cope with negative emotions [12, 31, 36, 39, 40]. A qualitative study furthermore showed that smokers who perceive quitting as difficult and frightening, and who fear withdrawal symptoms, have difficulty identifying with a positive future non-smoking self [23]. In sum, previous work suggests a range of factors that relate to identity in smokers: SEP, age, gender, smoking duration, physical nicotine dependence, cigarettes per day, age of onset, quitting behaviour, attitudes and social norms, smoking as coping with negative emotions, quitting self-efficacy, and control over withdrawal symptoms. However, no studies to date have examined clusters of identity constructs within smokers and how these can be explained.

The objective of this exploratory study was two-fold. First, we examined among adult daily smokers how a range of smoking-related identity constructs cluster within individuals, and whether classes of smokers can be distinguished based on clusters of these identity constructs. We included non-smoker, quitter, and smoker self-identity, smoker and non-smoker group-identity, and expected identity loss when quitting smoking [10, 41]. Second, we examined which demographic, smoking and psychological characteristics (possibly in interaction) explain class membership. In addition to the factors mentioned above, we included clarity and frequency of thinking about the future self and consideration of future consequence, as these likely are associated with future identities as quitter and non-smoker [42,43,44,45,46]. Latent class analysis and regression tree analysis were used as statistical techniques.

Method

Design

Observational online cross-sectional study. This study reports on the pre-test measures of a longitudinal experimental study, which examined the effect of an identity-based intervention on non-smoker, quitter, and smoker self-identity and expected identity loss. These results will be reported elsewhere [47]. Data from the two follow-up measurements was not included in the current study, as these measurements were affected by the intervention. STROBE reporting guidelines were used [48].

Participants

Participants were recruited through various means in order to reach a sufficiently large and diverse sample. They were invited after previous research participation (26%), or participated for university course credits (18%; students from two universities participated), or were recruited through social media (12%), snowball sampling (8%), a radio program (7%), approaching smokers in public places (7%), newspaper (2%), health website (3%), Google (2%), or a flyer at a cigarette point-of-sale (1%) (missing for 13%). Inclusion criteria for the larger experimental study were that participants had to be 18 years or older, smoke daily, and intend to quit some time.Footnote 1 Participants were 231 adult daily smokers (age M = 37.75, SD = 18.53; cigarettes per day M = 12.07, SD = 8.06; 71% female; 12%, 54%, and 35% low, middle, and higher SEP, respectively). Participants were Dutch (93%) or Dutch-speaking Belgian (7%).

Procedure

Data were collected in The Netherlands and Belgium between July 2017 and July 2018, using the Qualtrics survey program (www.qualtrics.com). Before completing the survey, participants were informed about the study aim (i.e., investigating how smokers think about smoking, quitting, themselves and the future), that participation was voluntary, and that data would be analysed and stored anonymously and treated confidentially. Two gift coupons of € 100,- and six gift coupons of € 50,- were divided among participants who also completed two follow-up questionnaires.

Measures

Variables used in the current study are described below. There were no missing values. Bivariate correlations between identity variables and explanatory variables that were included in the final regression tree model are presented in Table 1.

Table 1 Descriptive statistics and Pearson correlations between variables included in the final models (N = 231)

Identity

Smoker, quitter and non-smoker self-identity were measured with eight (α = 0.81), seven (α = 0.72), and seven (α = 0.80) items respectively, in order to allow for thorough measurement of these constructs (cf. [10]). Items were adapted from the Smoker Self-Concept Scale and the Abstainer Self-Concept Scale [49] and work by Tombor and colleagues [20] and Van den Putte and colleagues [12], e.g. ‘Smoking is part of “who I am”’, and ‘I can see myself as a non-smoker’. The items ‘I feel at ease with the idea that I would be a quitter/non-smoker’ in the quitter and non-smoker self-identity scales were replaced by two items adapted from the smoker self-identity scale (i.e. ‘To continue smoking fits with who I am’ and ‘To continue smoking fits with how I want to live’, [12, 24, 36]. We measured smoker (α = 0.79) and non-smoker group-identity (α = 0.68) with four items each, for example ‘In general, I am glad that I am part of the group of smokers’ (adapted from Cameron’s three factor model of group identity [50], ‘affect’ subscale). Previous work has shown that these five scales are reliable [10]. Finally, we measured expected identity loss when quitting smoking with four items, e.g. ‘If I quit smoking, I will have to give up a part of myself’ (α = 0.83, adapted from [41]). Answers ranged from [1] ‘totally disagree’ to [5] ‘totally agree’ for all items. Scales were made by calculating for each participant the mean scores across the scale items, which were then rounded to integer values for use in the latent class analysis.

Explanatory variables

Demographic characteristics

Participants reported their age and gender, and educational level as an indicator of SEP with answer categories ranging from [1] ‘no education’ to [8] ‘university’, and [9] ‘other, namely’ for which text responses were recoded into one of the eight categories (cf. [9, 10]).

Smoking history

Age at smoking onset, number of years they had been smoking, number of previous quit attempts, and date and duration of their most recent quit attempt (cf. [9, 10]).

Nicotine dependence

We used the Fagerström Test for Nicotine Dependence (FTND) to measure physical nicotine dependence [51]. Participants provided the number of cigarettes per day, which was recoded to calculate the FTND score and also used as a separate variable in the analyses. We also measured mental dependence on smoking with two items asking how much participants would miss smoking if they were to stop smoking for good ([1] ‘I wouldn’t miss it’—[4] ‘I would miss it very much’) and how important smoking is to them ([1] ‘not important’—[4] ‘very important’]) [52]. Given the correlation below 0.60, the mental dependence items were used separately in the analyses (r = 0.56).

Intention and motivation to quit

Participants were asked when (if at all) they intended to quit smoking, with answer categories [1] ‘within 1 month’, [2] ‘within 6 months’ [3] ‘within 2 years’, [4] ‘within 5 years’, [5] ‘within 10 years’, [6] ‘quit sometime ever, but not within 10 years’, [7] ‘always remain smoking, but less’; or [8] always to remain smoking, and not less’ [9, 10]. Motivation to quit was measured with one item, i.e. ‘I am motivated to quit smoking within three months’, [1] ‘totally disagree’—[7] ‘totally agree’.

Self-efficacy and perceived behavioural control over withdrawal symptoms

Self-efficacy was assessed with four items asking how confident participants were about being able to decrease smoking, and to quit smoking for one day, one week and one month, [1] ‘very unconfident’—[5] ‘very confident’ (α = 0.78). Perceived behavioural control over withdrawal symptoms was measured with two items, i.e., ‘If I would quit smoking…’ ‘I feel I will have control over my feelings of withdrawal from cigarettes’ and ‘I believe that I will be capable of dealing adequately with withdrawal symptoms from smoking’, [1] ‘totally agree’—[5] ‘totally disagree’ [53], r = 0.68.

Attitude toward smoking and quitting

Measured with two separate items, i.e. ‘What is your overall opinion on smoking?’ and ‘If you would quit smoking within the next 3 months, this would be…’, with [1] ‘very positive’ to [5] ‘very negative’ [36].

Social norms (injunctive)

Measured with one item, i.e., ‘How do you think that most of the people important to you would feel about you quitting smoking within the next 3 months?’ ([1] ‘strongly disapprove’—[5] ‘strongly approve’) [36].

Consideration of future consequences (CFC)

Measured with the twelve-item Consideration of Future Consequences Scale [43], translated into pre-vocational/general secondary education level Dutch [54], e.g. ‘I consider how things might be in the future, and try to influence those things with my day to day behaviour’ (α = 0.79).

Future self thought

Clarity of future self was measured with three items (e.g. ‘When I picture myself in the future, I see clear and vivid images’, α = 0.68) and frequency of future self thought with one item (i.e., ‘It is common for me to spend time thinking about myself as I might be in future stages of life) with answers ranging from [1] ‘not at all true for me’ to [6] ‘completely true for me’ (adapted from [42]).

Anxiety

General anxiety over the past two weeks was measured with three items from the Generalized Anxiety Disorder scale [55], e.g. ‘Over the past two weeks I was not able to stop or control worrying’ with [1] ‘not at all’, [2] ‘several days’, [3] ‘over half of the days’ and [4] ‘nearly every day’ (α = 0.89). Perceived control over anxiety was measured with three items from the revised Anxiety-Control Questionnaire, e.g. ‘How well I cope with difficult situations depends on whether I have outside help’, with [1] ‘totally agree’—[5] ‘totally disagree’[56]. The anxiety control items were used separately in the analyses as scale reliability was low (α = 0.53).

Statistical analyses

We first performed latent class analyses on the identity variables to find the optimal classes solution, which were followed by regression tree analyses to explain class membership by the explanatory variables [57]. The analyses were performed in R statistical software version 3.2.5 [58].

Latent class analysis

Latent class analyses were using the poLCA package [59]. The analysis aims to reduce heterogeneity in a population to a number of latent classes, i.e. existing but unobserved subgroups of participants. This fits the purpose of identifying subgroups of smokers based on how a range of smoking-related identity constructs cluster within individuals. The model aims to maximize similarity within a class and difference between the classes [60]. A series of models were fit ranging from 1 to 5 classes. We used a maximum of 1000 iterations, and repeated each analysis 100 times to decrease chances of obtaining local maxima. The models were evaluated using maximum log-likelihood (LL), Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC), Akaike Information Criterion (AIC), and relative entropy values. Lower LL, BIC and AIC values indicate better fit. The BIC takes loss of parsimony into account and has been proposed as the most accurate fit measure for basic latent class models [59]. Furthermore, relative entropy values > 0.80 indicate sufficient certainty in classification. After selection of the best fitting model, conditional probabilities were examined to interpret the classes.

Regression tree analysis

Regression tree analyses were performed using the Rpart and Partykit packages [61, 62]. This procedure examines in a data-driven manner whether variables interact in explaining the outcome (i.e. class membership), and searches for optimal cut-off values in explanatory variables. Regression tree analysis examines potential interactions between explanatory variables (in contrast to more traditional techniques such as logistic regression analysis that require pre-specification of interactions), and as such may lead to novel findings. At the same time, k-fold cross-validation inherent to the regression tree analysis procedure is performed to ‘prune’ the tree. The minimum number of participants per leaf was fixed at 10, and the minimum increase in fit (complexity parameter) was set at 0.0001. For the remaining parameters we used default options. The selection process of the initial, non-pruned tree was performed 1000 times. A correct classification rate (CCR) based on the final model was calculated, which was compared to the a priori CCR (i.e., all participants assigned to the largest class). We repeated the analysis without the variable that emerged as dominant in the final model in order to better understand the data.

Results

Latent class analysis

The model with two latent classes showed the best fit to the data according to the BIC value, which is the preferred fit measure as it takes parsimony into account (see Table 2, [59]). The relative entropy value indicated high certainty in classification (54% and 46% of the sample in Class 1 and 2, respectively). Participants in Class 1 reported stronger smoker self- and group-identities, stronger expected identity loss when quitting smoking, and weaker quitter self-identities and non-smoker self- and group-identities than participants in Class 2 (see Fig. 1 for conditional item response probabilities). The two classes differed significantly on all identity variables (see Table 3). From here on, Class 1 will be referred to as ‘Identified smokers’ and Class 2 as ‘Conflicted smokers’.

Table 2 Model characteristics (N = 231)
Fig. 1
figure 1

Conditional item response probabilities for the six identity variables in both classes

Table 3 Scores on identity variables in the two classes: Descriptive statistics and t-tests (N = 231)

The three-class solution had a less favourable BIC value, and is presented in Additional file 1: Table S1. In short, in the three-class solution, Class 1 represented smokers whose smoker self-identities were oriented towards smoking, whereas Class 2 and Class 3 smokers seemed oriented towards quitting, with this pattern being more pronounced in Class 2 than 3. Group-identity seemed more important in Class 1 and 3 than in Class 2, which had most pronounced scores on self-identity. The corresponding regression tree analysis did not show predictors of class membership.

Regression tree analysis

Mental dependence on smoking, consideration of future consequences (CFC), age of smoking onset, self-efficacy, and clarity of future self thought, in interaction, explained class membership, see Fig. 2 (CCR = 0.78, a priori CCR = 0.54). The other ‘explanatory variables’ (see Method, Measures) did not emerge as predictors in the regression tree analysis. Mental dependence emerged as the dominant variable. Among participants with stronger mental dependence (see Fig. 2, left side), those with weaker CFC likely belonged to Identified smokers (probability = 0.83). Among those with stronger CFC, participants who had started smoking before the age of 14.5 years likely belonged to Identified smokers (probability = 0.88). For those who were 14.5 years or older at smoking onset, self-efficacy explained class membership, such that participants with lower self-efficacy again most likely belonged to Identified smokers (probability = 0.65), but those with higher self-efficacy most likely belonged to Conflicted smokers (probability = 0.69). Participants with weaker mental dependence (see Fig. 2, right side) who had started smoking before age 17.5 were likely to belong to Identified smokers if clarity of future self thought was low (probability = 0.74), whereas those with higher clarity of future self thought were likely to belong to Conflicted smokers (probability = 0.72). Those with weak mental dependence and age of onset at age 17.5 or later were very likely to belong to Conflicted smokers (probability = 0.93).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Final regression tree model. Mental dependence refers to the item ‘How much would you miss smoking if you were to stop smoking for good’

The follow-up analysis without mental dependence showed a tree with one split on physical nicotine dependence, such that smokers with weak physical nicotine dependence were likely to belong to Conflicted smokers (FTND 0 or 1, probability = 0.66), whereas more dependent smokers were more likely to belong to Identified smokers (FTND > 1, probability = 0.64; CCR = 0.65).

Discussion

This study provided new insight into how a comprehensive set of smoking-related identity constructs cluster within daily smokers, and how the resulting identity-based classes relate to demographic, smoking and psychological characteristics. The study confirmed emerging evidence from previous work that different identity constellations exist within smokers. Two classes emerged based on identity constructs. In short, the identity of Class 1 ‘Identified’ smokers was oriented more toward smoking, and the identity of Class 2 ‘Conflicted’ smokers was oriented more toward non-smoking, with the class of Identified smokers being only slightly larger (54%). This means that a substantial group of smokers is conflicted about their smoking, which may lead them to strongly wish to quit and become a non-smoker. Class membership was explained by (the interaction between) mental dependence on smoking, CFC, age at smoking onset, self-efficacy, and clarity of the future self. The latent class model had good fit to the data, and 78% of participants were classified correctly based on the final regression tree model (vs. 54% a priori). When the analysis was repeated without mental dependence, physical nicotine dependence explained class membership.

The extent to which smokers are dependent on smoking, both mentally and physically, seems key in explaining identity-based class membership, with smoking being more strongly embedded in identity among more dependent smokers (Identified smokers). Interestingly, mental and physical dependence shared only 16% of variance in the current study, in line with previous findings [9, 10]. Mental dependence on smoking was more important than physical nicotine dependence, which makes sense as this taps into the psychological importance of smoking [35]. Age of smoking onset also contributed to explaining class membership, both among smokers with strong mental dependence and those who were less dependent. As adolescence is typically considered as a period in which identities strongly develop [63], it is likely that teenagers who start smoking when they are younger and when their identity still needs to develop, are more susceptible to developing smoking-oriented identities (Identified smokers). Two variables concerning the future also distinguished between the two classes: consideration of future consequences (CFC; among more mentally dependent smokers) and clarity of thinking about the future self (among less mentally dependent smokers who started smoking in their teenage years). Smokers who were more oriented toward the consequences of their behaviour in the present and those who found it difficult to picture themselves in the future, respectively, were more likely to belong to the class of Identified smokers. This makes sense, as the class of Identified smokers represents an identity constellation that is more oriented in the present, with stronger (current) smoker identities, weaker (future) identities as quitter and non-smoker, and stronger expected loss of identity when quitting smoking. This finding corresponds with previous work showing that people with stronger CFC can generate more vivid images about themselves in the future and are more motivated by these future identities than lower CFC counterparts [44, 45]. Finally, self-efficacy distinguished between the identity-based classes among the specific subgroup of smokers with stronger mental dependence, relatively strong CFC, and age of smoking onset after 14.5 years, such that the identity of less self-efficacious smokers in this subgroup was more smoking-oriented (Identified smokers) whereas those with stronger self-efficacy had more non-smoking-oriented identity constellations (Conflicted smokers).

Despite well-established SEP differences in a range of smoking characteristics [64], SEP did not distinguish between the two classes in the regression tree models. Post hoc analyses in this sample showed significant SEP differences in mental dependence (the dominant variable in the regression tree), self-efficacy, and physical nicotine dependence (see Additional file 2: Table S2). It is likely that SEP is indirectly related to identity, through these variables, and therefore did not emerge as an independent explanatory variable. No significant SEP differences were found in the other explanatory variables included in the regression tree, although previous work has shown significant associations between SEP and a number of these variables [9, 64,65,66,67,68]. It is also possible that SEP did not explain class membership as the sample was skewed toward middle and higher SEP smokers. Several important behavioural variables did not explain class membership either, such as the number of cigarettes per day, and the number of years smoking. As for SEP, this likely results from related variables explaining class membership, and indirect associations may exist here as well. More research is warranted to fully understand how, and through which mechanisms, identity and behaviour are associated in the context of smoking.

This study has limitations. First, although a broad recruitment strategy was used, the sample was somewhat small and in some respects selective. A larger sample size would allow for thorough analysis of more complicated models with more classes, and for explaining membership of small classes [69]. The sample was not fully representative of the population of smokers as middle and higher SEP (as mentioned above), and female smokers were overrepresented. In the Netherlands in 2018, smoking was most common among those with lower SEP and among men. Specifically, about 23% of those with lower SEP were daily smokers, compared to 19% and 8% of those with middle and higher SEP, respectively. Eighteen percent of men smoked daily compared to 14% of women [70]. Relatedly, the study took place among Western European smokers. Future research may examine whether different classes emerge in other populations. Second, in order to keep survey length to a minimum, some potentially relevant variables were not included to explain class membership (e.g. current self-concept clarity [71]). Relatedly, although class membership was predicted correctly for the large majority of participants, 22% were still classified incorrectly by the regression tree model. The addition of other relevant variables might improve classification. The current study nevertheless extends previous work by being the first to examine the current selection of identity and other variables in combination. Third, the cross-sectional design prevented claims about directionality of relationships, or predictive validity. The current cross-sectional survey served as the baseline assessment of a larger longitudinal experimental study, such that participants were randomized to an future self intervention or control condition directly after completing the survey. Future observational longitudinal research may examine the direction of relationships between identity-based classes and factors that explain class membership (e.g. dependence). In addition, predictive validity of identity-based classes compared to separate identity constructs regarding smoking and quitting behaviour is as yet unknown. Fourth, certain identity constructs, or parts of identity, may be more active or salient in a given situation than others [72]. The online nature of this study prevented us from controlling the setting in which surveys were completed (e.g. at work, in a bar), but these may have affected salience of identity constructs. This in turn may have influenced strength of identity constructs as reported by participants as well as the resulting classes solution.

The current findings call for studies in different populations, and potentially different settings, to examine whether the same identity-based classes emerge. In addition, longitudinal studies are needed to assess development of identity constellations as well as class transitions within smokers over time, directionality in the relationship between identity-based classes and explanatory variables, and predictive validity of identity-based clusters regarding smoking and quitting behaviour. If explanatory variables indeed affect clusters of identity, strategies targeting for example mental dependence on smoking or consideration of future consequences may help to prevent smokers from developing identities that further complicate quitting smoking. In addition, the finding that people who started smoking at a younger age are more likely to be identified smokers provides support for increasing the legal age for selling tobacco.

Current findings also have practical implications. A substantial group of smokers is conflicted about their smoking, identifies more strongly with non-smoking and quitting than with smoking, and does not really expect to lose identity when quitting. Whereas healthcare professionals still hesitate to address smoking [73,74,75], this group of smokers is likely to welcome a discussion of quitting smoking and perhaps also professional smoking cessation support. Although Conflicted smokers may be ‘low hanging fruit’, smoking should also not be left undiscussed with Identified smokers. However, healthcare professionals should be careful not to threaten identity and trigger defensive or victimizing responses in this group, as was found to be a consequence of antismoking measures in smokers with weaker non-smoker self- and group-identities [9]. Optimal ways to address both groups should be studied, but in general open questions about smoking and quitting are likely to work well in starting the conversation in both groups [76]. In addition, interventions that increase non-smoker and quitter self-identity and decrease smoker self-identity, as well as help smokers to regain a complete sense of identity when experiencing identity loss during and after quitting, are potentially successful.

Availability of Data and Material

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Notes

  1. Inclusion criteria were mentioned in the study information. Participants were included in the current analyses regardless of their self-reported ‘Intention to quit’.

References

  1. West R. Theory of addiction. Oxford: Blackwell; 2006.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Caldwell AE, et al. Harnessing centred identity transformation to reduce executive function burden for maintenance of health behaviour change: the Maintain IT model. Health Psychol Rev. 2018;12(3):231–53.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Berkman ET, Livingston JL, Kahn LE. Finding the “self” in self-regulation: the identity-value model. Psychol Inq. 2017;28(2–3):77–98.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Tajfel H, Turner JC. An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. In: Austin WG, Worchel S, editors. The social psychology of intergroup relations. Monterey: Brooks/Cole Publishing Company; 1979. p. 33–47.

    Google Scholar 

  5. Tajfel H, Turner JC. The social identity theory of intergroup behavior. In: Worchel S, Austin WG, editors. The psychology of intergroup relations. Chicago: Nelson-Hall; 1986. p. 7–24.

    Google Scholar 

  6. Turner JC, et al. Rediscovering the social group: a self-categorization theory. Oxford: Basil Blackwell; 1987.

    Google Scholar 

  7. Best D, et al. Overcoming alcohol and other drug addiction as a process of social identity transition: the social identity model of recovery (SIMOR). Addict Res Theory. 2015;24(2):111–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Frings D, Albery IP. The social identity model of cessation maintenance: formulation and initial evidence. Addict Behav. 2015;44:35–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Meijer E, et al. Quitting smoking: the importance of non-smoker identity in predicting smoking behaviour and responses to a smoking ban. Psychol Health. 2015;30(12):1387–409.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Meijer E, et al. Socio-economic status in relation to smoking: the role of (expected and desired) social support and quitter identity. Soc Sci Med. 2016;162:41–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Meijer E, et al. Smokers’ identity and quit advice in general practice: general practitioners need to focus more on female smokers. Patient Educ Couns. 2017;101(4):730–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Van den Putte B, et al. The effects of smoking self-identity and quitting self-identity on attempts to quit smoking. Health Psychol. 2009. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015199.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Helweg-Larsen M, Sorgen LJ, Pisinger C. Does it help smokers if we stigmatize them? A test of the stigma-induced identity threat model among US and Danish smokers. Soc Cognit. 2019;37(3):294–313.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Falomir-Pichastor JM, et al. Antismoking norm and smokers’ antismoking attitudes: the interplay between personal and group-based self-esteem. Eur J Soc Psychol. 2013;43(3):192–200.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Freeman MA, Hennessy EV, Marzullo DM. Defensive evaluation of antismoking messages among college-age smokers: the role of possible selves. Health Psychol. 2001;20(6):424–33.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Høie M, Moan IS, Rise J. An extended version of the theory of planned behavour: prediction of intentions to quit smoking using past behaviour as moderator. Addict Res Theory. 2010;18(5):572–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Moan IS, Rise J. Quitting smoking: applying an extended version of the theory of planned behavior to predict intention and behavior. J Appl Biobehav Res. 2005;10:39–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Shadel WG, Mermelstein R, Borrelli B. Self-concept changes over time in cognitive-behavioral treatment for smoking cessation. Addict Behav. 1996;21:659–63.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Tombor I, et al. Does non-smoker identity following quitting predict long-term abstinence? Evidence from a population survey in England. Addict Behav. 2015;45:99–103.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Tombor I, et al. Positive smoker identity as a barrier to quitting smoking: findings from a national survey of smokers in England. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2013;133(2):740–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Vangeli E, Stapleton J, West R. Residual attraction to smoking and smoker identity following smoking cessation. Nicot Tob Res. 2010;12(8):865–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Vangeli E, West R. Transition towards a ‘non-smoker’ identity following smoking cessation: an interpretative phenomenological analysis. Br J Health Psychol. 2012;17(1):171–84.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Meijer E, et al. Identity processes in smokers who want to quit smoking: a longitudinal interpretative phenomenological analysis. Health (London). 2018. https://doi.org/10.1177/1363459318817923.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. Meijer E, et al. A longitudinal study into the reciprocal effects of identities and smoking behaviour: findings from the ITC Netherlands survey. Soc Sci Med. 2018;200:249–57.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Markus H, Nurius P. Possible selves. Am Psychol. 1986;41:954–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Barreto ML, Frazier LD. Coping with life events through possible selves. J Appl Soc Psychol. 2012;42(7):1785–810.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Oyserman D, James L. Possible identities. In: Schwartz SJ, Luyckx K, Vignoles VL, editors. Handbook of identity theory and research. New York: Springer; 2011. p. 117–45.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  28. Notley C, Colllins R. Redefining smoking relapse as recovered social identity–secondary qualitative analysis of relapse narratives. J Subst Use. 2018;23(6):660–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Brown TJ, et al. Re-configuring identity postpartum and sustained abstinence or relapse to tobacco smoking. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2019. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph16173139.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Shadel WG, Cervone D. The role of the self in smoking initiation and smoking cessation: a review and blueprint for research at the intersection of social-cognition and health. Self Identity. 2011;10(3):386–95.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Dono J, et al. “I’m not the anti-smoker now. I just don’t smoke anymore”: social obstacles to quitting smoking among emerging adults. Addict Res Theory. 2019. https://doi.org/10.1080/16066359.2019.1623205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Buckingham SA, Frings D, Albery IP. Group membership and social identity in addiction recovery. Psychol Addict Behav. 2013;27(4):1132–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Dingle GA, et al. Breaking good: breaking ties with social groups may be good for recovery from substance misuse. Br J Soc Psychol. 2015;54(2):236–54.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Vangeli E, et al. Predictors of attempts to stop smoking and their success in adult general population samples: a systematic review. Addiction. 2011;106(12):2110–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Falomir-Pichastor JM, et al. Tobacco dependence and smoking cessation: the mediating role of smoker and ex-smoker self-concepts. Addict Behav. 2020;102: 106200.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Meijer E, et al. Identity change among smokers and ex-smokers: findings from the ITC Netherlands survey. Psychol Addict Behav. 2017;31(4):465.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Rodriguez D, et al. The role of the subjective importance of smoking (SIMS) in cessation and abstinence. J Smok Cessat. 2018;14(1):1–11.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Hertel AW, Mermelstein RJ. Smoker identity development among adolescents who smoke. Psychol Addict Behav. 2016;30(4):475–83.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Hertel AW, Mermelstein RJ. Smoker identity and smoking escalation among adolescents. Health Psychol. 2012;31(4):467–75.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  40. Rise J, Sheeran P, Hukkelberg S. The role of self-identity in the theory of planned behavior: a meta-analysis. J Appl Soc Psychol. 2010;40(5):1085–105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Dupont P, et al. Smoker’s identity scale: measuring identity in tobacco dependence and its relationship with confidence in quitting. Am J Addict. 2015;24(7):607–12.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. McElwee ROB, Haugh JA. Thinking clearly versus frequently about the future self: exploring this distinction and its relation to possible selves. Self Identity. 2010;9(3):298–321.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Strathman A, et al. The consideration of future consequences: weighing immediate and distant outcomes of behavior. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1994;66:742–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Ouellette JA, et al. Using images to increase exercise behavior: prototypes versus possible selves. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2005;31(5):610–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Stephan E, Shidlovski D, Sedikides C. Self-prospection and energization: the joint influence of time distance and consideration of future consequences. Self Identity. 2017;17(1):22–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Murphy L, Dockray S. The consideration of future consequences and health behaviour: a meta-analysis. Health Psychol Rev. 2018;12(4):357–81.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Penfornis KM, et al. My future-self has (not) quit smoking: An experimental study into the effect of a future-self intervention on smoking-related self-identity constructs. Submitted.

  48. von Elm E, et al. The strengthening the reporting of observational studies in epidemiology (STROBE) statement: guidelines for reporting observational studies. Lancet. 2007;370(9596):1453–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Shadel WG, Mermelstein RJ. Individual differences in self-concept among smokers attempting to quit: validation and predictive utility of measures of the smoker self-concept and abstainer self-concept. Ann Behav Med. 1996;18(18):151–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Cameron JE. A three-factor model of social identity. Self Identity. 2004;3(3):239–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Heatherton TF, et al. The Fagerström test for nicotine dependence: a revision of the Fagerstrom Tolerance Questionnaire. Br J Addict. 1991;86:1119–27.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Dijkstra A, Tromp D. Is the FTND a measure of physical as well as psychological tobacco dependence? J Subst Abuse Treat. 2002;23:367–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Schnoll RA, et al. Increased self-efficacy to quit and perceived control over withdrawal symptoms predict smoking cessation following nicotine dependence treatment. Addict Behav. 2011;36(1–2):144–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Rappange DR, Brouwer WB, van Exel NJ. Back to the consideration of future consequences scale: Time to reconsider? J Soc Psychol. 2009;149(5):562–84.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Spitzer RL, Kroenke K, Williams LBW. A brief measure for assessing generalized anxiety disorder—the GAD-7. Arch Intern Med. 2006;166(10):1092–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Brown TA, et al. The structure of perceived emotional control: psychometric properties of a revised Anxiety Control Questionnaire. Behav Ther. 2004;35:75–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Amaral R, et al. Disentangling the heterogeneity of allergic respiratory diseases by latent class analysis reveals novel phenotypes. Allergy. 2019;74(4):698–708.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. R Core Team. R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2014.

    Google Scholar 

  59. Linzer DA, Lewis J. poLCA: an R package for polytomous variable latent class analysis. J Stat Softw. 2011;42:1–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Hickendorff M, et al. Informative tools for characterizing individual differences in learning: latent class, latent profile, and latent transition analysis. Learn Individ Differ. 2018;66:4–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  61. Therneau T, Atkinson B, Ripley B. Rpart: recursive partitioning and regression trees. R Package Version. 2015;4:1–9.

    Google Scholar 

  62. Hothorn T, Zeileis A. partykit: a modular toolkit for recursive partytioning in R. J Mach Learn Res. 2015;16(1):3905–9.

    Google Scholar 

  63. Erikson EH. Identity: youth and crisis. New York: Norton; 1968.

    Google Scholar 

  64. Hiscock R, et al. Socioeconomic status and smoking: a review. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2012;1248:107–23.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Harrell JS, et al. Smoking initiation in youth: the roles of gender, race, socioeconomics, and developmental status. J Adolesc Health. 1998;23(5):271–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Adams J, White M. Time perspective in socioeconomic inequalities in smoking and body mass index. Health Psychol. 2009;28(1):83–90.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Guthrie LC, Butler SC, Ward MM. Time perspective and socioeconomic status: A link to socioeconomic disparities in health? Soc Sci Med. 2009;68(12):2145–51.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  68. Na J, et al. Social-class differences in self-concept clarity and their implications for well-being. J Health Psychol. 2016. https://doi.org/10.1177/1359105316643597.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  69. Nylund-Gibson K, Choi AY. Ten frequently asked questions about latent class analysis. Transl Issues Psychol Sci. 2018;4(4):440–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  70. Nationaal Expertisecentrum Tabaksontmoediging. Kerncijfers roken 2018. Utrecht: Trimbos Instituut; 2020.

    Google Scholar 

  71. Campbell JD, et al. Self-concept clarity: measurement, personality correlates, and cultural boundaries. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1996;70(1):141–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  72. Wheeler SC, Demarree KG, Petty RE. Understanding the role of the self in prime-to-behavior effects: the active-self account. Pers Soc Psychol Rev. 2007;11(3):234–61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Meijer E, et al. “It’s on everyone’s plate”: a qualitative study into physicians’ perceptions of responsibility for smoking cessation. Subst Abuse Treat Prev Policy. 2018;13(1):48.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  74. Meijer E, Van der Kleij R, Chavannes NH. Facilitating smoking cessation in patients who smoke: a large-scale cross-sectional comparison of fourteen groups of healthcare providers. BMC Health Serv Res. 2019;19(1):750.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  75. Meijer E, et al. Determinants of providing smoking cessation care in five groups of healthcare professionals: a cross-sectional comparison. Patient Educ Couns. 2019. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pec.2019.01.015.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. Miller RW, Rollnick S. Motivational interviewing: preparing people to change. New York: Guilford Press; 2002.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank Naomi Hoogerdijk and Danai Thanopoulou for their help in data collection and initial analysis.

Funding

This study was funded by a poster award received from CAHAG (Dutch general practitioner advisory group for COPD and asthma) by Dr. E. Meijer. CAHAG did not have any involvement in the study.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

EM contributed to the conception and design of the study, acquisition of the data, statistical analyses and interpretation of the data, and drafting of the manuscript. WG contributed to the conception and design of the study, interpretation of the data, and drafting of the manuscript. CvL, NC, and BvdP contributed to the conception and design of the study, and drafting of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to E. Meijer.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

This study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and the study protocol was approved by Leiden University’s Ethical Board (CEP17-0505/192). Participants provided informed consent before completing the survey.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

All authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1:

Scores on identity variables in the three classes.

Additional file 2:

Scores on the explanatory variables in lower, middle and higher SES-groups.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Meijer, E., Gebhardt, W.A., van Laar, C. et al. Identified or conflicted: a latent class and regression tree analysis explaining how identity constructs cluster within smokers. BMC Psychol 10, 231 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-022-00937-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-022-00937-y

Keywords