Diamond A. Executive functions. Annu Rev Psychol. 2013;64(1):135–68. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Willoughby MT, Wirth RJ, Blair CB. Executive function in early childhood: longitudinal measurement invariance and developmental change. Psychol Assess. 2012;24(2):418–31. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025779.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Garon N, Bryson SE, Smith IM. Executive function in preschoolers: a review using an integrative framework. Psychol Bull. 2008;134(1):31–60. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.134.1.31.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Garon N, Smith IM, Bryson SE. A novel executive function battery for preschoolers: sensitivity to age differences. Child Neuropsychol. 2014;20(6):713–36. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2013.857650.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Willoughby MT, Wirth RJ, Blair CB. Contributions of modern measurement theory to measuring executive function in early childhood: an empirical demonstration. J Exp Child Psychol. 2011;108(3):414–35. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2010.04.007.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Willoughby M, Blair C. Test–retest reliability of a new executive function battery for use in early childhood. Child Neuropsychol. 2011;17(6):564–79. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2011.554390.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Shaw P, Kabani NJ, Lerch JP, Eckstrand K, Lenroot R, Gogtay N, et al. Neurodevelopmental trajectories of the human cerebral cortex. J Neurosci. 2008;28(14):3586–94. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5309-07.2008.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Diamond A. The early development of executive functions. In: Bialystok R, Craik FIM, editors. Lifespan cognition: mechanisms of change. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2006. p. 70–95.
Chapter
Google Scholar
Pechtel P, Pizzagalli DA. Effects of early life stress on cognitive and affective function: an integrated review of human literature. Psychopharmacology. 2011;214(1):55–70. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-010-2009-2.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Hughes C, Ensor RA. How do families help or hinder the emergence of early executive function? New Dir Child Adolesc Dev. 2009;123:35–50. https://doi.org/10.1002/cd.234.
Article
Google Scholar
Bronfenbrenner U, Evans GW. Developmental science in the 21st century: emerging questions, theoretical models, research designs and empirical findings. Soc Dev. 2000;9(1):115–25. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9507.00114.
Article
Google Scholar
Brooks-Gunn J, Johnson A, Leventhal T. Disorder, turbulence, and resources in children’s homes and neighborhoods. In: Evans GW, Wachs TD, editors. Chaos and its influence on children’s development: an ecological perspective. Washington: American Psychological Association; 2010. p. 155–70.
Chapter
Google Scholar
Wachs T, Evans G. Chaos in context. In: Evans G, Wachs T, editors. Chaos and its influence on children’s development: an ecological perspective. American Psychological Association: Washington; 2010. p. 3–13.
Chapter
Google Scholar
Matheny AP, Wachs TD, Ludwig JL, Phillips K. Bringing order out of chaos: psychometric characteristics of the confusion, hubbub, and order scale. J Appl Dev Psychol. 1995;16(3):429–44. https://doi.org/10.1016/0193-3973(95)90028-4.
Article
Google Scholar
Brieant A, Holmes CJ, Deater-Deckard K, King-Casas B, Kim-Spoon J. Household chaos as a context for intergenerational transmission of executive functioning. J Adolesc. 2017;58:40–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2017.05.001.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Brown ED, Ackerman BP, Moore CA. Family adversity and inhibitory control for economically disadvantaged children: preschool relations and associations with school readiness. J Fam Psychol. 2013;27(3):443–52. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032886.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Andrews K, Atkinson L, Harris M, Gonzalez A. Examining the effects of household chaos on child executive functions: a meta-analysis. Psychol Bull. 2020;147(1):16–32. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000311.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Berry D, Blair C, Willoughby M, Garrett-Peters P, Vernon-Feagans L, Mills-Koonce WR, et al. Household chaos and children’s cognitive and socio-emotional development in early childhood: does childcare play a buffering role? Early Child Res Q. 2016;34:115–27. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2015.09.003.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Martin A, Razza RA, Brooks-Gunn J. Specifying the links between household chaos and preschool children’s development. Early Child Dev Care. 2012;182(10):1247–63. https://doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2011.605522.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Sturge-Apple ML, Davies PT, Cicchetti D, Hentges RF, Coe JL. Family instability and children’s effortful control in the context of poverty: sometimes a bird in the hand is worth two in the bush. Dev Psychopathol. 2017;29(3):685–96. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579416000407.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Landry S, Smith K, Swank P, Guttentag C. A Responsive parenting intervention: the optimal timing across early childhood for impacting maternal behaviors and child outcomes. Dev Psychol. 2008;44:1335–53. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013030.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Bernier A, Beauchamp MH, Carlson SM, Lalonde G. A secure base from which to regulate: attachment security in toddlerhood as a predictor of executive functioning at school entry. Dev Psychol. 2015;51(9):1177–89. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000032.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Vernon-Feagans L, Willoughby M, Garrett-Peters P. Predictors of behavioral regulation in kindergarten: Household chaos, parenting, and early executive functions. Dev Psychol. 2016;52(3):430–41. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000087.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Grossmann K, Grossman K, Waters E. Attachment from infancy to adulthood: the major longitudinal studies. Guilford Press; 2005.
Google Scholar
Cheng N, Lu S, Archer M, Wang Z. Quality of maternal parenting of 9-month-old infants predicts executive function performance at 2 and 3 years of age. Front Psychol. 2018;8:1–12. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02293.
Article
Google Scholar
Hughes C, Devine RT. For better or for worse? Positive and negative parental influences on young children’s executive function. Child Dev. 2019;90(2):593–609. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12915.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Sulik MJ, Blair C, Mills-Koonce R, Berry D, Greenberg M, The Family Life Project Investigators. Early parenting and the development of externalizing behavior problems: Longitudinal mediation through children’s executive function. Child Dev. 2015;86(5):1588–603. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12386.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Valcan DS, Davis H, Pino-Pasternak D. Parental behaviours predicting early childhood executive functions: a meta-analysis. Educ Psychol Rev. 2018;30:607–49. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-017-9411-9.
Article
Google Scholar
Sosic-Vasic Z, Kröner J, Schneider S, Vasic N, Spitzer M. The association between parenting behavior and executive functioning in children and young adolescents. Front Psychol. 2017;8:1–8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00472.
Article
Google Scholar
Lee DB, Assari S, Miller AL, Justin HH, Marc EH, Lee DB. Positive parenting moderates the effect of socioeconomic status on executive functioning: a three-generation approach. J Child Fam Stud. 2019;28:1878–85.
Article
Google Scholar
Coldwell J, Pike A, Dunn J. Household chaos—links with parenting and child behaviour. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discip. 2006;47(11):1116–22. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01655.
Article
Google Scholar
Corapci F, Wachs TD. Does parental mood or efficacy mediate the influence of environmental chaos upon parenting behavior? Merrill Palmer Q. 2002;48(2):182–201.
Article
Google Scholar
Marsh S, Dobson R, Maddison R. The relationship between household chaos and child, parent, and family outcomes: a systematic scoping review. BMC Public Health. 2020;20:1–27. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-020-08587-8.
Article
Google Scholar
Wachs T. Environmental chaos and children’s development: expanding the boundaries of chaos. Atlanta, GA: Society for Research in Child Development; 2005.
Google Scholar
Deater-Deckard K, Mullineaux PY, Beekman C, Petrill SA, Schatschneider C, Thompson LA. Conduct problems, IQ, and household chaos: a longitudinal multi-informant study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discip. 2009;50(10):1301–8. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02108.
Article
Google Scholar
Holochwost SJ, Gariépy JL, Propper CB, Neblett NG, Volpe V, Neblett E, et al. Sociodemographic risk, parenting, and executive functions in early childhood: the role of ethnicity. Early Child Res Q. 2016;36:537–49. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2016.02.001.
Article
Google Scholar
Blair C, Granger DA, Willoughby M, Mills-Koonce R, Cox M, Greenberg MT, et al. Salivary cortisol mediates effects of poverty and parenting on executive functions in early childhood. Child Dev. 2011;82(6):1970–84. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01643.x.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Rhoades BL, Greenberg MT, Lanza ST, Blair C. Demographic and familial predictors of early executive function development: contribution of a person-centered perspective. J Exp Child Psychol. 2011;108(3):638–62. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2010.08.004.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Evans GW, Eckenrode J, Marcynyszyn LA. Chaos and the macrosetting: The role of poverty and socioeconomic status. In: Evans GW, Wachs TD, editors. Chaos and its influence on children’s development: an ecological perspective. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2010. p. 225–38. https://doi.org/10.1037/%0A12057-014.
Chapter
Google Scholar
Lichter DT, Wethington E. Chaos and the diverging fortunes of American children: a historical perspective. In: Evans GW, Wachs TD, editors. Chaos and its influence on children’s development: an ecological perspective. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2010. p. 15–328. https://doi.org/10.1037/12057-002.
Chapter
Google Scholar
Mills-Koonce WR, Willoughby MT, Garrett-Peters P, Wagner N, Vernon-Feagans L. The interplay among socioeconomic status, household chaos, and parenting in the prediction of child conduct problems and callous-unemotional behaviors. Dev Psychopathol. 2016;28(3):757–71. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579416000298.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Vernon-Feagans L, Garrett-Peters P, Willoughby M, Mills-Koonce R, Cox M, Blair C, et al. Chaos, poverty, and parenting: predictors of early language development. Early Child Res Q. 2012;27(3):339–51. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2011.11.001.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Valiente C, Lemery-Chalfant K, Reiser M. Pathways to problem behaviors: chaotic homes, parent and child effortful control, and parenting. Soc Dev. 2007;16(2):249–67. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00383.
Article
Google Scholar
Pennebaker JW, Chung CK, Ireland M, Gonzales A, Booth RJ. The development and psychometric properties of LIWC2007. Austin; 2007. www.LIWC.net.
Anderson P. Assessment and development of executive function (EF) during childhood. Child Neuropsychol. 2002;8(2):71–82. https://doi.org/10.1076/chin.8.2.71.8724.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Sherman EMS, Brooks BL. Behavior rating inventory of executive function - preschool version (BRIEF-P): test review and clinical guidelines for use. Child Neuropsychol. 2010;16(5):503–19. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297041003679344.
Article
Google Scholar
Matthews J, Ponitz C, Morrison F. Early gender differences in self-regulation and academic achievement. J Educ Psychol. 2009;101:689–704. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014240.
Article
Google Scholar
Montroy JJ, Bowles RP, Skibbe LE, McClelland MM, Morrison FJ. The development of self-regulation across early childhood. Dev Psychol. 2016;52(11):1744–62. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000159.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Wanless S, Kim K, Zhang C, Degol J, Chen J, Chen F. Trajectories of behavioral regulation for Taiwanese children from 3.5 to 6 years and relations to math and vocabulary outcomes. Early Child Res Q. 2016;34:104–14. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2015.10.001.
Article
Google Scholar
Lawson GM, Hook CJ, Farah MJ. A meta-analysis of the relationship between socioeconomic status and executive function performance among children. Dev Sci. 2017;21(2):1–22. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12529.
Article
Google Scholar
Cuevas K, Deater-Deckard K, Kim-Spoon J, Wang Z, Morasch KC, Bell MA. A longitudinal intergenerational analysis of executive functions during early childhood. Br Psychol Soc. 2014;32:50–64. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjdp.12021.
Article
Google Scholar
Gueron-Sela N, Camerota M, Willoughby M, Vernon-Feagans L, Cox M, The Family Life Project Key Investigators. Maternal depressive symptoms, mother–child interactions and children’s executive function. Dev Psychol. 2018;54(1):71–82. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000389.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Hughes C, Roman G, Hart MJ, Ensor R. Does maternal depression predict young children’s executive function? A 4-year longitudinal study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2013;54(2):169–77. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12014.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
McCubbin HI, Patterson J, Wilson L. Family inventory of life events and changes (FILE). In: McCubbin H, Thompson A, McCubbin M, editors. Family assessment: Resiliency, coping, and adaptation-Inventories for research and practice. Madison, WI: University of Wisconsin System; 1983. p. 103–78.
Google Scholar
Saxbe DE, Repetti R. No place like home: home tours correlate with daily patterns of mood and cortisol. Person Soc Psychol Bull. 2010;36(1):71–81. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167209352864.
Article
Google Scholar
QSR International Pty Ltd. NVivo qualitative data analysis software. Version 12 Plus. 2018.
Stevenson-Hinde J, Shouldice A. Maternal interactions and self-reports related to attachment classifications at 4.5 years. Child Dev. 1995;66(3):583–96.
Article
Google Scholar
Prime H, Browne D, Akbari E, Wade M, Madigan S, Jenkins JM. The development of a measure of maternal cognitive sensitivity appropriate for use in primary care health settings. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2015;56(4):488–95.
Article
Google Scholar
Biringen Z. The emotional availability (EA) scales. 4th ed. Fort Collins: Department of Human Development and Family Studies, Colorado State University; 2008.
Google Scholar
Weintraub S, Bauer PJ, Zelazo PD, Wallner AK, Dikmen SS, Heaton RK, et al. I. National Institutes of ealth Toolbox Cognition Battery (CB): introduction and pediatric data. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev. 2013;78(4):1–149. https://doi.org/10.1111/mono.12031.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Carlson SM. Developmentally sensitive measures of executive function in preschool children. Dev Neuropsychol. 2005;28(2):595–616. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326942dn2802.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Strommen E. Verbal Self-regulation in a children’s game: impulsive errors on “Simon Says.” Child Dev. 1973;44(4):849–53.
Article
Google Scholar
Zelazo PD, Anderson JE, Richler J, Wallner-Allen K, Beaumont JL, Weintraub S II. NIH toolbox cognition battery (CB): measuring executive function and attention. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev. 2013;78(4):16–33.
Article
Google Scholar
Bauer PJ, Dikmen SS, Heaton RK, Mungas D, Slotkin J, Beaumont JL III. NIH toolbox cognition battery (CB): measuring episodic memory. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev. 2013;78(4):34–48.
Article
Google Scholar
Dikmen SS, Bauer PJ, Weintraub S, Mungas D, Slotkin J, Beaumont JL, et al. Measuring episodic memory across the lifespan: NIH toolbox picture sequence memory test. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2014;20(6):611–9.
Article
Google Scholar
Radloff L. The CES-D scale: a self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Appl Psychol Meas. 1997;1(3):385–401. https://doi.org/10.1177/014662167700100306.
Article
Google Scholar
Muthén L, Muthén B. Mplus user’s guide, 8th edn. Muthén & Muthén; 2017.
Song MK, Lin FC, Ward SE, Fine JP. Composite variables: when and how. Nurs Res. 2013;62(1):45. https://doi.org/10.1097/NNR.0b013e3182741948.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
MacKinnon DP, Lockwood CM, Williams J. Confidence limits for the indirect effect: distribution of the product and resampling methods. Multivar Behav Res. 2004;39:99–128. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327906mbr3901_4.
Article
Google Scholar
Hu L, Bentler P. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional versus new alternatives. Struct Equ Model. 1999;6:1–55. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118.
Article
Google Scholar
Rioux C, Little T. Missing data treatments in intervention studies: what was, what is, and what should be. Int J Behav Dev. 2019. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025419880609.
Article
Google Scholar
Dumas JE, Nissley J, Nordstrom A, Smith EP, Prinz RJ, Levine DW. Home chaos: sociodemographic, parenting, interactional, and child correlates. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2005;34(1):93–104. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15374424jccp3401_9.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Evans G, Kliewer W, Martin J. The role of the physical environment in the health and well-being of children. In: Schroeder H, editor. New directions in health psychology assessment. Washington, DC: Hemisphere Publishing Corporation; 1991. p. 127–57.
Google Scholar
Evans GW, Stecker R. Motivational consequences of environmental stress. J Environ Psychol. 2004;24(2):143–65. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0272-4944(03)00076-8.
Article
Google Scholar
Schmitt SA, Finders JK, McClelland MM. Residential mobility, inhibitory control, and academic achievement in preschool. Early Educ Dev. 2015;26(2):189–208. https://doi.org/10.1080/10409289.2015.975033.
Article
Google Scholar
Coe JL, Davies PT, Hentges RF, Sturge-Apple ML. Understanding the nature of associations between family instability, unsupportive parenting, and children’s externalizing symptoms. Dev Psychopathol. 2019. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579418001736.
Article
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Fomby P, Cherlin A. Family instability and child well-being. Am Sociol Rev. 2007;72(2):181–204. https://doi.org/10.1038/jid.2014.371.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Forman E, Davies P. Family instability and young adolescent maladjustment: the mediating effects of parenting quality and adolescent appraisals of family security. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2003;32:94–105.
Article
Google Scholar
Barton AW, Brody GH, Yu T, Kogan SM, Chen E, Ehrlich KB. The profundity of the everyday: family routines in adolescence predict development in young adulthood. J Adolesc Heal. 2018. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2018.08.029.
Article
Google Scholar
Deater-Deckard K, Wang Z, Chen N, Bell MA. Maternal executive function, harsh parenting, and child conduct problems. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discip. 2012;53(10):1084–91. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02582.x.
Article
Google Scholar
O’Toole S, Monks CP, Tsermentseli S. Associations between and development of cool and hot executive functions across early childhood. Br J Dev Psychol. 2018;36(1):142–8. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjdp.12226.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Lensing N, Elsner B. Development of hot and cool executive functions in middle childhood: three-year growth curves of decision making and working memory updating. J Exp Child Psychol. 2018;173:187–204. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2018.04.002.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar