Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Early maladaptive schemas as common and specific predictors of skin picking subtypes

Abstract

Background

Three distinct subtypes of Skin Picking (SP) have been identified in previous research: Focused, Automatic and Mixed. Early Maladaptive Schemas (EMS) were not investigated across the subtypes. Understanding which EMS are associated with the subtypes might suggest the evaluation of Schema Therapy for SP and guide clinicians using it according to subtypes. The current study explored the relationship between EMS and SP subtypes in community adults.

Methods

Five hundred ninety-six adults [mean age = 35.23 years, 66% females] self-reporting SP behaviours completed the Milwaukee Inventory for Dimensions of Adult Skin Picking and the Young Schema Questionnaire-Long form third version (YSQ-L3).

Results

Higher Dependence/Incompetence EMS was a common predictor of both Focused and Automatic subtypes, while lower Emotional Deprivation EMS and younger age predicted all three subtypes. Higher Approval/Recognition Seeking, Mistrust/Abuse and Failure to Achieve were specific predictors of Automatic, Focused and Mixed subtypes, respectively. Lower Social Isolation/Alienation and Enmeshment/Undeveloped Self were specific predictors of Focused subtype. Male gender was a specific predictor of Mixed subtype.

Conclusions

The assessment and psychological treatment of individuals with SP behaviour may focus on specific EMS. Future longitudinal studies using clinical samples may clarify this association.

Peer Review reports

Background

Skin picking subtypes

Skin Picking (SP) consists of repetitive picking behaviours associated with distress and often social avoidance, consequence of skin damage caused by picking [1]. Individuals pick small irregularities or skin lesions (scars, pimples or scabs), resulting from previous picks, in the most accessible areas, such as face, arms/hands/legs [2]. Prevalence ranges between 1.4 and 5.4% in the community [1] with women and younger people reporting this behaviour more frequently [3].

Different subtypes with distinct functional characteristics were identified. Arnold and colleagues [4] identified three subtypes: (1) Compulsive SP, performed in full awareness to cope with negative emotions; (2) Impulsive/Automatic SP, performed with minimal awareness; (3) a Mixed subtype sharing features of both. In non-clinical individuals, Walther and colleagues [5] identified two subtypes: an Automatic one, that tends to occur outside of one’s awareness, including situations where the individual picks his/her skin while engaged in a sedentary activity (e.g., reading/watching television); a Focused subtype, performed to cope with negative feelings or to correct perceived imperfections. These characteristics of the SP subtypes have been confirmed in further studies where the Automatic one correlated with measures of emotional clarity and the Focused one with measures of emotion regulation difficulties [6]. In non-clinical samples Pozza and colleagues [3] reported three subtypes, including an Automatic, a Focused and a Mixed one.

The distinction between subtypes has implications for practice. Focused SP would be associated with emotion regulation deficits, whereas the Automatic subtype would be related to poorer awareness of picking [7]. Various emotion regulation difficulties, including impulsiveness, were associated with Focused/Compulsive subtype [8]. It might be expected that Focused SP benefits from treatment increasing emotion regulation, whereas Automatic SP benefits from treatment increasing awareness of picking behaviour and the individual’s capacity to stop it [7]. However, research provided mixed findings about the specificity of emotion regulation deficits to Focused subtype, as they correlated also with the Automatic one [9].

Personality constructs and early maladaptive schemas in SP

Little research explored personality dimensions across subtypes. In non-clinical/clinical samples avoidant personality traits predicted Automatic SP, borderline traits predicted both Automatic and Focused SP and sadistic ones predicted Mixed SP [10].

A construct related to personality and interpersonal processes are Early Maladaptive Schemas (EMS), defined as “a broad, pervasive theme or pattern, comprised of memories, emotions, cognitions, and bodily sensations, regarding oneself and one’s relationships with others, developed during childhood or adolescence, elaborated throughout one’s lifetime and dysfunctional to a significant degree” [11]. EMS are trait-like features which develop through interactions between temperament and adverse relational experiences during childhood, when one or more of five basic psychological needs (secure attachment, autonomy, realistic limits, self-directedness, and playfulness) are unmet [11]. When an EMS is activated, the person may respond to it by adopting a maladaptive coping style, including compensation, avoidance or surrender modalities [11]. EMS are grouped in five core domains of unmet needs; they would develop through the interactions between innate temperament and early adverse relational experiences during childhood, when one or more of five basic psychological needs (secure attachment, autonomy, realistic limits, self-directedness, and playfulness) are not satisfied by the caregivers [12]. An overview of all the EMS and the related core domains is presented in Table 1.

Table 1 Early maladaptive schemas and core domains

EMS often act outside of cognitive awareness as a vulnerability factor for a variety of psychological conditions, including depressive/anxiety and personality disorders [11, 13,14,15]. Schema Therapy (ST) [11], developed to modify EMS, assumes that childhood experiences have a key role in the development of emotion regulation strategies. Adverse childhood experiences can lead to fear and avoidance of negative emotions [11].

Cognitive behavioural models explained development and maintenance processes of SP to help conceptualization in clinical practice [16]. SP behaviours are triggered by biased thoughts/beliefs deriving from EMS (“People won’t like me at the party”); activation of such thoughts leads to negative emotions, then to enactment of SP behaviour. Positive/negative reinforcement plays as maintenance process. However, no study examined the role of EMS in SP behaviours and subtypes.

Rationale and objectives

It is important to understand processes associated with SP to refine its conceptualization, then optimise treatment. The role of personality is under-investigated: no study examined EMS across subtypes. Understanding which EMS are associated with subtypes may suggest the future use of ST according to the SP subtype. The current study explored the relationship between EMS and SP subtypes in community adults who self-reported SP behaviours.

Material and methods

Participants

Five-hundred ninety-six community adults (Table 2) [mean age = 35.23 years, 66% females] were recruited through convenience sampling by psychologists in different public settings (universities, libraries, sports/volunteering associations). Participants were identified in the context of a large survey on body focused repetitive behaviours conducted in the general population. Participants were provided with study aims, a description of SP behaviours, the fact that they occur in the general population. Eligibility criteria included the fact that participants had provided written informed consent and that they had scored at least one standard deviation above the normative mean [6] on the Milwaukee Inventory for the Dimensions of Adult Skin Picking (MIDAS) [5], a self-report measure of SP behaviours, suggesting the presence of SP behaviours to some extent. Participation was anonymous, voluntary and uncompensated. The study was approved by the ethical committee of the university institution where it was conducted.

Table 2 Demographics of the group (n = 596)

Measures

When each participant was considered as eligible, he/she was taken aside to complete two questionnaires individually: the MIDAS [5] and the Young Schema Questionnaire-Long form third version (YSQ-L3) [17].

The MIDAS is a 12-item questionnaire assessing SP subtypes: each item is rated from 1 (not true for any of my SP behaviours) to 5 (true for all my SP behaviours). It is the only measure of SP subtypes: a Focused one, which concerns specific body areas and occurs in response to negative emotions or bodily sensations, and an Automatic subtype, which occurs without awareness during activities not related to picking. On 92 participants self-reporting SP, the validation study [5] identified two subscales, measuring respectively Focused and Automatic SP. The Italian version [6] showed 3 factors, Focused (example item: “I pick my skin when I am experiencing a negative emotion such as stress, anger, frustration, or sadness”), Automatic (“I pick my skin when I am concentrating on another activity”), and Mixed SP (“I pick my skin while I am looking in the mirror”).13 In the current group, internal consistency was good for Focused (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.87) and Automatic (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.81) and acceptable for the Mixed scale (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.70).

The YSQ-L3 is a 232-item questionnaire assessing 18 EMS. Participants are asked to rate each statement on a 6-point Likert scale (not true at all = 1, this describes me perfectly = 6). Items are clustered by 18 scales and grouped into 5 domains, bringing together the EMS that develop together: (1) Disconnection/Rejection (EMS: Abandonment, Mistrust/Abuse, Emotional Deprivation, Defectiveness/Shame, Social Isolation/Alienation); (2) Impaired Autonomy/Performance (EMS: Dependence/Incompetence, Vulnerability to Harm/Illness, Enmeshment/Undeveloped Self, Failure); (3) Impaired Limits (EMS: Entitlement/Grandiosity, Insufficient Self-Control/Self-Discipline); (4) Other-Directedness (EMS: Subjugation, Self-Sacrifice, Approval/Recognition-Seeking); and (5) Overvigilance/Inhibition (EMS: Negativity/Pessimism, Emotional Inhibition, Unrelenting Standards/Hypercriticalness, Punitiveness). Higher scores reflect stronger EMS. In the current group, internal consistency was good to excellent across the 18 scales (Cronbach’s alpha range = 0.82–0.91).

Statistical analysis

To test the relationship between SP subtypes and EMS, Pearson’s bivariate correlation coefficients were calculated between MIDAS and YSQ-L3 scores. Coefficient values were interpreted as: 0 < r < |0.30| = weak, |0.30| < r < |0.50| = moderate, |0.50| < r < ±|0.70| = strong, r < ±|0.70| = very strong. Fisher’s z coefficients were computed to compare the strength of the correlation indices between the scores on each one of the MIDAS scales and the scores on each one of the YSQ-L3 scales. Analyses of variance were carried out to explore gender differences on MIDAS scores. Analyses of covariance were conducted by including YSQ-L3 scale scores, age and gender as predictors and MIDAS scores (Focused, Automatic, Mixed scores) as outcomes. Statistical analyses were conducted through SPSS version 25.0 with a significance level set at p < 0.05.

Results

Correlations between EMS and SP subtypes

Pearson’s coefficients between MIDAS and YSQ-L3 scores are presented in Table 3. MIDAS Automatic scores correlated positively and strongly with MIDAS Focused scores and positively and moderately with MIDAS Mixed scores. MIDAS Focused scores correlated moderately and positively with MIDAS Mixed scores.

Table 3 Bivariate correlations between MIDAS and YSQ-L3 scores and gender differences on MIDAS scores (n = 596)

MIDAS Automatic scores correlated positively and weakly with scores on all the YSQ-L3 scales, except with YSQ-L3 Approval/Recognition Seeking and YSQ-L3 Dependence/Incompetence scores, which correlated moderately with MIDAS Automatic scores. MIDAS Automatic scores correlated negatively and weakly with YSQ-L3 Emotional Deprivation scores.

MIDAS Focused scores correlated positively and weakly with all YSQ-L3 scale scores and negatively and weakly with YSQ-L3 Emotional Deprivation scores.

MIDAS Mixed scores correlated positively and moderately with YSQ-L3 Failure to Achieve and with YSQ-L3 Negativity/Pessimism scores and positively and weakly with the scores on all the other YSQ-L3 scales, except for YSQ-L3 Emotional Deprivation, which correlated weakly and negatively with MIDAS Mixed scores.

Age correlated negatively and weakly with all the MIDAS scales scores: the intensity of all the three SP subtypes was higher for younger individuals. Gender differences were found only on MIDAS Mixed scores: females had significantly higher scores than males.

The results of Fisher’s z tests showed that scores on the MIDAS Mixed scale correlated with scores on the YSQ-L3 Social Isolation/Alienation, YSQ-L3 Defectiveness/Shame, YSQ-L3 Failure to Achieve, and YSQ-L3 Negativity/Pessimism scales more strongly than scores on the other MIDAS scales. Scores on the MIDAS Automatic scale correlated with scores on the YSQ-L3 Dependence/Incompetence scale more strongly than scores on the other MIDAS scales.

Predictive effect of EMS on SP subtypes

The results of analyses of covariance are in Tables 4, 5 and 6. YSQ-L3 scores, age (which correlated negatively and weakly with all the MIDAS scale scores) and gender were included as predictors and MIDAS scores as outcomes. Higher YSQ-L3 Dependence/Incompetence and YSQ-L3 Approval/Recognition Seeking scores predicted higher MIDAS Automatic scores, while lower YSQ-L3 Emotional Deprivation scores and younger age predicted higher MIDAS Automatic scores. Scores on the other scales of the YSQ-L3 did not predict significantly MIDAS Automatic scores.

Table 4 Multiple linear regression of MIDAS automatic scores on YSQ – L3 scores (n = 596)
Table 5 Multiple linear regression of MIDAS focused scores on YSQ - L3 scores (n = 596)
Table 6 Multiple linear regression of MIDAS Mixed scores on YSQ-L3 scores (n = 596)

Higher YSQ-L3 Dependence/Incompetence and YSQ-L3 Mistrust/Abuse scores predicted higher MIDAS Focused scores. Lower YSQ-L3 Emotional Deprivation, YSQ-L3 Social Isolation/Alienation, YSQ-L3 Enmeshment/Undeveloped Self scores and younger age predicted higher MIDAS Focused scores. Scores on the other scales of the YSQ-L3 did not predict significantly MIDAS Focused scores.

Lower YSQ-L3 Emotional Deprivation scores, male gender and younger age predicted higher MIDAS Mixed scores. Higher YSQ-L3 Failure to Achieve scores predicted higher MIDAS Mixed scores at borderline significance. Scores on the other scales of the YSQ-L3 did not predict MIDAS Mixed scores.

In conclusion, higher Dependence/Incompetence EMS was a common predictor of both Focused and Automatic subtypes, while lower Emotional Deprivation EMS and age predicted all three subtypes. Higher Approval/Recognition Seeking, higher Mistrust/Abuse and higher Failure to Achieve were specific predictors of Automatic, Focused and Mixed subtypes, respectively. Lower Social Isolation/Alienation and Enmeshment/Undeveloped Self were specific predictors of Focused subtype. Male gender was a specific predictor of Mixed subtype.

Discussion

Little research investigated personality features in SP subtypes. The present study was the first contribution assessing the role of EMS in SP subtypes in a large community group self-reporting picking behaviour. Understanding processes associated with SP might help development of tailored conceptualization and treatment strategies targeting different EMS for different subtypes.

Intercorrelation between Automatic and Focused subtypes was strong, whereas correlations of both Automatic and Focused subtypes with Mixed one were moderate. This finding was in contrast with evidence [5] showing that the subtypes were not correlated but it was consistent with prior research reporting moderate to strong intercorrelations [6].

Correlations between subtypes and EMS were all positive except for Emotional Deprivation EMS, which negatively correlated with all three subtypes: individuals with higher expectation that their needs for emotional support will not be adequately met by others reported SP to less extent than those reporting stronger Emotional Deprivation EMS.

Correlations between three SP subtypes and EMS were all weak, except for the association between Mixed SP and Negativity/Pessimism and Failure to Achieve EMS, which were moderate. Thus, individuals self-reporting Mixed SP endorsed stronger beliefs of being fundamentally inadequate relative to peers in areas of achievement and a focus on the negative aspects of life while neglecting the positive ones. This evidence is consistent with the association between Mixed SP and depression [6]. A moderate correlation was found also between Automatic SP and Approval/Recognition Seeking EMS: individuals with more intense Automatic SP behaviour tend to put excessive emphasis on gaining approval from others, at the expense of developing a true sense of self, consistently with reports indicating approval seeking and separation anxiety as predictors of avoidant personality [18]. This evidence might support the hypothesis that avoidant personality traits might be involved in the Automatic subtype, and that this subtype might be related to the so-called vulnerable narcissistic personality picture, often associated to avoidant personality, in the same manner as self-injurious behaviour [19]. Analyses of covariance showed that the three SP subtypes were associated with common EMS and differentially with specific EMS.

EMS common to subtypes

Higher Dependence/Incompetence EMS predicted stronger both Automatic and Focused SP, whereas lower Emotional Deprivation was common to all subtypes. This result indicated that individuals viewing themselves as unable to handle everyday responsibilities without help from others tend to report both subtypes. The relation between the two subtypes and Dependence/Incompetence EMS may be explained by the fact that individuals who are not able to stop picking may feel frustrated, then may get into a helpless vicious cycle [20]. The connection between these EMS and SP might be an effect also of the association between subtypes, particularly Focused, and depression [21]. This result is consistent with a study [22] showing that Dependence/Incompetence EMS were associated with non-suicidal self-injurious behaviour. Supporting the link between Emotional Dependence and SP, Estévez and colleagues [23] reported that Emotional Dependence was associated with impulsive behaviour and that it mediated the relation between Attachment and Impulsivity. The result about the role of Dependence in Automatic SP is interesting since loneliness was an emotional trigger of SP behaviour [24]. In addition, the association between a sense of incompetence and SP may be attributed to a negative self-judgemental attitude, a cognitive factor found to be related to depressive traits [25].

Emotional Deprivation EMS resulted a protective factor against all the three subtypes of SP, as more intense SP behaviour was associated with lower Emotional Deprivation. This result indicated that those individuals self-reporting picking behaviour to less extent believed that their needs for emotional support are not adequately met by others. This result might be in contrast with the literature, since Emotional Deprivation is believed a risk or maintenance factor for a number of symptoms and disorders [26]. However, it might be hypothesized that individuals who have SP tendencies have difficulties identifying/describing feelings, given the relation between Alexithymia and SP [21]; thus, they could not identify these needs as unmet. Since Obsessive Compulsive Disorder (OCD) and body focused repetitive behaviours have clinical overlap [20], a further explanation might be in research on parenting associated with development of OCD [27], where OCD severity was linked with parental overprotection.

Younger age predicted stronger levels on all three SP subtypes, supporting evidence that SP behaviours concern mostly late adolescents and young adults [28].

EMS specific to SP subtypes

Higher Approval/Recognition Seeking predicted stronger Automatic SP. This association might be explained by perfectionism reported in SP: feelings of dissatisfaction and imperfection regarding the body can activate the picking episode with the aim to obtain approval/recognition by others and avoid feelings of shame or social rejection [28, 29]. Many individuals suffering from SP begin picking at an area of imperfection/blemish that they fixate on [30]. The role of Approval/Recognition Seeking seemed in line with case reports of Body Focused Repetitive Behaviours: Pélissier and O’Connor [31] described a patient who identified frustration/impatience as dominant emotions during the injurious behaviour (occurring when she was waiting or on wasting time) and identified automatic thoughts (“I’m not fast enough”, “I’m not performing well”), which increased her tension provoking the behaviour. Approval/Recognition Seeking might be considered an outcome of an authoritarian parenting and less parental acceptance which literature found playing a role in the development of OCD spectrum [27, 32].

Higher Emotional Deprivation predicted less intense both Automatic and Focused SP. This result was in contrast with previous evidence [9], where avoidant traits were related to SP and with research [26] indicating that shame proneness/social avoidance were associated with SP, due to skin lesions. Maybe the use of a community group accounts for this difference, as the individuals did not have clinical SP resulting in tissue damage.

Higher Mistrust/Abuse EMS predicted Focused subtype. This relation was consistent with evidence demonstrating that the Focused subtype is associated with borderline traits [10], since the Disconnection/Rejection domain was the most specific to borderline personality and Abuse/Mistrust EMS was one of the most closely associated EMS to borderline traits [26]. The association between Focused SP and borderline personality was demonstrated in a study where patients with Borderline Personality Disorder reported higher Focused SP behaviour than controls [10]. The role of Mistrust/Abuse EMS may be consistent with evidence showing that SP is associated with a history of childhood sexual/interpersonal abuse [33], which often predicts borderline personality [34], The association between Mistrust/Abuse EMS and Focused SP found in the current study might be consistent with models of self-injurious behaviour, which is seen as a compensatory regulation in posttraumatic adaptation [35]. In a study with outpatients, Mistrust/Abuse EMS correlated moderately with socially avoidant and vindictive interpersonal behaviours. This evidence supports the relation found in the current study between Mistrust/Abuse and SP, as in previous research also avoidant and passive–aggressive personality predicted significantly the Focused SP subtype [10].

The lack of a significant relation between SP subtypes and Shame/Defectiveness EMS appeared in contrast with studies demonstrating an association between social avoidance/shame and SP behaviour [36].

Higher Failure to Achieve EMS predicted stronger Mixed SP tendencies. Individuals with stronger Mixed SP tendencies seem to focus on the negative aspects of life while minimizing the positive ones and viewing themselves as inadequate relative to peers. These associations might be explained by the role of negative self-evaluation including higher disgust propensity in OCD spectrum disorders [37, 38].

Conclusions

The three SP subtypes were associated with common EMS and with distinct EMS. Higher Dependence/Incompetence EMS was a common predictor of both Focused and Automatic subtypes, while lower Emotional Deprivation EMS and age were common predictors of all three subtypes. Higher Approval/Recognition Seeking, higher Mistrust/Abuse and higher Failure to Achieve were specific predictors of Automatic, Focused and Mixed subtypes, respectively. Lower Social Isolation/Alienation and Enmeshment/Undeveloped Self were specific predictors of Focused subtype. Male gender was a specific predictor of Mixed subtype.

Some limitations should be considered. First, the study used a community group reporting SP but it did not ascertain the diagnosis of SP through a clinician-administered instrument. The cross-sectional design prevented conclusions about the causal link between EMS and SP: EMS might be reinforced by the consequences produced by picking behaviour in the body and the difficulty stopping it (e.g., Dependence/Incompetence EMS). A longitudinal design would provide more confidence in the associations among these variables, since the variables are measured at two (or more) points in time, with lag relationships assessed.

The effects of some clinical variables, such as anxiety and depression, were not controlled for. Given that the study consists of correlating one set of self-report scales that involve reporting negative aspects about oneself (i.e., skin picking behaviours) with another set of scales that involve reporting negative aspects about oneself (i.e., EMS), a key point would be to control for the large component of self-report scales related to negative reporting about oneself. It might be expected that most of correlations found would reduce considerably once this reporting bias is controlled. Alternative methods to measure the activation of EMS as triggers of SP episodes might be useful. Future studies for example might use app-based diaries to assess whether activation of EMS precedes picking.

In conclusion, the present findings expand the literature on the relationship between personality characteristics and SP subtypes, investigating for the first time the role of EMS: SP subtypes seem to be associated with common and specific EMS. This highlights a potential future application and evaluation of ST in a personalised approach for this impairing, yet under-investigated, condition.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

EMS:

Early Maladaptive Schemas

MIDAS:

Milwaukee Inventory for the Dimensions of Adult Skin Picking

OCD:

Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder

SP:

Skin Picking

ST:

Schema Therapy

YSQ-L3:

Young Schema Questionnaire-Long form third version

References

  1. Odlaug BL, Lust K, Schreiber LRN, Christenson G, Derbyshire K, Grant JE. Skin picking disorder in university students: health correlates and gender differences. Gen Hosp Psychiatry. 2013;35:168–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Snorrason I, Smári J, Ólafsson RP. Emotion regulation in pathological skin picking: findings from a non-treatment seeking sample. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 2010;41:238–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Pozza A, Mazzoni GP, Patassini I, et al. Studio sulle proprietà psicometriche della versione italiana del Milwaukee Inventory for the Dimensions of Adult Skin Picking (MIDAS) in campioni clinici e non clinici. Psicoterapia Cognitiva e Comportamentale. 2016;22:35–63.

    Google Scholar 

  4. Arnold LM, McElroy SL, Mutasim DF, Dwight MM, Lamerson CL, Morris EM. Characteristics of 34 adults with psychogenic excoriation. J Clin Psychiatry. 1998;59:509–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Walther MR, Flessner CA, Conelea CA, Woods DW. The Milwaukee Inventory for the Dimensions of Adult Skin Picking (MIDAS): initial development and psychometric properties. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 2009;40:127–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Schienle A, Zorjan S, Übel S, Wabnegger A. Prediction of automatic and focused skin picking based on trait disgust and emotion dysregulation. J Obsessive Compuls Relat Disord. 2018;16:1–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Twohig MP, Woods DW. Habit reversal as a treatment for chronic skin picking in typically developing adult male siblings. J Appl Behav Anal. 2001;34:217–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Snorrason Í, Smári J, Ólafsson RP. Motor inhibition, reflection impulsivity, and trait impulsivity in pathological skin picking. Behav Ther. 2011;42:521–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Pozza A. The role of alexithymia traits in compulsive skin picking behaviors. In: Teixeira R, Moorman PP, Bermond B, editors. Current developments in alexithymia: a cognitive and affective deficit. New York: Nova Publishers; 2018. p. 131–46.

    Google Scholar 

  10. Pozza A, Giaquinta N, Dèttore D. Borderline, avoidant, sadistic personality traits and emotion dysregulation predict different pathological skin picking subtypes in a community sample. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2016;12:1861–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Young JE, Klosko JS, Weishaar ME. Schema therapy: a practitioner’s guide. New York: Guilford Press; 2003. p. 7–8.

    Google Scholar 

  12. Rafaeli E, Bernstein DP, Young J. Schema therapy: distinctive features. New York, NY: Routledge; 2011.

    Google Scholar 

  13. Brotchie J, Meyer C, Copello A, Kidney R, Waller G. Cognitive representations in alcohol and opiate abuse: the role of core beliefs. Br J Clin Psychol. 2004;43:337–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Pinto-Gouveia J, Castilho P, Galhardo A, Cunha M. Early maladaptive schemas and social phobia. Cognitive Ther Res. 2006;30:571–84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Reeves M, Taylor J. Specific relationships between core beliefs and personality disorder symptoms in a non-clinical sample. Clin Psychol Psychother. 2007;14:96–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Woods DW, Snorrason I, Epsil FM. Trichotillomania, skin picking, & other body-focused repetitive behaviors. In: Grant JE, Stein DJ, Woods DW, Keuthen NJ, editors. Cognitive-behavioral therapy in adults. Arlington: American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc; 2012. p. 21–41.

    Google Scholar 

  17. Young JE, Brown G. Young schema-questionnaire. In: Young JE, editor. Cognitive therapy for personality disorders: a Schema-focused approach. 2nd ed. Sarasota: Professional Resource Press; 1994. p. 63–76.

    Google Scholar 

  18. Eikenæs I, Pedersen G, Wilberg T. Attachment styles in patients with avoidant personality disorder compared with social phobia. Psychol Psychother-T. 2016;89:245–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Dawood S, Schroder HS, Donnellan MB, Pincus AL. Pathological narcissism and non-suicidal self-injury. J Personal Disord. 2018;32:87–108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Snorrason I, Belleau EL, Woods DW. How related are hair pulling disorder (trichotillomania) and skin picking disorder? A review of evidence for comorbidity, similarities and shared etiology. Clin Psychol Rev. 2012;32:618–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Pozza A, Giaquinta N, Dèttore D. Impulsività ed Alessitimia nei sottotipi di Skin Picking: uno studio su un ampio campione non-clinico. G Ital Psicol. 2017;44:917–38.

    Google Scholar 

  22. Lewis SP, Lumley MN, Grunberg PH. Early maladaptive schemas and non-suicidal self-injury among young adults: a preliminary investigation. Couns Psychol Q. 2015;28:386–402.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Estévez A, Chávez-Vera MD, Momeñe J, Olave L, Vázquez D, Iruarrizaga I. The role of emotional dependence in the relationship between attachment and impulsive behavior. Ann Psychol. 2018;34:438–45.

    Google Scholar 

  24. Wilhelm S, Keuthen NJ, Deckersbach T, et al. Self-injurious skin picking: clinical characteristics and comorbidity. J Clin Psychiatry. 1999;60:454–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Barcaccia B, Baiocco R, Pozza A, Pallini S, Mancini F, Salvati M. The more you judge the worse you feel. A judgemental attitude towards one’s inner experience predicts depression and anxiety. Pers Individ Diff. 2019;138:33–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Barazandeh H, Kissane DW, Saeedi N, Gordon M. A systematic review of the relationship between early maladaptive schemas and borderline personality disorder/traits. Pers Individ Diff. 2016;94:130–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Timpano KR, Keough ME, Mahaffey B, Schmidt NB, Abramowitz J. Parenting and obsessive compulsive symptoms: implications of authoritarian parenting. Int J Cogn Ther. 2016;24:151–64.

    Google Scholar 

  28. Odlaug BL, Grant JE. Trichotillomania and pathologic skin picking: clinical comparison with an examination of comorbidity. Ann Clin Psychiatry. 2008;20:57–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Deckersbach T, Wilhelm S, Keuthen N. Self-injurious skin picking: clinical characteristics, assessment methods, and treatment modalities. Brief Treat Crisis Interv. 2003;3:249–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Pélissier MC, O’Connor K. Cognitive-behavioral treatment of trichotillomania, targeting perfectionism. Clin Case Stud. 2004;3:57–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Weingarden H, Renshaw KD. Shame in the obsessive compulsive related disorders: a conceptual review. J Affect Disord. 2015;171:74–84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Pozza A, Albert U, Dèttore D. Perfectionism and intolerance of uncertainty are predictors of OCD symptoms in children and early adolescents: a prospective, cohort, one-year, follow-up study. Clin Neuropsychiatry. 2019;16:53–61.

    Google Scholar 

  33. Neziroglu F, Rabinowitz D, Breytman A, Jacofsky M. Skin picking phenomenology and severity comparison. Prim Care Companion J Clin Psychiatry. 2008;10:306–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Özten E, Sayar GH, Gül Eryılmaz GK, Işık S, Karamustafalıoğlu O. The relationship of psychological trauma with trichotillomania and skin picking. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2015;11:1203–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Ball JS, Links PS. Borderline personality disorder and childhood trauma: evidence for a causal relationship. Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2009;11:63–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Yates TM. The developmental psychopathology of self-injurious behavior: compensatory regulation in posttraumatic adaptation. Clin Psychol Rev. 2004;24:35–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Tucker BT, Woods DW, Flessner CA, Franklin SA, Franklin ME. The skin picking impact project: phenomenology, interference, and treatment utilization of pathological skin picking in a population-based sample. J Anxiety Disord. 2011;25:88–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Pozza A, Mazzoni GP, Berardi D, Dèttore D. Studio preliminare sulle proprietà psicometriche della versione italiana della Disgust Propensity and Sensitivity Scale-Revised (DPSS-R) in campioni non-clinici e campioni clinici con Disturbo Ossessivo-Compulsivo e Disturbi d’ansia. Psicoterapia Cognitiva e Comportamentale. 2016;22:271–96.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

None.

Funding

This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

AP designed the study, collected the data, conducted the literature searches, conducted the statistical analysis, wrote the first draft of the paper, checked the editing of the final version of the paper. UA designed the study, conducted the literature searches, reviewed the first draft of the paper, checked the editing of the final version of the paper. DD designed the study, reviewed the first draft of the paper, checked the editing of the final version of the paper. The authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Andrea Pozza.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The study was approved by the ethical committee of the Florence University where it was conducted. Participants provided written informed consent written consent.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Pozza, A., Albert, U. & Dèttore, D. Early maladaptive schemas as common and specific predictors of skin picking subtypes. BMC Psychol 8, 27 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-020-0392-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-020-0392-y

Keywords