- Research
- Open access
- Published:
Inhibitory control and academic achievement – a study of the relationship between Stroop Effect and university students’ academic performance
BMC Psychology volume 12, Article number: 498 (2024)
Abstract
While previous research has identified executive functions as predictors of academic performance in school children, similar studies conducted among adults show mixed results. One of the reasons given for executive functions having a limited effect on academic achievements in adulthood is that they are usually fully developed by that time. Since these executive functions are at their peak at that age, the individual differences in these as well as their influence on academic performance in adults are harder to trace. The paper describes a study conducted among 107 university students the goal of which was to find out whether there is any relationship between the adult students’ inhibitory control values measured with the Stroop Test and their academic achievements. Although the results indicate a weak correlation between the Stroop Effect and the students’ academic performance of low statistical significance, which seems to confirm the outcomes of the previous studies focusing on adults, the study reveals an unexpected statistically significant correlation between the students’ grade averages and the number of their incorrect color identifications. This phenomenon appears to be worth pursuing in future research since it suggests the existence of another, relatively quickly measurable, variable possibly reflecting other predictors of academic performance in adults such as a degree of their manifested conscientiousness, their ability to concentrate on an assigned, relatively short, one-off task and their attitude to fulfilling this task. The Stroop Test, despite not being originally designed for this purpose, might thus be used as a simple tool suitable for providing information about these variables via the subject’s number of color identification errors. Such information can subsequently inform the activities that educators may include in their curricula to foster conscientiousness and concentration in the students lacking these.
Introduction
As documented by previous research, academic performance as the “level of knowledge demonstrated in an area or subject compared to the norm for the particular age and level of education” [46] has been affected by a myriad of factors. These are socioeconomic [66], student-related (e.g. students’ self-control and class attendance) [25, 29], or psychosocial [60]. Another factor documented as affecting academic performance is a complex of executive functions which appear to play a significant role in language development as well as the processing and organization of received information [57]. The processing of received information is done either through automatic attention or controlled attention. In the case of the former, the attention responses direct attention automatically to a target regardless of concurrent inputs or memory load. In the case of the latter, an active attention of the subject is required, which also makes the information processing limited in terms of processing capacity [63].
Executive functions encompass cognitive skills related to attention control, i.e. the process by which attention is selectively directed to specific aspects of a representation, particularly in misleading situations [11]. One of the attention control mechanisms can be switching attention between tasks where, in the case of a card-sort test, for instance, the subject must switch between different rules by which they sort cards (e.g. first by their shape and then by their color). Another mechanism is the inhibition of attention when it comes to the stimuli that need to be ignored. This inhibition is activated in, for instance, multilinguals when they need to suppress their temptation to use one (or more) of their languages not needed or inappropriate for a given situation [11, 13, 77, etc.]. Other researchers [2, 53, etc.] work with the terms of inhibition (the ability to suppress dominant responses, a term synonymous to the attention inhibition mentioned above), shifting (the ability to switch over between tasks, a term synonymous to switching attention mentioned above) and monitoring (the ability to update information in the working memory). The working memory as a function that enables individuals to temporarily remember information while competitively processing information [54] has been mentioned as a factor influencing school performance even more than intelligence with the latter predicting a wide range of indicators of academic success [48, 52].
Effect of executive functions on academic performance
Multiple studies emphasize the fact that educational research should pay more attention to executive functions since these represent essential ingredients for successful academic functioning [75] and since they also appear to be connected with school dysfunction as deficits in them have been associated with disabilities in mathematics and reading [51, 70]. According to Pascual [57], who also mentions cognitive flexibility, i.e. the ability to temporarily manipulate information, and planning, the executive functions represent “distinct, but related, higher-order neurocognitive processes that control thought and behaviors aimed at achieving an objective goal” (p. 2). The existence of relationship between executive functions and academic achievement is also supported by other studies most of which investigate this relationship in children of pre-school or early-school age (e.g. [4, 9, 19, 28, 76, etc.]) or those that study it in the context of learning disabilities [3, 37, 49, 64].
Some studies also stress the fact that the positive contribution of executive functions to academic performance is domain-dependent, i.e. that certain executive functions contribute to gaining certain knowledge or skill more than others. Thus, inhibition, for instance, appears to be beneficial when it comes to mathematics and science [57]. Similarly, Gerst [38] found a direct relationship between inhibition and the ability to conduct mathematical calculations in children aged 10–11.
As there seems to be a variation in the way in which younger and older children solve calculations due to the age-related shift from the procedural-based processing in arithmetic tasks to more memory-based [72, 7, 16, etc.] it is also different types of interference that appear to disrupt children at different ages. In a dual-task study McKenzie et al. [50], for instance, found out that the mathematical processing of 6-year-old children was disrupted only by a visuo-spatial passive interference task whereas in the 8-year-old ones it was disrupted by both a visuo-spatial and a phonological interference task. In this respect, the type of information processing deployed in problem solving appears to determine the type of irrelevant stimuli that need to be suppressed through inhibition for the students to complete an arithmetic task efficiently.
The executive function of inhibition is usually defined as the ability to suppress dominant but irrelevant responses and prioritize important information instead. This way “it moderates behavior, suppresses impulsive reactions to a stimulus, and enables an appropriate and thoughtful response” [57]. Cognitive inhibition is thus responsible for planning, analyzing and choosing the most appropriate response.
Negative effects of a low degree of inhibitory control on academic performance
The relationship between poor academic performance and poor performance in tasks requiring the inhibition of irrelevant information has been pointed out by multiple studies. Espy et al. [33], investigating how working memory and inhibitory control affect arithmetic competency, identified differences in the ability to inhibit irrelevant stimuli as a factor responsible for unique variance in mathematical skills. In mathematics, inhibitory control is used to inhibit information that should not be maintained in working memory for upcoming responding [27]. Similarly, Espy et al. point out that to flexibly shift responding in the face of conflicting rules requires maintaining the rule in mind and inhibiting prepotent, previous responses [33]. Passolunghi & Lanfranchi [55] mention inhibition as a factor influencing performance at the numerical competence test.
The importance of the role that inhibition plays in reading and listening comprehension has been pointed out by studies focusing on children. Passolunghi et al. [56], for instance, stress that groups of poor problem solvers tend to perform poorly in a working memory test requiring inhibition of irrelevant information and that this condition appears to be related to poor recall of critical information and greater recall of to-be-inhibited information. In addition, the process of reading often involves exposure to visual distractions such as images, graphs, etc. present in the very text as well as external physical distractors in the environment in which the reading takes place. In such situations, the inhibitory control helps the reader to stay focused on the written content. Similarly, De Beni et al. [26], showed that the “poor comprehenders” had a significantly lower performance in the listening span test associated with a higher number of intrusions. These intrusions can be background noise or competing sounds that need to be ignored for the listener to focus on and understand what a speaker is saying. In addition, both reading and listening often involve interpretation of figurative language, where the inhibition of the literal, irrelevant information enables the reader or listener to grasp the relevant meaning [39].
Inhibitory control as a facilitator of learning
The positive correlation between a degree of inhibitory control and academic achievement has been documented by other studies as well. Duckworth et al. [29], for instance, stress behavioral inhibition (self-control) as affecting academic performance. St Clair-Thompson & Gathercole [62] identify inhibition as a factor associated with achievement in English, mathematics, and science in 11- and 12-year-old children while Blair & Razza [14] point out that the inhibitory control correlated with both early math and reading ability in their study conducted among 3- to 5-year-old children. Privitera et al. [59] give a reason for why the improved inhibitory control leads to greater academic performance; the students with improved inhibitory control can focus on tasks both within and outside of the classroom better, ignoring the ever-growing number of distractions present in their environments. The authors also claim that this improved focus may result in superior academic performance. Irvan & Tsapali [44] point out the positive effect of improved inhibitory control on academic performance stating that the inhibition as an executive function appears particularly crucial for young children growing up and learning as they are exposed to constant distractions vying for their attention. Blair & Razza [14] also suggest that curricula designed to improve self-regulation skills and enhance early academic abilities may be most effective in helping children succeed in school.
Age-dependent effect of executive functions on academic performance
On the other hand, some sources conclude that the relationship between executive functions, with inhibitory control representing one of them, and academic performance appears to depend on age. Bryce et al. [15] focused on the relationship between executive functions and metacognitive skills, which they have identified as most significant predictors of educational achievements in their study groups of 5- and 7-year-old children. Their results indicate that executive functions appear to be more related to metacognitive skills in 5-year-olds than in 7-year-olds. In the study conducted among subjects aged 5–17, Best et al. [10] analyzed a varying correlation between executive functions and academic achievement in relation to age concluding that the correlation is strongest at the ages of 6, 8–9 and subsequently appears to be of somewhat consistent strength in the late childhood and adolescence. This conclusion partly contradicts the findings of Altemeier et al. [1], who claim that the effect of executive functions on academic performance may be more evident earlier in schooling, when academic skills are less automatic and require more effortful planning to execute. Similarly, some other studies point out inhibition as the strongest predictor of academic success at children’s early age such as that by Senn et al. [61]. They found out that while working memory contributed to academic success to a greater extent in older children, the inhibitory control did this in younger ones. Other authors [8, 42, etc.] mention the complete maturation of inhibitory processes by around the age of 12. The decrease in the potential of executive functions to predict academic performance during secondary education and even more so during university studies has also been touched upon by Pascual & Robres [57].
Methods used to measure the inhibitory control and the role of anterior cingulate
Scientists have developed several methods to measure inhibitory control, whose choice partly depends on the type of inhibition that is being targeted. Response inhibition, a term referring to the process of countermanding a prepotent motor response, has generally been assessed using non-selective stopping tasks such as the stop signal, go/no-go, and anti-saccade tasks. These tests require participants to intermittently suppress a motor response to a given presentation of a conditional stimulus or cue [6, 20, 71, 73]. Attentional inhibition, which refers to the ability to resist interference from stimuli in the external environment, has been investigated using visual matching tasks requiring participants to judge whether target and comparison stimuli are the same or different and, at the same time, requiring them to ignore task-irrelevant distracters [36, 68, 71].
Response inhibition and attentional inhibition are also commonly measured with Stimulus-Response Compatibility tasks, such as the Eriksen Flanker (Flanker), Simon, and Stroop tasks [32, 65, 69, 71]. The Stroop task (test) is utilized for comparing reaction times to stimuli in the condition where this control is not deployed (congruent condition) with the reaction times requiring inhibiting irrelevant stimuli (incongruent condition). The test has been shown to activate either the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex or anterior cingulate for cognitive inhibition [74]. As Imbrosciano & Berlach [43] point out, anterior cingulate is considered to be responsible for selecting an appropriate response when the brain is exposed to two conflicting conditions. Bush et al. [17] hypothesize that anterior cingulate dysfunction is responsible for producing core features of ADHD, namely inattention and impulsivity. Anterior cingulate activation has been linked to detection of conflict and its resolution [18] as well as to academic results in college students [40]. The last-named researchers investigated the activity of anterior cingulate in connection with the error-related negativity (ERN, an electrophysiological signal associated with the anterior cingulate monitoring process, occurring approximately 100 ms after an error is made) and found a correlation between the magnitude of ERN and undergraduate students’ academic performance suggesting that the error detection mechanism is stronger in the students who perform better at university. Veroude et al. [74] observed a positive correlation between average course grades and the activation of anterior cingulate cortex in freshmen enrolled in a medical college during cognitive inhibition on the Stroop task finding no relationship between the course grades and activation of the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Similarly, there are other studies which suggest a link between inhibitory control and academic performance associating the activation of anterior cingulate with cognitive control across tasks (e.g [31, 34]).
The aim of the study
The aim of the study described in this paper was to find out whether there is any relationship between adult students’ inhibitory control measured with the Stroop Test and their academic achievement. To measure the degree of inhibitory control, a computerized version of the Stroop Test was used.
Based on the previous research (e.g. [29, 40, 55, 57, 74] the initial hypothesis was that there might be a relationship between inhibitory control and academic performance. In this respect, the participants with a higher degree of inhibitory control (lower Stroop Effect) were expected to be those with a higher grade average and lower failure rate than those indicating a lower degree of inhibitory control (higher Stroop Effect). On the other hand, if the correlation between the two variables were to be found, it was not expected to be significantly high since the previous research studying this phenomenon in relation to age points to the inhibitory control exerting its influence on academic performance chiefly at an individual’s young age [1, 4, 9, 19, 28, 76].
Method
Participants
107 students studying at (undisclosed) University, Stockholm, Sweden, (29 males, 78 females, mean age = 25.83 years, SD age = 6.32 years, age range = 19–52 years) participated in the study. Originally, 110 students were involved in the study, but 3 of them were removed as outliers due to the overwhelming majority of their grades being at the extreme ends of the grading scale, i.e. either VGs or Us, and only few Gs (for more information on the grading scale, see the next section). This was done to exclude the students whose extraordinary performance in certain academic subjects might be due to either their extra talents for, or their exceptional motivation to study, these subjects. The participants were recruited from students each of whom was enrolled in one of three teacher education programs, i.e. either primary (N = 21), secondary (N = 51), or upper secondary (N = 35). The reason why the participants were recruited from this group was that most of the courses they study within these programs are somewhat similar in terms of contents. Besides, the students are also assessed in these courses mainly by the same teachers. The original idea was to recruit the highest number of volunteers enrolled in the three programs who were, at the same time, studying the courses given by the department in which the study was conducted. Nevertheless, the final number of the participants was determined by their willingness to participate in the study and it was also restricted by the fact that all of them had to be tested on campus in a computer laboratory within the limited time of the project. The volunteers had no neurological or psychiatric disorders. All the participants signed an informed consent with their participation in the study.
Data collection and analysis
Information about the participants’ age and sex was collected via questionnaires distributed among the participants prior to the execution of the Stroop Test. The students’ university grade averages were computed based on their past course grades and their calculation included a computational model (see below) used in another study [30] researching the effect of mother tongue proficiency on the students’ academic performance.
The study was conducted in an institution using the grading scale consisting of three grades, i.e. VG, G, and U, a system commonly used in Swedish universities. According to this system, VG represents “passed with distinction,” G denotes “passed,” and U denotes “failed.” To facilitate a statistical analysis, these grades were assigned numerical values of 4, 2, and 0, respectively. This approach mirrors the GPA calculation method, where the highest grade corresponds to 4, the middle one to 2, and the fail grade to 0. Each student’s failure rate, expressed as a percentage, was computed as the ratio of their fail grades (Us) to the total number of grades received. Approximately 30 grades, encompassing both courses and graded modules, were considered for the computation of grade averages and failure rates per student. In instances where a student received multiple fail grades for the same course or module, each of these was included as a distinct grade in the calculation.
To measure the participants’ inhibitory control, a computerized version of the Stroop Test available at https://www.psytoolkit.org/ was used. The task was performed in English since the students represent a relatively uniform group when it comes to their English knowledge, which is at the C1 level of Common European Framework of Reference for languages. Moreover, English represents the language that all the participants have studied in a language instructional setting and thus the color identification rule in the Stroop test had to be followed in the context of their knowledge previously adopted at school. In this respect, the experiment made the participants deploy controlled information processing [63] through the application of a new cognitive concept requiring the inhibition of the semantic contents they have learnt at school before. This way an attempt was made at inducing the situation activating those cognitive processes that resemble the ones which are in operation in school environments when new concepts are learnt or when adjustments are made to the already acquired knowledge.
The task consisted of two conditions on which the participants were tested: (a) congruent trials, where the names of colors displayed on the screen matched the colors these were displayed in, (b) incongruent trails, where the names of colors displayed on the screen did not match the colors these were displayed in. For each trial type the students were instructed to identify the color of the word as quickly as possible by pressing a corresponding key on their keyboards. The keys the subjects were instructed to press were those that bore the initial letters of the names of the colors the words were displayed in. Therefore, when the word “red”, for instance, got displayed in blue, the students were supposed to press the b key (“b” standing for “blue”). The explicit instruction given to the students was to disregard the meaning of the words and focus solely on the color in which these words were displayed.
There were four colors used in the test (red, yellow, blue and green) and the students were instructed to press the r, y, b and g keys, respectively, to indicate these. Before each of the words was presented in the middle of the screen against the black background (for up to 2 s or until the participant responded), a fixation cross was displayed in the same position for 200 milliseconds for the participant to know where the word would appear. Once the participant made their choice, either a word “correct” or “wrong” popped up for 500 milliseconds depending on whether their choice had been correct or not. The computer script in which the test was run measured the participants’ reaction times in both the congruent and incongruent conditions and counted the errors they made when indicating a wrong color. The Stroop test was run under these conditions twice – once as a practice session with thirty trials, whose purpose was to make sure all the participants understood what they were supposed to do as well as to enable them to practice the key-color associations, and then as the test itself with 120 test trials. Half of the test trials were in the congruent condition and the other half in the incongruent one. The congruent and incongruent conditions were mixed and presented to the subjects randomly.
The main Stroop effect size was calculated for the individual participants according to the following formula using the reaction times recorded for congruent as well as incongruent trials:
The reaction times used in the formula above were collected together with the numbers of incorrect color identifications from files saved on a server once the tests had been completed.
Subsequently, Pearson’s bivariate correlation analysis was conducted on the collected data to find out the correlation coefficients between the participants’ grade averages (as well as failure rates) and their Stroop Effect values. Similar analyses were also done for their reaction times in congruent and incongruent conditions as well as their number of Stroop Test mistakes, i.e. the situations where a color was not correctly identified.
As the study group the analysis was conducted with consisted of three sub-groups of students (each sub-group consisting of students enrolled in one of the three teacher training programs), One-Way ANOVA was used to compare these sub-groups for Stroop Effect, reaction times for congruent trials, reaction times for incongruent trials, and number of color identification errors. Since the sub-groups were of unequal sizes, the test of homogeneity of variances was run on all the variables with the subsequent Tukey HSD post hoc test to identify the significance of the differences between the sub-groups.
Results
The students’ (N = 107) mean Stroop Effect, mean reaction times (in ms) in congruent and incongruent conditions, the mean number of errors as well as the mean number of times the participants exceeded the time limit are given in Table 1 below. The table also lists students’ grade average and fail rate average as well as standard deviations for each variable.
Pearson’s bivariate correlation analysis shows a very weak negative correlation of low statistical significance, r(107) = − 0.13, p = .20, between the Stroop Effect and participants’ grade averages and basically no correlation between the former and participants’ fail rate, r(107) = 0.04, p = .68, (see Table 2 below). Similarly, there seems to be no statistically significant relationship between the participants’ grade averages and their reaction times in congruent (r(107) = − 0.06, p = .58) and incongruent (r(107) = − 0.10, p = .31) conditions as well as no significant relationship between the grade averages and the number of times the participants exceeded the time limit for color identification (r(107) = − 0.07, p = .46). The only statistically significant relationship detected appears to be the one between the students’ grade averages and the number of mistakes (incorrect color identifications) made during the Stroop Test with a weak negative correlation, r(107) = − 0.21, p = .03. Scatter plots for Stroop Effect values and the number of errors made during the test in relation to the students’ grades are shown in Figs. 1 and 2 below. The diagonal lines in the scatter plots represent identity lines indicating the points in which the values of the variables correlate perfectly while the distances of the data points from these lines represent degrees of correlation; the closer the points are to the line, the stronger the correlation between the variables. Tables 3 and 4 show the comparison of measured variables across the educational programs and related post hoc Tukey HSD, respectively.
The Tukey HSD post hoc test (see Table 4 below) shows the comparison of the distribution of the values within the sub-groups, where the only statistically significant Stroop Effect mean difference of 74.65 ms (p = .002) can be found between the primary and upper-secondary programs (the students of the latter indicating a lower mean).
Discussion
The goal of this study was to find out whether there was some relationship between the inhibitory control of the students studying at (undisclosed) University, Stockholm, Sweden, measured with the Stroop Test and their academic performance. Based on the previous research focusing on links between school performance and executive functions [51, 70, 75], the hypothesis was that the participants with a higher grade average and lower fail rate might tend to manifest a higher degree of inhibitory control indicated with a lower Stroop Effect. Nevertheless, this correlation was expected to be weak in the study group consisting of university students since the other research into the area shows the strongest correlation between the inhibitory control and the academic performance in subjects at their early age [8, 10, 15, 42, 61].
The results show that although there is a very weak negative correlation (r(107) = − 0.13) between the Stroop Effect and participants’ grade averages, which might suggest some effect of the degree of their inhibitory control on their school performance that is in line with the previous research focusing on this phenomenon, this relationship has not turned out to be statistically significant (p = .20). As regards the students’ failure rates, these turned out to be completely independent of the Stroop Effect values.
As regards the comparison of the distribution of all the observed values within the different sub-groups of students depending on what program they study, the only statistically significant difference was found in Stroop Effect (mean difference of 74.65 ms (p = .002)) between the primary and upper-secondary programs, with the latter indicating a lower Stroop effect.
Another statistically significant relationship detected was the one between the students’ grade averages and the number of mistakes (incorrect color identifications) made during the Stroop Test with a weak negative correlation (r(107) = − 0.21, p = .03) indicating that the students performing worse academically (having lower grades) made more mistakes during the test. This relationship may suggest that the degree of conscientiousness the students approach the assigned task of Stroop Test with might be in direct proportion to the degree of conscientiousness they approach their university studies with in general. That is [41], point out that conscientiousness predicts better performance on the Stroop task in terms of fewer errors and diminished incongruency effects. They even suggest that this personality trait may promote certain attentional processes even as cognitive capacities decline at a later age. Similarly, other studies [45, 67], deploying other attention control tasks, also found a relationship between conscientiousness and cognitive performance. Finally, as the myriad of studies shows conscientiousness, defined as dependability and will to achieve, as being in direct relationship with academic performance as well [21, 23, 24, 58, etc.], the results of the study described in this paper might be indicative of the newly found relationship between the number of color identification errors (as a factor reflecting this conscientiousness) and academic performance, albeit this relationship has not been studied before.
One of the reasons why the weak correlation between the students’ Stroop Effect and their school performance shows low statistical significance may be that the grade averages had been calculated from the grades the students obtained for a wide range of school subjects ranging from mathematics, to languages, history, etc. That is, the meta-analysis conducted by Pascual & Robres [57] shows that the degree to which executive functions affect school performance depends on the subject studied. This phenomenon can be observed, for example, in the relationship between mathematics and the visuo-spatial aspect of working memory. Similar observations have also been made when it comes to other executive functions, which appear to be more related to performance in mathematics than in a language, for instance. Moreover, the meta-analysis points out that most studies identify working memory as a better predictor of school performance than inhibition and that executive functions represent an important predictor of academic performance and future learning problems at an early age. However, the predictive capacity of executive functions in relation to academic performance seems to decrease during secondary education and even more so during university education, which might be the case with the study described in this article. The reason for this phenomenon could be minimal individual differences in executive functioning in certain age groups. Bialystok [12], for instance, in her study of the Stroop task performance, mentions no differences in Stroop Effect among university undergraduates giving the cognitive performance in this age group being at its peak as a reason for the phenomenon. Likewise, Comalli et al. [22] demonstrated that older adults and children indicate longer response latencies than young adults. The aforementioned factors are also suspected of being the reasons why no correlation has been found between the students’ Stroop Effect and their failure rates.
The fact that the relationship between the students’ Stroop Effect and school performance shows low statistical significance might also be due to the Stroop Test activating different regions of the brain in different individuals – operations that have been documented as correlating with school grades. That is, Veroude et al. [74] report a significant main effect of cognitive inhibition being observed in the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, but not so much in the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (ACC). However, they report the activation of ACC for the “incongruent” condition being associated with higher grades. In this respect, they found an association with achievement only in the situations where the Stroop Test activated dorsal ACC, indicating that “involvement of this region can potentially predict differences in education success.” (p. 104). Other studies also show the involvement of both the ACC and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex during the Stroop task (e.g [47]). even though the ACC does not seem to be necessary for cognitive control as patients with damage to this region perform normally on the Stroop Test [35]. As the study described in this paper did not include functional magnetic resonance imaging, it was not possible to find out in which situations the inhibitory control in the subjects in incongruent conditions resulted from the activation of the ACC and in which situations it resulted from the activation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Hence, it is also impossible to assess the extent to which the activation of the former or the latter for inhibitory control might influence the subjects’ grades. Not distinguishing between these two conditions could thus have been one of the reasons for the weak p-value of the results and thus it might be desirable to differentiate between them in future research.
Finally, as the current study suggests a link between the number of mistakes made during the Stroop Test and the students’ grade averages, the potential of the test to be used to measure the degree of their manifested conscientiousness and the ability to concentrate on an assigned, relatively short, one-off task should be studied further. The results of such further testing might provide clues regarding to what extent these characteristics can be viewed as predictors of academic performance.
Conclusion
Overall, this study has shown that there was a weak correlation of low statistical significance between the participants’ grade averages and the inhibitory control measured with the Stroop Test. It has also shown no relationship between their failure rates and inhibitory control.
These findings suggest that differences in the impact of inhibitory control on cognitive functioning among young adults might be much smaller, if any, than in children or older people. This fact seems to be in line with the findings of previous studies which point out that individual differences in executive functions are greatest while these functions are either under development, i.e. in children, or when they are in decline, i.e. in the elderly.
The study has also revealed that the students with lower grades made more color identification errors than those with higher grades. This phenomenon is worth pursuing in the future since the Stroop Test, or any other test where subjects need to follow a relatively simple rule, might be indicative (via their error rates) of their conscientiousness, a way in which they approach a certain assigned task or a degree of their ability to handle the task. Consequently, these findings can offer educators insights into their students’ specific weaknesses in these domains, empowering them to address these areas through tailored teaching approaches, such as individualized activities.
Data availability
The data pertinent to this study and used in the analysis are enclosed in a separate file uploaded at the submission of the paper.
References
Altemeier L, Jones J, Abbott RD, Berninger VW. Executive functions in becoming writing readers and reading writers: note taking and report writing in third and fifth graders. Dev Neuropsychol. 2006;29:161–73.
Antón E, García YF, Carreiras M, Dunabeitia JA. Does bilingualism shape inhibitory control in the elderly? J Mem Lang. 2016;90:147–60.
Alloway T. Working memory, but not IQ, predicts subsequent learning in children with learning difficulties. Eur J Psychol Assess. 2009;25(2):92–8.
Alloway T, Bibile V, Lau G. Computerized working memory training: can it lead to gains in cognitive skills in students? Comput Hum Behav. 2013;29(3):632–8.
Anderson P. Assessment and development of executive function (EF) during childhood. Child Neuropsychol. 2002;8:71–82.
Aron AR, Robbins TW, Poldrack RA. Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex: one decade on. Trends Cogn Sci. 2014;18:177–85.
Barrouillet P, Le´pine R. Working memory and children’s use of retrieval to solve addition problems. J Exp Child Psychol. 2005;91(3):183–204.
Bédard AC, Nichols S, Barbosa JA, Schachar R, Logan GD, Tannock R. The development of selective inhibitory control across the life span. Dev Neuropsychol. 2002;21:93–111.
Berg D. Working memory and arithmetic calculation in children: the contributory roles of processing speed, short-term memory, and reading. J Exp Child Psychol. 2008;99:288–308.
Best JR, Miller PH, Naglieri JA. Relations between executive function and academic achievement from ages 5 to 17 in a large, representative national sample. Learn Individ Differ. 2011;21:327–36.
Bialystok E, Martin MM. Attention and inhibition in bilingual children: evidence from the dimensional change card sort task. Dev Sci. 2004;7(3):325–39.
Bialystok E, Martin M, Viswanathan M. Bilingualism across the lifespan: the rise and fall of inhibitory control. Int J Biling. 2005;9(1):103–19.
Bialystok E, Craik FIM, Luk G. Cognitive control and lexical access in younger and older bilinguals. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2008;34(4):859–73.
Blair C, Razza RP. Relating effortful control, executive function, and false belief understanding to emerging math and literacy ability in kindergarten. Child Dev. 2007;78:647–63.
Bryce D, Whitebread D, Szucs D. The relationships among executive functions, metacognitive skills, and educational achievement in 5 and 7-year-old children. Metacognition Learn. 2015;10:181–98.
Bull R, Espy K. Working memory, executive functioning, and children’s mathematics. Educ Psychol. 2006;93–123.
Bush G, Frazier JA, Rauch SL, Seidman LJ, Whalen PJ, Jenike MA, Rosen BR, Biederman J. Anterior cingulate cortex dysfunction in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder revealed by MRI and the counting Stroop. Biol Psychiatry. 1999;45(12):1542–52.
Carter CS, Van Veen V. ACC and conflict detection: an update of theory and data. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2007;7(4):367–79.
Cartwright K. Insights from cognitive neuroscience: the importance of executive function for early reading development and education. Early Educ Dev. 2012;23(1):24–36.
Chambers CD, Garavan H, Bellgrove MA. Insights into the neural basis of response inhibition from cognitive and clinical neuroscience. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2009;33:631–46.
Chamorro-Premuzic T. Creativity versus conscientiousness: which is a better predictor of student performance? Appl Cogn Psychol. 2006;20(4):521–31.
Comalli PE Jr, Wapner S, Werner H. Interference effects of Stroop color-word test in childhood, adulthood, and aging. J Genet Psychol. 1962;100:47–53.
Conrad N, Patry MW. Conscientiousness and academic performance: a mediational analysis. Int J Scholarsh Teach Learn. 2012;6(1).
Costa PT Jr. Revised NEO personality inventory and NEO five-factor inventory. Prof Man.
Crede M, Roch SG, Kieszczynka UM. Class attendance in college: a meta-analytic review of the relationship of class attendance with grades and student characteristics. Rev Educ Res. 2010;80(2):272–95.
De Beni R, Palladino P, Pazzaglia F, Cornoldi C. Increases in intrusion errors and working memory deficit of poor comprehenders. Q J Exp Psychol. 1998;51:305–20.
Diamond A. Development of the ability to use recall to guide action, as indicated by infants’ performance on AB. Child Dev. 1985;56:868–83.
Diamond A. Activities and programs that improve children’s executive functions. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2012;21(5):335–41.
Duckworth AL, Taxer JL, Eskreis-Winkler L, Galla BM, Gross JJ. Self-control and academic achievement. Annu Rev Psychol. 2019;70:373–99.
Dvorak M. The varying relationship between perceived oral and written mother tongue proficiency and academic performance in native multilingual students at their secondary school and university. In: 13th International Conference The Future of Education: 2023; Bologna, Italy: Bologna: Pixel International Conferences; 2023;24–27.
Egner T, Etkin A, Gale S, Hirsch J. Dissociable neural systems resolve conflict from emotional versus nonemotional distractors. Cereb Cortex. 2007;18:1475–84.
Eriksen BA, Eriksen CW. Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Attent Percept Psychophys. 1974;16:143–9.
Espy KA, McDiarmid MM, Cwik MF, Stalets MM, Hamby A, Senn TE. The contribution of executive functions to emergent mathematical skills in preschool children. Dev Neuropsychol. 2004;26:465–86.
Evers EA, Van der Veen FM, Van Deursen JA, Schmitt JA, Deutz NEP, Jolles J. The effect of acute tryptophan depletion on the BOLD response during performance monitoring and response inhibition in healthy male volunteers. Psychopharmacology. 2006;187:200–8.
Fellows LK, Farah MJ. Is anterior cingulate cortex necessary for cognitive control? Brain. 2005;128(4):788–96.
Friedman NP, Miyake A. The relations among inhibition and interference control functions: a latent-variable analysis. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2004;133:101–35.
Gathercole S, Alloway T, Willis C, Adams A. Working memory in children with reading disabilities. J Exp Child Psychol. 2006;93(3):265–81.
Gerst EH, Cirino PT, Fletcher JM, Yoshida H. Cognitive and behavioral rating measures of executive function as predictors of academic outcomes in children. Child Neuropsychol. 2017;23:381–407.
Glucksberg S, Newsome MR, Goldvarg Y. Inhibition of the literal: filtering Metaphor-Irrelevant Information during Metaphor Comprehension. Metaphor Symbol. 2001;16(3–4):277–98.
Hirsh JB, Inzlicht M. Error-related negativity predicts academic performance. Psychophysiology. 2010;47:192–6.
Huff MJ, Gretz MR, Keefer LA. Conscientiousness predicts performance on the Stroop task but not other attentional control tasks in older and younger adults. Imagin Cogn Pers. 2023;43(2):150–68.
Huizinga M, van der Molen MW. Age-group differences in set-switching and set-maintenance on the Wisconsin Card sorting Task. Dev Neuropsychol. 2007;31:193–215.
Imbrosciano A, Berlach RG. The Stroop test and its relationship to academic performance and general behaviour of young students. Teach Dev. 2005;9:1.
Irvan R, Tsapali M. The role of Inhibitory Control in Achievement in Early Childhood Education. Camb Educational Res E-J. 2020;7:168–90.
Jackson JD, Balota DA. Mind-wandering in younger and older adults: converging evidence from the sustained attention to Response Task and reading for comprehension. Psychol Aging. 2012;27(1):106–19.
Jiménez M. Competencia social: intervención preventiva en la escuela [Social competence: preventive intervention at school]. Infanc Soc. 2000;24:21–48.
Laird AR, McMillan KM, Lancaster JL, Kochunov P, Turkeltaub PE, Pardo JV, Fox PT. A comparison of label-based review and ALE meta-analysis in the Stroop task. Hum Brain Mapp. 2005;25:6–21.
Lee K, Lee Pe M, Ang SJ, Stankov L. Do measures of working memory predict academic proficiency better than measures of intelligence? Psychol Sci. 2009;51(4):403–19.
Locascio G, Mahone EM, Eason SH, Cutting LE. Executive dysfunction among children with reading comprehension deficits. J Learn Disabil. 2010;43(5):44.
McKenzie B, Bull R, Gray C. The effects of phonological and visual-spatial interference on children’s arithmetical performance. Educ Child Psychol. 2003;20(3):93–108.
McLean JF, Hitch GJ. Working memory impairments in children with specific arithmetic learning difficulties. J Exp Child Psychol. 1999;74:240–60.
Mellanby J, Martin M, O’Doherty J. The ‘gender gap’ in final examination results at Oxford University. Br J Psychol. 2000;91(3):377–90.
Miyake A, Friedman NP. The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: four general conclusions. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2012;21(1):8–14.
Miyake A, Shah P. Models of working memory: mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control. Cambridge: University; 1999.
Passolunghi MC, Lanfranchi S. Domain-specific and domain-general precursors of mathematical achievement: a longitudinal study from kindergarten to first grade. Br J Educ Psychol. 2012;82:42–63.
Passolunghi MC, Cornoldi C, De Liberto S. Working memory and intrusions of irrelevant information in a group of specific poor problem solvers. Mem Cogn. 1999;27:779–90.
Pascual CA, Muñoz MN, Robres QA. The relationship between executive functions and academic performance in primary education: review and meta-analysis. Front Psychol. 2019;10:1582.
Poropat AE. A meta-analysis of the five-factor model of personality and academic performance. Psychol Bull. 2009;135(2):322–38.
Privitera AJ, Zhou Y, Xie X. Inhibitory control as a significant predictor of academic performance in Chinese high schoolers. Child Neuropsychol. 2023;29(3):457–73.
Robbins SB, Lauver K, Le H, Davis D, Langley R, Carlstrom A. Do psychosocial and study skill factors predict college outcomes? A meta-analysis. Psychol Bull. 2004;130(2):261–88.
Senn TE, Espy KA, Kaufmann PM. Using path analysis to understand executive function organization in preschool children. Dev Neuropsychol. 2004;26:445–64.
St Clair-Thompson HL, Gathercole SE. Executive functions and achievements in school: shifting, updating, inhibition, and working memory. Q J Exp Psychol. 2006;59:745–59.
Schneider W, Shiffrin RM. Controlled and Automatic Human Information Processing: I. Detection, Search, and attention. Psychol Rev. 1977;84(1).
Schuchardt K, Mähler C, Hasselhorn M. Working memory deficits in children with specific learning disorders. J Learn Disabil. 2008;41(6):514–23.
Simon JR. Reactions toward the source of stimulation. J Exp Psychol. 1969;81:174–6.
Sirin SR. Socioeconomic status and academic achievement: a meta-analytic review of research. Rev Educ Res. 2005;75(3):417–53.
Soubelet A. Age-cognition relations and the personality trait of conscientiousness. J Res Pers. 2011;45(6):529–34.
Stahl C, Voss A, Schmitz F, Nuszbaum M, Tuscher O, Lieb K, et al. Behavioral components of impulsivity. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2014;143:850–86.
Stroop JR. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J Exp Psychol. 1935;18:643–62.
Swanson HL. Reading comprehension and working memory in skilled readers: is the phonological loop more important than the executive system? J Exp Child Psychol. 1999;72:1–31.
Tiego J, Testa R, Bellgrove MA, Pantelis C, Whittle S. A hierarchical model of inhibitory control. Front Psychol. 2018;9:1339.
Titz C, Karbach J. Working memory and executive functions: effects of training on academic achievement. Psychol Res. 2014;78:852–68.
Verbruggen F, Logan GD. Response inhibition in the stop-signal paradigm. Trends Cogn Sci. 2008;12:418–24.
Veroude K, Jolles J, Knezevic M, Vos CMP, Croiset G, Krabbendam L. Anterior cingulate activation during cognitive control relates to academic performance in medical students. Trends Neurosci Educ. 2013;2:100–6.
Visu-Petra L, Cheie L, Benga O, Miclea M. Cognitive control goes to school: the impact of executive functions on academic performance. Proc Soc Behav Sci. 2011;11:240–4.
Willoughby MT, Kupersmidt JB, Voegler-Lee ME. Is preschool executive function causally related to academic achievement? Child Neuropsychol. 2012;18(1):79–91.
Yow WQ, Li X. Balanced bilingualism and early age of second language acquisition as the underlying mechanisms of a bilingual executive control advantage: why variations in bilingual experiences matter. Front Psychol. 2015;6:164.
Funding
Open access funding provided by Södertörn University. The research project described in the paper was partially funded by the School of Teacher Education, Södertörn University, Stockholm.
Open access funding provided by Södertörn University.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
The sole author, Martin Dvorak, has conducted the collection of data, its analysis and all the other procedures described in this paper.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Ethics approval and consent to participate
The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki (as revised in 2000). All the subjects participated in the research voluntarily and signed the informed consent. The need for ethics approval has been deemed unnecessary according to Swedish national regulations (Lag (2003:460) om etikprövning av forskning som avser människor), as no information collected or processed during the analysis represents data stipulated in the law as those requiring ethics approval.
Consent for publication
Not applicable.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher’s note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Dvorak, M. Inhibitory control and academic achievement – a study of the relationship between Stroop Effect and university students’ academic performance. BMC Psychol 12, 498 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-024-01984-3
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-024-01984-3