Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Interpersonal reactivity index adaptation among expectant seroconcordant couples with HIV in Zambézia Province, Mozambique

Abstract

Background

The ability to understand another’s emotions and act appropriately, empathy, is an important mediator of relationship function and health intervention fidelity. We adapted the Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI) – an empathy scale – among seroconcordant expectant couples with HIV in the Homens para Saúde Mais (HoPS+) trial – a cluster randomized controlled trial assessing couple-based versus individual treatment on viral suppression – in Zambézia Province, Mozambique.

Methods

Using baseline data from 1332 HoPS+ trial participants (666 couples), an exploratory factor analysis assessed culturally relevant questions from the IRI. Because empathy is interdependent among couples, we validated the results of the exploratory factor analysis using a dyadic confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) with dyadic measurement invariance testing. Finally, we assessed the relationship between scores on our final scale and basic demographic characteristics (sex, age, education, and depression) using t-tests.

Results

We found two subscales: 1) a seven-item cognitive empathy subscale (Cronbach’s alpha 0.78) and 2) a six-item affective empathy subscale (Cronbach’s alpha 0.73). The dyadic CFA found acceptable model fit and metric invariance across partners (Comparative Fit Index (CFI) = 0.914, Tucker Lewis Index = 0.904, Root Mean Squared Error of Approximation = 0.056, ΔCFI = 0.011). We observed higher cognitive (p: 0.012) and affective (p: 0.049) empathy among males and higher cognitive (p: 0.031) and affective (p: 0.030) empathy among younger participants. More educated participants had higher affective empathy (p: 0.017) and depressed participants had higher cognitive empathy (p: < 0.001). This two-subscale, 13-item version of the IRI measures cognitive and affective empathy in HoPS+ trial participants and adults while accounting for the interdependent nature of empathy within partner dyads.

Conclusions

This scale will allow us to assess the interplay between empathy and other psychometric constructs (stigma, social support, etc.) in the HoPS+ trial and how each relates to retention in HIV, adherence to treatment, and prevention of maternal to child HIV transmission. Furthermore, this scale can be adapted for other sub-Saharan African populations, which will allow researchers to better assess HIV-related intervention efficacy.

Trial registration

This study is within the context of the HoPS+ trial, registered at ClinicalTrials.gov as number NCT03149237. Registered May 11, 2017.

Peer Review reports

Background

As of 2017, there were 36.9 million people living with HIV (PLWH) globally, 19.6 million of whom lived within Eastern and Southern sub-Saharan Africa (SSA) [1]. In Zambézia Province, Mozambique, HIV prevalence is estimated to be 15% within the general population, with higher estimated rates in pregnant women [2]. Given the well-documented association between higher maternal HIV viral load and higher likelihood of infant HIV diagnosis [3], understanding the role of empathy and partner empathy - within the medical, social, economic, and cultural setting of rural Mozambique – on retention in care, adherence to treatment, and maternal-to-child transmission among PLWH is essential to decreasing HIV/AIDS-related morbidity and mortality in Mozambique and globally. With the increased focus on couples-based interventions to improve HIV outcomes in SSA, [4,5,6,7,8,9,10] understanding the interpersonal skills of each member within a couple, such as empathy, may be key mediators of the couples-based intervention efficacy. For example, partners who are better able to understand each other’s perspective may be better able to support them in adherence to antiretroviral medication regimes. However, to our knowledge, an empathy scale has never been validated in or adapted anywhere in SSA, which limits our ability to measure the impact of empathy on retention in HIV care in this setting.

Introduction to empathy, partner empathy, and the interpersonal reactivity index

Empathy, the ability to understand another’s emotions and act accordingly, has a cognitive domain — the ability to understand the experiences of others — and an affective domain — emotional responses to others’ experiences [11,12,13]. Empathy is considered essential to understanding the mental states and experiences of others, a key component of social behavior that allows individuals to function within groups [12, 13]. Specific to this study, empathy is recognized as a key mediator of health intervention fidelity and, within the context of HIV, higher physician empathy is associated with improved patient outcomes [14,15,16,17]. It stands to reason that partner empathy would increase emotional support, treatment adherence, retention in care, and relationship quality – all of which contribute to reducing mother-to-child transmission of HIV.

Recently, there has been increased interest in examining empathy among partners, given that partner interdependence within a dyad results in survey item correlation among partners [18,19,20]. These new methods recognize how one partner’s affective empathy influences the other partner’s cognitive empathy and vice versa [20]. They also show that one partner’s empathy is related to supportive behaviors directed towards the other partner [21]. These between-partner relationships demand a methodological approach that recognize subject interdependence (i.e., does not assume subject independence).

The Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI) was developed to assess the distinct cognitive and affective empathy domains [11]. Péloquin and Lafontaine [19] developed and used a modified version of the IRI to assess empathy among couples [19]. Levesque used a dyadic model to further validate the updated IRI scale [19, 20]. We could not, however, find evidence of IRI validation among couples outside of North America. This may limit its assessment of empathy in individuals in other regions, such as within SSA, in individuals with lower levels of education, and within the context of a dyadic relationship outside of North America. This has important implications for interventions targeted to improve health outcomes, including interventions for people living with HIV in SSA.

The current study

This study uses baseline data, specifically demographic data and the IRI, from an ongoing cluster randomized controlled trial — Homens para Saúde Mais (HoPS+) [Men for Health Plus] — that assesses the impact of involving HIV-positive male partners in routine prenatal care for women living with HIV [5]. The HoPS+ trial represents a unique opportunity to take the first step in adapting a measure that will allow researchers to assess empathy, as well as changes associated with behavioral interventions, in Mozambique and, with subsequent studies, in SSA.

The purpose of this study was to adapt the IRI among study participants in the ongoing HoPS+ study through an exploratory factor analysis, dyadic confirmatory factor analysis, and dyadic measurement invariance testing. We used a dyadic approach to account for how each partner’s perceived empathy impacts the other partner’s perceived empathy. This informs understanding of how supportive behaviors, shaped by between-partner interdependence, may augment engagement with and outcomes from HoPS+ and other behavioral interventions. Further, we assess correlations between the adapted IRI measure and related demographic and psychological (e.g., depression) factors, to provide further convergent and divergent validity evidence for the adapted measure. These results will help us evaluate the effect of our intervention on male and female empathy in males and pregnant women living with HIV in Mozambique and SSA and lay the groundwork for future assessments of dyadic constructs, including empathy, in SSA.

Methods

Participant selection

The HoPS+ study protocol is described in detail elsewhere [5]. Briefly, trained local study personnel collected baseline age, sex, education, marital status, occupation, IRI, and Patient Health Questionaire-9 data using a REDCap® (Research Electronic Data Capture) survey administered to pregnant women living with HIV and their seroconcordant male partner from 24 clinic sites in Zambézia Province [5, 22].

This analysis included the first 666 couples, or 1332 individual participants, from all HoPS+ study sites beginning on the date of study initiation (November 16th, 2017) through June 13th, 2019, when data were downloaded from our REDCap® database [22]. Our final study population, after excluding 147 participants (42 complete couples and 51 additional individuals) for missing data, included 1185 individuals (567 complete couples and 51 additional individuals) from 24 sites (Table 1). This included 595 (50.2%) females and 590 (49.8%) males with a median age of 25 (Interquartile Range (IQR) 21–30) and 5 years (IQR 2–7) of education. The most common occupation was farming (46.9%). Three districts, Pebane (30%), Inhassunge (16.5%), and Namacurra (19.3%), were overrepresented in the validation sample as compared with the excluded sampled population (18.4, 1.4, and 8.8%% respectively, p-value < 0.001), likely because when we randomly selected HoPS+ sites, more were located in these three districts than the other two. Furthermore, because of their size, recruitment was initially faster than the more remote, smaller districts. Mungia (43.1%) and Chuabo (37.6%) were the most commonly spoken languages, based on the most popular language(s) in each study district.

Table 1 Patient Demographic Information

Setting

Zambézia Province, located in north-central Mozambique, is home to approximately 4.4 million people from five primary ethnic groups (Chuabo, Macua-Lomwe, Manhaua, Merenge, and Senas) who speak at least four languages in our study area [5]. It has some of the poorest health and development indicators in Mozambique. Mozambique’s literacy rate is 47%; only 28% of women are literate (vs. 60% of men) and these numbers are lower in rural communities [23]. The majority of inhabitants are subsistence farmers [23]. Nationwide, 40% of the population live in poverty, but 80% of those poor live in rural areas like Zambézia [24]. In addition to these contextual conditions, the HIV prevalence in the province is estimated to be 15%, one of the highest in the country [2].

Scale translation and adaptation

This is a novel setting for employing the IRI, and this population presented unique challenges in study implementation. Measures were translated (and back translated to confirm meaning was maintained) from English to Portuguese (a shared language among translators, and a commonly spoken language in Mozambique) and then from Portuguese to Muniga, Chuabo, Lomue, and Nharringa. At least seven trilingual interpreters carefully reviewed each study question and made modifications relevant to the local sociocultural and linguistic context. Specific phrases, including feeling “touched” were not translated verbatim, but were replaced with similar, locally relevant concepts. The final measure was subsequently field tested at each study site before enrolling participants. During interviewer-assisted survey implementation (due to low levels of literacy among participants), statements and response categories were read aloud in each participant’s preferred language on enrollment. Responses were captured by the study counselor. Twenty-four trained counselors fluent in the local language and Portuguese were trained to capture participant responses over two 5-day training sessions. All counselors were supervised in the field by a study manager, who provided regular booster trainings to ensure consistency of survey delivery.

Instruments

Interpersonal reactivity index

The IRI consists of four empathy domains with seven questions each (28 questions total) [11]. The fantasy scale (FS) assesses one’s ability to place oneself in fictional situations; the perspective-taking scale (PT) reflects one’s ability to understand another person’s point of view; the empathic concern scale (EC) measures one’s ability to have caring feelings towards another individual; and the personal distress scale (PD) characterizes an individual’s own negative feelings when witnessing adverse events in others [11]. The fantasy and perspective taking scales constitute the cognitive component of empathy, while the empathic concern and personal distress scales constitute the affective component of empathy [11]. More recent research, albeit conducted in North America, further supports that distinct cognitive and affective empathy domains undergird the IRI scale [25,26,27,28,29]. This includes the development of two-factor empathy scales [25, 28] and imaging and molecular research that suggest distinct, but interrelated, cognitive and affective neural circuitry [27, 29].

Each item is scored on a 5-point Likert-like scale ranging from “Does not describe me well” (0) to “Describes me very well” (4). Although the original IRI contained nine reverse scored items, in the above-described scale adaptation, all questions were positively phrased and scored to avoid confusion during translation and survey administration as well as to improve response accuracy. Previous studies report Cronbach’s alpha values for IRI subscales from 0.70–0.83 and correlation coefficients of 0.01–0.37 between subscales [11, 30,31,32].

Patient health Questionaire-9

The Patient Health Questionaire-9 (PHQ-9) measures the nine attributes that characterize major depressive disorder [33]. Participants rate each attribute from ‘Not at all’ (0) to ‘Nearly every day’ (3) and were considered depressed if they scored 10 or greater. The PHQ-9 has been validated to screen PLWH in SSA for depressive symptoms [34,35,36] and has been used to measure depressive symptoms in Mozambique [37]. Participants who disclosed suicidal ideation (item 9 on the PHQ-9) were immediately assessed by our trained counselors and referred to the psychologist based at each site for additional psychological services.

Missing data

We excluded participants with missing IRI data on more than eight questions (~ 30% of answers), an average of more than two questions per subscale (n = 147; 11%). We believed that these criteria excluded potentially biased data from interactions among interviewer-interviewee pairs who had difficulty administering or understanding the survey, given the low levels of education among our participants. Those missing more than one PHQ-9 item (n = 124; 10.5%) were excluded from our analysis as described above. We used a stricter threshold for the PHQ-9 because it has previously been used in Mozambique and because it was a shorter measure.

Missing IRI and PHQ-9 data for the participants with eight or fewer missing IRI items (n = 276; 23.3%) and one or fewer missing PHQ-9 items (n = 112; 9.4%) were imputed over 10 data sets using non-missing empathy survey questions with the multivariate imputation by chained equations (mice) package version 3.4.0 [38]. We used a chi-squared test to assess the differences between included and excluded participants by sex, marital status, district, highest education, occupation, and predicted language (based on the most frequently spoken language in each district because we did not collect individual level data on language).

Included (n = 1185) and excluded (n = 147) participants did not statistically differ by age or occupation (Table 1). However, there were statistically significant differences by relationship status – a higher percentage of included participants self-identified as in a domestic partnership (37.8 to 24.5%) and fewer self-identified as married (21 to 33.3%). Included and excluded participants also had different levels of education and were differentially representative of districts (p < 0.001) and predicted languages (p < 0.001) (Table 1).

Data preparation

IRI questions were translated into several new languages and administered to participants in Zambézia, who were markedly different from the college-educated and junior high school students in previous IRI validations [19, 20, 30,31,32]. Although it has been used in South Africa as a composite 28-item scale or in its 4-subscale form [39, 40], to our knowledge the IRI has never been validated in or adapted to SSA. Given the new context, language translation, and rephrasing of negatively worded items, we did not feel comfortable making the a priori configural assumptions necessary to start with a confirmatory factor analysis of the IRI. We therefore hypothesized that the factor structure might differ from the previously identified four-factor structure [11]. We randomly split, without replacement, the full sample of 666 couples into two groups to identify (in the first phase) and then confirm, via confirmatory factor analysis (in the second phase) the factor structure of the IRI in discrete dyadic samples. Data were treated as ordinal in both EFA and CFA validation analyses. All data cleaning and analysis was conducted in R, version 3.5.1 (R Foundation for Statistical Computing) (2018-07-02) [41].

Exploratory factor analysis

The exploratory factor analysis (EFA) included 400 individuals (200 dyads split for the analysis). After re-examining the question translations for this analysis, we removed questions number 4 and 9 from the pool of items subjected to analyses, due to discrepancies after translation that changed the meaning of the questions (Supplementary Table 1). We performed maximum-likelihood exploratory factor analyses on the polychoric correlation matrices of two, three, and four-factor solutions consistent with the affective and cognitive attributes of empathy and IRI for couples scale (two factor-model suggested by extant theory) [19, 25,26,27,28,29], the results of a parallel analysis on the imputed datasets (three factor-model suggested by parallel analyses) [42, 43], and previous versions of the IRI (four factors in the initial validation) [11]. We used an oblique promax rotation and selected items for a particular factor if the loading was greater than 0.40 and unique to one factor (i.e., the question did not load greater than 0.40 for another factor; if so, it was discarded). We assessed internal factor consistency with Cronbach’s alpha to compare our results with previous IRI psychometric evidence.

Dyadic confirmatory factor analysis and measurement invariance

We used a dyadic confirmatory factor analysis (CFA), which included 466 dyads distinct from those analyzed in the EFA, to capture the interdependence between partner responses to items on the IRI (Fig. 1). This perspective presupposes that the latent construct being modeled achieves measurement invariance [44]. This means that the latent construct — empathy in this case — means the same thing and is measured the same way across partners. Thus, measurement invariance needs to be tested as part of the dyadic model validation. We examined three progressively restrictive levels of measurement invariance — configural invariance, metric invariance, and scalar invariance — using latent variable analysis (lavaan version 0.6–3) [45]. Statistical power for invariance testing may be “reasonable” with a sample of 200 or more dyads and “adequate” with 400 dyads [46]. Robust absolute and incremental fit indices are reported using standard benchmarks [47,48,49,50].

Fig. 1
figure 1

A Conceptual Diagram of the Dyadic Empathy Confirmatory Factor Analysis. Cognitive and Affective dimensions of empathy are uniquely modeled for male and female partners. Dashed lines from each empathy construct to the corresponding items (boxes) indicate the relation between the construct and the items used to model it. Circles pointing to items represent each item’s error term. Covariances, represented by the curved and dashed lines between the constructs, capture the relationships between partners’ dimensions of empathy. Double-headed arrows between error terms capture the interdependence between partners’ responses to survey items

Configural invariance assesses statistical equivalence of factor loading patterns across both partners. Configural invariance provides evidence that, when underlying latent constructs are measured between partners, items are organized in a similar fashion (i.e., the same set of items measure the same latent constructs for both partners). Constructs exhibiting metric invariance confirm statistically equivalent factor loading values across latent constructs and actors. Metric invariance provides evidence that items are similarly related to the underlying constructs across both actors. Finally, scalar invariance confirms that items are expected to have the same value across actors when the corresponding latent construct equals zero. In other words, the scaling of items is equivalent across partners. Scalar invariance indicates whether the amount of variation in each group is equivalent, and that each groups’ mean differences are interpretable [51, 52]. Power analyses for invariance testing suggests the use of changes in alternative fit indices (ΔAFI). Because the sensitivity of model fit indices may vary by sample size and model specification, simulations on sample sizes larger than 300 suggests evaluating model differences via changes in the confirmatory fit index (ΔCFI) between .002 and .01, and changes in the root mean squared error of approximation (ΔRMSEA) between .007 and .015. ΔRMSEA has been shown to be more sensitive than ΔCFI [46, 53]. Thus, due to the uncertainty of the novel context and language translation, we chose to use the more conservative options of ΔCFI > .002, and did not consider ΔRMSEA (though we do report it) for evaluating measurement invariance.

We report adjusted (robust) and unadjusted fit indices for each configural, metric, and scalar invariance model (see Table 4). Robust fit indices reflect modifications to traditional fit indices to account for failure to meet distributional assumptions, such as non-normality [47, 48]. These adjustments to absolute (Root Mean Squared Error of Approximation - RMSEA) and incremental (Comparative Fit Index (CFI) & Tucker Lewis Index (TLI)) fit indices make use of the Satorra-Bentler scaling constant to modify the equations of the naïve fit estimates [54]. Simulation studies have found these adjustments to be robust across a variety of unmet distributional assumptions and attenuate the overestimation of fit quality.

Demographic comparisons of cognitive and affective empathy

To further probe the construct validity of the resulting factor model, we assessed inter-scale correlation, correlation between subscales and the continuous PHQ-9 scores, and the relationship between each scale and sex, age, education, and depression. We used unpaired t-tests to assess the relationships between each subscale and the dichotomous measure of sex, age (> 26 years-old), education (> 7 years), and depression (> 9 PHQ-9 score is depressed). We hypothesized that participants who were older (> 26) would have lower empathy scores given their exposure to the war of independence and/or civil war and its sequelae which devastated Zambézia Province [55, 56]; that women would have higher empathy scores consistent with previous validations [30,31,32]; that those with more depressive symptoms – due to a higher degree of internalization of difficult emotional situations [57,58,59,60] – and that participants with higher levels of education would have higher levels of empathy, which has been observed in other settings [55]. We then assessed mean empathy subscale scores by district and predicted language with a one-way analysis of variance.

Results

Exploratory factor analysis

All the exploratory factor analyses (343 individuals) resulted in factor structures that mixed items from the four IRI subscales, and, to a lesser extent, between the cognitive and affective attributes of empathy (Table 2). In the absence of a theoretical framework to explain mixing of cognitive and affective items, we selected the two-factor solution (distinct cognitive and affective empathy factors) to minimize these discrepancies. This aligns with more recently developed empathy scales and the number of factors on the IRI for couples scale, which allows for improved interpretability of the two-factor solution instead of the three- or four-factor solutions, which had subscale mixing that limited interpretability [25, 28]. Across ten imputed datasets, factor correlations ranged from 0.56 to 0.59. We then removed cognitive questions that loaded on the predominantly affective scale and vice versa to maintain the integrity of previous empathy scale frameworks [11, 30,31,32].

Table 2 Interpersonal Reactivity Index Exploratory Factor Analysis Question Loading Across Ten Multiply Imputed Iterations (n = 343)

The first factor contained ten items that crossed the 0.40 factor loading threshold in at least two of the ten multiply imputed datasets. Seven of those ten items were from cognitive subscales (fantasy and perspective taking). This seven-item subscale had an intra-scale Cronbach’s α of 0.78 and represents the cognitive empathy subscale (Table 3). The second factor contained nine items that crossed the 0.40 threshold in at least two of the imputed datasets. Six of those nine items were from affective subscales (personal distress and empathetic concern) with an intra-scale Cronbach’s α of 0.73. These six items represent the affective empathy subscale (Table 3).

Table 3 Intra-Scale Correlation for the Cognitive and Affective Scales

Confirmatory factor analysis and measurement invariance

Following the two-factor solution found by the EFA, we sought to confirm this factor structure within partner dyads using dyadic CFA and invariance testing. We did this by imposing gradually increased levels of measurement invariance (Table 4). Results were aggregated across ten multiply imputed data sets. First, we found that the configural model (n = 396 dyads, 792 individuals) – where the constellation of items and latent construct was compared for male vs female partners – offered the following fit to the data: CFI = 0.903, TLI = 0.887, RMSEA = 0.059. Second, we found that the model constrained to metric invariance had a similar, but marginally (and unexpectedly, because more constrained models generally are not a better fit to the data) better fit than the baseline configural model (CFI = 0.914, TLI = 0.904, RMSEA = 0.056, ΔCFI = 0.011 > 0.002). Lastly, we found that the scalar model provided a satisfactory fit, but did not fit the data as well as the metric model and did not exhibit scalar invariance (CFI = 0.897, TLI = 0.903, RMSEA = 0.057, ΔCFI = 0.017 > 0.002) [46]. Therefore, we determined that the two-factor model exhibited metric, but not scalar, invariance. Factor correlations ranged from 0.03 to 0.31 across imputed datasets.

Table 4 Adjusted and Unadjusted Fit Indices for Dyadic Invariance Testing Confirmatory Factor Analysis

After EFA, application of the underlying IRI theoretical framework, and dyadic CFA, our finalized empathy instrument included 13 of the 28 original IRI items. The cognitive and affective subscales suggested by EFA and supported by dyadic CFA were moderately correlated within each participant (Pearson’s rho: 0.621, 95% CI: 0.585, 0.655) (Table 3).

Demographic comparisons

The exploratory and dyadic confirmatory factor analyses suggested a two-factor solution. To further probe the construct validity of this two-factor IRI model, we examined demographic and psychological differences on this adapted measure. These comparisons are confounded by the inability to establish scalar invariance (i.e., men and women interpreted the scaling of the items differently). Any statistically significant sex differences should therefore be interpreted with caution. We found unexpectedly lower mean scores on the cognitive subscale among women (Mean difference 95% CI: − 0.205, − 0.026) and those with a PHQ-9 score of 9 or lower (not depressed) at baseline (Mean difference 95% CI: − 0.352, − 0.120) and higher mean cognitive empathy score in participants 26 or younger (Mean difference 95% CI: 0.010, 0.196) (Table 5). We observed lower affective empathy scores among females (Mean difference 95% CI: − 0.186, − 0.0004) and those with more than 7 years of education (Mean difference 95% CI: − 0.247, − 0.024) and higher affective empathy scores in participants 26 or younger (Mean difference 95% CI: 0.010, 0.200) (Table 5). There is at least one district and one language that has a mean affective and cognitive empathy score that is statistically different from the others (Table 5). There was a low positive correlation (rho = 0.152, 95% CI: 0.093–0.211) between continuous PHQ-9 scores and cognitive empathy (Table 3).

Table 5 Demographic Comparisons of the Cognitive and Affective Scales

Discussion

Our exploratory factor analysis, consideration of the distinct cognitive and affective domains of empathy, and dyadic CFA suggested that a refined, shortened IRI scale is an acceptable measure of empathy in this population of adult partners living with HIV in Zambézia Province, Mozambique. Instead of the original four factor structure, exploratory factor analyses suggested, and the CFA with dyadic invariance testing confirmed, two empathy subscales: an affective subscale (with items assessing personal distress and empathetic concern) and a cognitive scale (with items assessing perspective taking and fantasy), which aligns with more recently developed empathy scales and recent data that describe the neurological processes underlying empathy [25,26,27,28,29]. The obtained subscales were moderately correlated with each other and had comparable intra-scale reliabilities among women and men.

This analysis takes an important initial step toward advancing our capacity to assess empathy changes associated with behavioral interventions in SSA, building upon insights from one province in Mozambique. This paper assesses empathy within romantic partners via a dyadic approach to more accurately capture the interdependence of this relational process. In turn, this yields a more precise assessment of partner empathy, which augments our capacity to assess how the HoPS+ intervention (in this case) or other couples-based interventions may change partner empathy, which itself may contribute to positive “downstream” treatment outcomes. For example, more empathic partners may better enable adherence to medication protocols, attending clinic care, and emotional support. This may also lead to interventions that target this important mediator of health intervention fidelity [13,14,15,16,17].

Our analysis established metric invariance, suggesting that the IRI items loaded similarly for male and female partners within the dyadic CFA analytic framework. Despite having a satisfactory fit, scalar invariance could not be established because of a statistically significant decreased fit from the metric invariance model (ΔCFI = 0.017 > 0.002). While the CFI and TLI fall below the commonly targeted thresholds of model fit proposed by Hu & Bentler [49], we feel the use of robust adjusted estimates, adoption of conservative ΔAFI thresholds, translation into four new languages, and application to an Mozambican sample with a median 5 years of formal education (as opposed to college-educated college-aged students) makes this fit a satisfactory introduction of this scale to a new population. More work can and should be done to further refine the measurement of cognitive and affective empathy in SSA.

Although our two-factor IRI had higher inter-scale correlations (0.62) within each participant than previous IRI or IRI for couple validations (range of 0.01–0.44), we had very similar intra-scale correlations and the dyadic CFA showed inter-scale correlations of 0.03–0.31 among partners [11, 19, 20, 30,31,32]. We cannot rule out that the higher intra-scale correlations are due to the new context for IRI questions.

The lack of scalar invariance across sex suggests that partners may exhibit different baseline values, limiting the degree to which mean differences (t-tests) can be interpreted across partners. In practice, this implies that partners with a latent empathy value of zero should not be expected to have the same average response on survey items. Thus, results from sex comparisons of empathy levels (e.g., men were observed to have higher levels of affective and cognitive empathy) ought to be considered with this in mind. Furthermore, similar empathy across both sexes aligns with our experience working with the communities in Zambézia Province. Higher empathy among younger individuals and higher empathy among those with more education aligns with previous research [55]. In contrast to a review that suggests higher affective empathy with depression – secondary to an increased focus on the self during a depressed state [58], we found no meaningful differences in affective empathy among patients with and without depression. However, our sample size is more than double that of all the reviewed studies combined and our participants are members of the community instead of mixed (hospitalized, on medications, etc.) and therefore are unlikely to act in the same way as those previously described [58].

Our study has several other limitations. Due to translation discrepancies, we excluded two items from the original IRI prior to starting our analyses. We excluded additional items when they loaded on the “incorrect” subscale (cognitive items on affective and vice versa), however, we feel it was reasonable to apply substantive considerations in this way, given the differences between this population and the populations on which the IRI was originally validated. Furthermore, having a more succinct scale will reduce missing responses when this scale is used in other settings. Additionally, about 11% of our participants were excluded from the analyses because they had more than two missing items per subscale. This resulted in a reduction of our sample size and statistical differences between the included and excluded participants.

Finally, the inclusion of four language groups in seven study districts resulted in substantial variability without a sufficient sample to conduct language- and/or district-specific analyses. Though we would have liked to have accounted for dyads within a language- or a district-nested model, the relatively low number of strata and sample sizes within some strata prevented these multilevel analyses. As our data collection continues, this may be a possibility for future research.

Conclusions

In conclusion, our findings provide support for a two-subscale version of the IRI that measures cognitive and affective empathy among HIV-positive adults living in Zambézia Province, Mozambique. Given the association between empathy and health intervention fidelity, this new scale should be useful in assessing the effectiveness of interventions designed to increase social support among couples, family, and groups. This scale validation will help us measure the effect of the HoPS+ intervention on male and female empathy within the context of partner dyads, which, in turn, is hypothesized to impact adherence to treatment, retention in care, and maternal-to-child HIV transmission. Furthermore, this dyadic approach provides inroads to assess how constructs such as empathy, stigma, physician trust, and social support (other constructs measured in HoPS+) are related to each other and positive treatment outcomes for PLWH. Future applications and validations will allow health researchers to develop interventions that target empathy within partner dyads as a mediator of health intervention uptake in HIV and other chronic health conditions. Subsequent scale validations/adaptations would benefit from testing this scale in different languages and in different regions in Mozambique and/or SSA.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

PLWH:

People living with HIV

SSA:

Sub-Saharan Africa

HoPS+:

Homens para Saúde Mais

IRI:

Interpersonal Reactivity Index

PHQ-9:

Patient Health Questionaire-9

EFA:

Exploratory Factor Analysis

CFA:

Confirmatory Factor Analysis

ΔAFI:

Changes in alternative fit indices

ΔCFI:

Changes in the confirmatory fit index

RMSEA:

Root Mean Squared Error of Approximation

CFI:

Comparative Fit Index

TLI:

Tucker Lewis Index

References

  1. (UNAIDS) JUNPoHA. Fact Sheet – July 2018. 2018.

    Google Scholar 

  2. IaM I. Inquérito de Indicadores de Imunização, Malária e HIV/SIDA em Moçambique (IMASIDA 2015), vol. 2017. Maputo: INS; 2017.

    Google Scholar 

  3. John GC, Nduati RW, Mbori-Ngacha DA, Richardson BA, Panteleeff D, Mwatha A, et al. Correlates of mother-to-child human immunodeficiency virus type 1 (HIV-1) transmission: association with maternal plasma HIV-1 RNA load, genital HIV-1 DNA shedding, and breast infections. J Infect Dis. 2001;183(2):206–12.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Audet CM, Blevins M, Chire YM, Aliyu MH, Vaz LM, Antonio E, et al. Engagement of men in antenatal care services: increased HIV testing and treatment uptake in a community participatory action program in Mozambique. AIDS Behav. 2016;20(9):2090–100.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. Audet CM, Graves E, Barreto E, De Schacht C, Gong W, Shepherd BE, et al. Partners-based HIV treatment for seroconcordant couples attending antenatal and postnatal care in rural Mozambique: A cluster randomized trial protocol. Contemp Clin Trials. 2018;71:63–9.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Audet CM, Graves E, Bravo M, Aliyu MH, Alvim F, Green AF, et al. Male engagement strategies effective in improving Option B+ retention in rural Mozambique. Paris: International AIDS Society; 2017. 2017.

    Google Scholar 

  7. Dunlap J, Foderingham N, Bussell S, Wester CW, Audet CM, Aliyu MH. Male involvement for the prevention of mother-to-child HIV transmission: A brief review of initiatives in east, west, and Central Africa. Curr HIV/AIDS Rep. 2014;11(2):109–18.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  8. Farquhar C, Kiarie JN, Richardson BA, Kabura MN, John FN, Nduati RW, et al. Antenatal couple counseling increases uptake of interventions to prevent HIV-1 transmission. J Acquir Immune Defic Syndr (1999). 2004;37(5):1620–6.

    Google Scholar 

  9. Ghanotakis E, Hoke T, Wilcher R, Field S, Mercer S, Bobrow EA, et al. Evaluation of a male engagement intervention to transform gender norms and improve family planning and HIV service uptake in Kabale, Uganda. Global Public Health. 2017;12(10):1297–314.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Peltzer K, Jones D, Weiss SM, Shikwane E. Promoting male involvement to improve PMTCT uptake and reduce antenatal HIV infection: a cluster randomized controlled trial protocol. BMC Public Health. 2011;11:778.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Davis MH. A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1980;10:85.

  12. Dvash J, Shamay-Tsoory SG. Theory of mind and empathy as multidimensional constructs neurological foundations. Top Lang Disord. 2014;34(4):282–95.

    Google Scholar 

  13. Leiberg S, Anders S. The multiple facets of empathy: a survey of theory and evidence. Prog Brain Res. 2006;156:419–40.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Derksen F, Bensing J, Lagro-Janssen A. Effectiveness of empathy in general practice: a systematic review. Br J Gen Pract. 2013;63(606):e76–84.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Flickinger TE, Saha S, Roter D, Korthuis PT, Sharp V, Cohn J, et al. Clinician empathy is associated with differences in patient-clinician communication behaviors and higher medication self-efficacy in HIV care. Patient Educ Couns. 2016;99(2):220–6.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Mercer SW, Higgins M, Bikker AM, Fitzpatrick B, McConnachie A, Lloyd SM, et al. General practitioners' empathy and health outcomes: A prospective observational study of consultations in areas of high and low deprivation. Ann Fam Med. 2016;14(2):117–24.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Lin C, Li L, Wan D, Wu Z, Yan Z. Empathy and avoidance in treating patients living with HIV/AIDS (PLWHA) among service providers in China. AIDS Care. 2012;24(11):1341–8.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Devoldre I, Davis MH, Verhofstadt LL, Buysse A. Empathy and social support provision in couples: social support and the need to study the underlying processes. J Psychol. 2010;144(3):259–84.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Peloquin K, Lafontaine MF. Measuring empathy in couples: validity and reliability of the interpersonal reactivity index for couples. J Pers Assess. 2010;92(2):146–57.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Levesque C, Lafontaine MF, Carona A, Flesch JL, Bjornson S. Dyadic Empathy, Dyadic Coping, and Relationship Satisfaction: A Dyadic Model. Europe’s J Psychologty. 2014;10(1):17.

    Google Scholar 

  21. Verhofstadt L, Devoldre I, Buysse A, Stevens M, Hinnekens C, Ickes W, et al. The role of cognitive and affective empathy in Spouses' support interactions: an observational study. PLoS One. 2016;11(2):e0149944.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. Harris PA, Taylor R, Thielke R, Payne J, Gonzalez N, Conde JG. Research Electronic Data Capture (REDCap)--a metadata-driven methodology and workflow process for providing translational research informatics support. J Biomed Inform. 2009; 42(2):377–81.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. USAID. Education Washington, DC 2019 [Available from: https://www.usaid.gov/mozambique/education.

    Google Scholar 

  24. The World Bank. Mozambique Economic Update: Less Poverty, but More Inequality 2018. Available from: https://www.worldbank.org/en/country/mozambique/publication/mozambique-economic-update-less-poverty-but-more-inequality.

    Google Scholar 

  25. Innamorati M, Ebisch SJH, Gallese V, Saggino A. A bidimensional measure of empathy: empathic experience scale. PLoS One. 2019;14(4):e0216164.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Batchelder L, Brosnan M, Ashwin C. The development and validation of the empathy components questionnaire (ECQ). PLoS One. 2017;12(1):e0169185.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Cox CL, Uddin LQ, Di Martino A, Castellanos FX, Milham MP, Kelly C. The balance between feeling and knowing: affective and cognitive empathy are reflected in the brain's intrinsic functional dynamics. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2012;7(6):727–37.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Reniers RL, Corcoran R, Drake R, Shryane NM, Vollm BA. The QCAE: a questionnaire of cognitive and affective empathy. J Pers Assess. 2011;93(1):84–95.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Shamay-Tsoory SG. The neural bases for empathy. Neuroscientist. 2011;17(1):18–24.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. De Corte K, Buysse A, Verhofstadt LL, Roeyers H, Ponnet K, Davis MH. Measuring empathic tendencies: reliability and validity of the Dutch version of the interpersonal reactivity index. Psychol Belg. 2007;47(4):235–60.

    Google Scholar 

  31. Fernandez AM, Dufey M, Kramp U. Testing the psychometric properties of the interpersonal reactivity index (IRI) in Chile empathy in a different cultural context. Eur J Psychol Assess. 2011;27(3):179–85.

    Google Scholar 

  32. Siu AMH, Shek DTL. Validation of the interpersonal reactivity index in a Chinese context. Res Social Work Prac. 2005;15(2):118–26.

    Google Scholar 

  33. American Psychiatric Association. DSM-5 Task Force. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders : DSM-5. 5th ed. Washington, D.C.: American Psychiatric Association; 2013. xliv. p. 947.

    Google Scholar 

  34. Cholera R, Gaynes BN, Pence BW, Bassett J, Qangule N, Macphail C, et al. Validity of the patient health questionnaire-9 to screen for depression in a high-HIV burden primary healthcare clinic in Johannesburg, South Africa. J Affect Disord. 2014;167:160–6.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Pence BW, Gaynes BN, Atashili J, O'Donnell JK, Tayong G, Kats D, et al. Validity of an interviewer-administered patient health questionnaire-9 to screen for depression in HIV-infected patients in Cameroon. J Affect Disord. 2012;143(1–3):208–13.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Monahan PO, Shacham E, Reece M, Kroenke K, Ong'or WO, Omollo O, et al. Validity/reliability of PHQ-9 and PHQ-2 depression scales among adults living with HIV/AIDS in Western Kenya. J Gen Intern Med. 2009;24(2):189–97.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Audet CM, Wainberg ML, Oquendo MA, Yu QR, Peratikos MB, Duarte CS, et al. Depression among female heads-of-household in rural Mozambique: A cross-sectional population-based survey. J Affect Disord. 2018;227:48–55.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. van Buuren S, Groothuis-Oudshoorn K. Mice: multivariate imputation by chained equations in R. J Stat Softw. 2011;45(3):1–67.

    Google Scholar 

  39. Barnfather N, Amod Z. Empathy and personal experiences of trainees in an emotional literacy and persona doll programme in South Africa. S Afr J Psychol. 2012;42(4):598–607.

    Google Scholar 

  40. MacRitchie V, Leibowitz S. Secondary traumatic stress, level of exposure, empathy and social support in trauma workers. S Afr J Psychol. 2010;40(2):149–58.

    Google Scholar 

  41. Team RC. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2018.

    Google Scholar 

  42. Henson RK, Roberts JK. Use of exploratory factor analysis in published research - common errors and some comment on improved practice. Educ Psychol Meas. 2006;66(3):393–416.

    Google Scholar 

  43. Costello AB, Osborne J. Best Practices in Exploratory Factor Analysis: Four Recommendations for Getting the Most From Your Analysis. Pract Assess Res Eval. 2005;10:7.

  44. Cook WL, Kenny DA. The actor-partner interdependence model: A model of bidirectional effects in developmental studies. Int J Behav Dev. 2005;29(2):101–9.

    Google Scholar 

  45. Rosseel Y. Lavaan: an R package for structural equation modeling. J Stat Softw. 2012;48(2):1–36.

    Google Scholar 

  46. Meade AW, Johnson EC, Braddy PW. Power and sensitivity of alternative fit indices in tests of measurement invariance. J Appl Psychol. 2008;93(3):568–92.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Brosseau-Liard PE, Savalei V. Adjusting incremental fit indices for nonnormality. Multivar Behav Res. 2014;49(5):460–70.

    Google Scholar 

  48. Brosseau-Liard PE, Savalei V, Li LB. An investigation of the sample performance of two nonnormality corrections for RMSEA. Multivar Behav Res. 2012;47(6):904–30.

    Google Scholar 

  49. Hu L-t, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct Equ Model. 1999;6(1):1–55.

    Google Scholar 

  50. Little TD. Longitudinal structural equation modeling, vol. xxii. New York: The Guilford Press; 2013. p. 386.

    Google Scholar 

  51. Putnick DL, Bornstein MH. Measurement invariance conventions and reporting: the state of the art and future directions for psychological research. Dev Rev. 2016;41:71–90.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  52. Sakaluk JK, Kilshaw R, Fisher AN, Leshner CE. Dyadic measurement invariance and its importance for replicability in romantic relationship research. preprint. PsyArXiv; 2019 2019/03/06/.

  53. Chen FF. Sensitivity of goodness of fit indexes to lack of measurement invariance. Struct Equ Model Multidiscip J. 2007;14(3):464–504.

    Google Scholar 

  54. Satorra A, Bentler PM. Corrections to test statistics and standard errors in covariance structure analysis. Latent variables analysis: Applications for developmental research. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications, Inc; 1994. p. 399–419.

    Google Scholar 

  55. Gruhn D, Rebucal K, Diehl M, Lumley M, Labouvie-Vief G. Empathy across the adult lifespan: longitudinal and experience-sampling findings. Emotion. 2008;8(6):753–65.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  56. Levy J, Goldstein A, Feldman R. The neural development of empathy is sensitive to caregiving and early trauma. Nat Commun. 2019;10(1):1905.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  57. O'Connor LE, Berry JW, Weiss J, Gilbert P. Guilt, fear, submission, and empathy in depression. J Affect Disord. 2002;71(1–3):19–27.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Schreiter S, Pijnenborg GHM, Aan Het Rot M. Empathy in adults with clinical or subclinical depressive symptoms. J Affect Disord. 2013;150(1):1–16.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Thoma P, Zalewski I, von Reventlow HG, Norra C, Juckel G, Daum I. Cognitive and affective empathy in depression linked to executive control. Psychiatry Res. 2011;189(3):373–8.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Tully EC, Ames AM, Garcia SE, Donohue MR. Quadratic associations between empathy and depression as moderated by emotion dysregulation. J Psychol. 2016;150(1):15–35.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the participants of the HoPS+ study for donating their time and effort to a novel couple-based intervention and our study counselors and peer advocates for providing support to participants in their effort to adhere to HIV care and treatment. We would also like to thank the three anonymous reviewers whose thoughtful comments improved the manuscript.

Funding

This work is supported by the National Institute of Mental Health grants R01MH113478. CMA is also supported by K01MH107255. Research reported in this publication was supported by NIGMS of the National Institutes of Health under award number T32GM007347. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

DES helped conceive the study, coordinated and conducted data analysis, and wrote and edited the manuscript. MBF substantially contributed to study design and data analysis, wrote portions of the manuscript, and edited the manuscript. MAD substantially contributed to study design and was influential in manuscript revisions. CD, EG, SVR, KAW, and AMK were involved in study design and manuscript revisions. AE, AM, and EB were involved in data collection and manuscript revisions. CMA helped conceive the study, coordinate data analysis, and was involved in substantive manuscript revisions. The author(s) read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Daniel E. Sack.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The HoPS+ study protocol was reviewed and approved by the Vanderbilt University Medical Center Institutional Review Board (IRB) (FWA00005756, IRB00000475–7, IRB00002125) and the Ministry of Health in Mozambique (FWA00003139 IRB00002657) as Non-Exempt Human Subjects Research. All participants provided written informed consent.

Consent for publication

This manuscript does not include any individual’s personal data.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary information

Additional file 1 Supplemental Table 1.

Original and Adapted Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI).

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Sack, D.E., Frisby, M.B., Diemer, M.A. et al. Interpersonal reactivity index adaptation among expectant seroconcordant couples with HIV in Zambézia Province, Mozambique. BMC Psychol 8, 90 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-020-00442-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-020-00442-0

Keywords